Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-30T20:35:54.079Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Above and beyond the concrete: The diverse representational substrates of the predictive brain

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 July 2019

Michael Gilead
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Zlotowski Center for Neuroscience, Ben-Gurion University, Beersheba84105, [email protected]://www.gileadlab.net/
Yaacov Trope
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, [email protected]://www.psych.nyu.edu/tropelab/
Nira Liberman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Tel-Aviv University, Tel-Aviv69978, Israel. [email protected]://en-social-sciences.tau.ac.il/profile/niralib

Abstract

In recent years, scientists have increasingly taken to investigate the predictive nature of cognition. We argue that prediction relies on abstraction, and thus theories of predictive cognition need an explicit theory of abstract representation. We propose such a theory of the abstract representational capacities that allow humans to transcend the “here-and-now.” Consistent with the predictive cognition literature, we suggest that the representational substrates of the mind are built as a hierarchy, ranging from the concrete to the abstract; however, we argue that there are qualitative differences between elements along this hierarchy, generating meaningful, often unacknowledged, diversity. Echoing views from philosophy, we suggest that the representational hierarchy can be parsed into: modality-specific representations, instantiated on perceptual similarity; multimodal representations, instantiated primarily on the discovery of spatiotemporal contiguity; and categorical representations, instantiated primarily on social interaction. These elements serve as the building blocks of complex structures discussed in cognitive psychology (e.g., episodes, scripts) and are the inputs for mental representations that behave like functions, typically discussed in linguistics (i.e., predicators). We support our argument for representational diversity by explaining how the elements in our ontology are all required to account for humans’ predictive cognition (e.g., in subserving logic-based prediction; in optimizing the trade-off between accurate and detailed predictions) and by examining how the neuroscientific evidence coheres with our account. In doing so, we provide a testable model of the neural bases of conceptual cognition and highlight several important implications to research on self-projection, reinforcement learning, and predictive-processing models of psychopathology.

Type
Target Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2019. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, R., Stephan, K., Brown, H., Frith, C. & Friston, K. (2013) The computational anatomy of psychosis. Frontiers in Psychiatry 4(47). Available at: http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00047.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T. & Schacter, D. L. (2007) Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia 45(7):1363–77. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amemiya, S. & Redish, A. D. (2016) Manipulating decisiveness in decision making: Effects of clonidine on hippocampal search strategies. Journal of Neuroscience 36(3):814–27. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.2595-15.2016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Apperly, I. A. (2008) Beyond simulation-theory and theory-theory: Why social cognitive neuroscience should use its own concepts to study “theory of mind.” Cognition 107(1):266–83. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asch, S. E. (1951) Effects of group pressure upon the modification and distortion of judgment. In: Groups, leadership and men, ed. Guetzkow, H., pp. 177–90. Carnegie Press.Google Scholar
Asimov, I. (1950) I, Robot. Gnome Press, New York.Google Scholar
Austin, J. L. (1975) How to do things with words. Oxford university press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Babkoff, H., Sing, H. C., Thorne, D. R., Genser, S. G. & Hegge, F. W. (1989) Perceptual-distortions and hallucinations reported during the course of sleep-deprivation. Perceptual and Motor Skills 68(3):787–98. Available at: http://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1989.68.3.787.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Badre, D. & D'Esposito, M. (2007) Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for a hierarchical organization of the prefrontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19(12):2082–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Badre, D. & D'Esposito, M. (2009) Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10(9):659–69. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Badre, D., Kayser, A. S. & D'Esposito, M. (2010) Frontal cortex and the discovery of abstract action rules. Neuron 66(2):315–26. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.025.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R., Spelke, E. S. & Wasserman, S. (1985) Object permanence in five-month-old infants. Cognition 20(3):191208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Banaji, M. R., Hardin, C. & Rothman, A. J. (1993) Implicit stereotyping in person judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65(2):272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bar, M., Kassam, K. S., Ghuman, A. S., Boshyan, J., Schmidt, A. M., Dale, A. M., Hämäläinen, M. S., Marinkovic, K., Schacter, D. L., Rosen, B. R. & Halgren, E. (2006) Top-down facilitation of visual recognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103(2):449–54. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0507062103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bar, M. (Ed.). (2011) Predictions in the brain: Using our past to generate a future. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, J. A. (2006) What have we been priming all these years? On the development, mechanisms, and ecology of nonconscious social behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology 36(2): 147–68. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M. & Frith, U. (1985) Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition 21(1):3746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, L. F., Quigley, K. S. & Hamilton, P. (2016) An active inference theory of allostasis and interoception in depression. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 371(1708): 20160011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. W. (1983) Ad hoc categories. Memory & Cognition 11(3):211–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. W. (1999) Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22(4):577660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. W. (2008) Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology 59:617–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. W. (2009) Simulation, situated conceptualization, and prediction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 364(1521):1281–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bassok, M., Dunbar, K. N. & Holyoak, K. J. (2012) Introduction to the special section on the neural substrate of analogical reasoning and metaphor comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 38(2):261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bechara, A., Damasio, H. & Damasio, A. R. (2003) Role of the amygdala in decision-making. In: Amygdala in brain function: Basic and clinical approaches, vol. 985, ed. Shinnick-Gallagher, P., Pitkanen, A., Shekhar, A. & Cahill, L., pp. 356-69. Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Berwick, R. C., Friederici, A. D., Chomsky, N. & Bolhuis, J. J. (2013) Evolution, brain, and the nature of language. Trends in cognitive sciences 17(2):8998.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bhatia, S. & Walasek, L. (2016) Event construal and temporal distance in natural language. Cognition 152:18. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.03.011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Binder, J. R. & Desai, R. H. (2011) The neurobiology of semantic memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15(11):527–36. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.10.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W. & Conant, L. L. (2009) Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex 19(12):2767–96. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp055.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, P. & Markson, L. (1998) Capacities underlying word learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2(2):6773. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(98)01121-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. (2009) Predicative minds: The social ontogeny of propositional thinking. Cambrdige, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bond, R. & Smith, P. B. (1996) Culture and conformity: A meta-analysis of studies using Asch's (1952b, 1956) line judgment task. Psychological Bulletin 119(1):111–37. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.119.1.111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S. & Cohen, J. D. (2001) Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review 108(3):624–52. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295x.108.3.624.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bower, G. H., Clark, M. C., Lesgold, A. M. & Winzenz, D. (1969) Hierarchical retrieval schemes in recall of categorized word lists. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 8(3):323–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowers, J. S. (2009) On the biological plausibility of grandmother cells: Implications for neural network theories in psychology and neuroscience. Psychological Review 116(1):220-51. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1037/a0014462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brentano, F. (1874) Psychology from an empirical standpoint. Routledge. 1995 (Original work published in 1874)Google Scholar
Buckner, R. L. & Carroll, D. C. (2007) Self-projection and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11(2):4957.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bunge, S. A., Wendelken, C., Badre, D. & Wagner, A. D. (2004) Analogical reasoning and prefrontal cortex: Evidence for separable retrieval and integration mechanisms. Cerebral Cortex 15(3):239–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camerer, C. F., Ho, T. H. & Chong, J. K. (2002) Sophisticated experience-weighted attraction learning and strategic teaching in repeated games. Journal of Economic Theory 104(1):137–88. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1006/jeth.2002.2927.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, J (1968) The masks of God: Creative mythology. New York: Viking Press.Google Scholar
Campbell, J. (1991/1959) The masks of God: Primitive mythology. Arkana.Google Scholar
Carey, S. (2009) The origin of concepts. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carver, C. S. & Scheier, M. F. (2001) On the self-regulation of behavior. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Carruthers, P. (2002) The cognitive functions of language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25(06):657–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. & Feldman, M. W. (1981) Cultural transmission and evolution. A quantitative approach. Princeton University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Chater, N. & Vitanyi, P. (2003) Simplicity: A unifying principle in cognitive science? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7(1):1922. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(02)00005-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, A. M. & Quillian, M. R. (1969) Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior 8(2):240–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A. (2013) Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36(3):181204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, J. E., Watson, S. & Friston, K. J. (2018) What is mood? A computational perspective. Psychological Medicine 48(14):2277–84. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291718000430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clayton, N. S., Dally, J. M. & Emery, N. J. (2007) Social cognition by food-caching corvids. The western scrub-jay as a natural psychologist. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 362(1480):507–22. [aMG]. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2006.1992.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, G. (2000) Hierarchical models in cognition: Do they have psychological reality? European Journal of cognitive psychology 12(1):136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conway, M. A. & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000) The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review 107(2):261–88. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295x.107.2.261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corballis, M. C. (2014) The recursive mind: The origins of human language, thought, and civilization. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Danker, J. F., Tompary, A. & Davachi, L. (2016) Trial-by-trial hippocampal encoding activation predicts the fidelity of cortical reinstatement during subsequent retrieval. Cerebral Cortex 27(7):3515–24.Google Scholar
Davachi, L. (2006) Item, context and relational episodic encoding in humans. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 16(6):693700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daw, N. D. & Dayan, P. (2014) The algorithmic anatomy of model-based evaluation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 369(1655):12. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0478.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daw, N. D., Gershman, S. J., Seymour, B., Dayan, P. & Dolan, R. J. (2011) Model-based influences on humans' choices and striatal prediction errors. Neuron 69(6):1204–15. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.027.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, T. (1997) The symbolic species. Norton.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., Piazza, M., Pinel, P. & Cohen, L. (2003) Three parietal circuits for number processing. Cognitive Neuropsychology 20(3-6):487506. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1080/02643290244000239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeLoache, J. S. (2004) Becoming symbol-minded. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8(2):66270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Luca, F., Benuzzi, F., Bertossi, E., Braghittoni, D., di Pellegrino, G. & Ciaramelli, E. (2018) Episodic future thinking and future-based decision-making in a case of retrograde amnesia. Neuropsychologia 110:92103. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.08.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D. & Haugeland, J. (1987) Intentionality. In: The Oxford companion to the mind, ed. Gregory, R. L.. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Desimone, R., Albright, T. D., Gross, C. G. & Bruce, C. (1984) Stimulus-selective properties of inferior temporal neurons in the macaque. The Journal of Neuroscience 4(8):2051–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickinson, A. (1985) Actions and habits: The development of behavioural autonomy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, Biological Sciences 308(1135):6778.Google Scholar
Doll, B. B., Duncan, K. D., Simon, D. A., Shohamy, D. & Daw, N. D. (2015) Model-based choices involve prospective neural activity. Nature Neuroscience 18(5):767–72. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3981.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doumas, L. A. A., Hummel, J. E. & Sandhofer, C. M. (2008) A theory of the discovery and predication of relational concepts. Psychological Review 115(1):143. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.115.1.1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DuPre, E., Luh, W.-M. & Spreng, R. N. (2016) Multi-echo fMRI replication sample of autobiographical memory, prospection and theory of mind reasoning tasks. Scientific Data 3:160116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunbar, R. & Dunbar, R. I. M. (1998) Grooming, gossip, and the evolution of language. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Echterhoff, G., Higgins, E. T. & Levine, J. M. (2009) Shared reality experiencing commonality with others’ inner states about the world. Perspectives on Psychological Science 4(5):496521. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01161.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eichenbaum, H., Yonelinas, A. P. & Ranganath, C. (2007) The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annual Review of Neuroscience 30:123–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eimontaite, I., Goel, V., Raymont, V., Krueger, F., Schindler, I. & Grafman, J. (2018) Differential roles of polar orbital prefrontal cortex and parietal lobes in logical reasoning with neutral and negative emotional content. Neuropsychologia 119:320–29. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.05.014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epstein, R. & Kanwisher, N. (1998) A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature 392(6676):598601. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1038/33402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J. S. T. (2003) In two minds: Dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7(10):454–59. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.08.012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fairhall, S. L. & Caramazza, A. (2013) Brain regions that represent amodal conceptual knowledge. The Journal of Neuroscience 33(25):10552–58. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0051-13.2013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farah, M. J. & McClelland, J. L. (1991) A computational model of semantic memory impairment – Modality specificity and emergent category specificity. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General 120(4):339–57. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.120.4.339CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faja, S., Murias, M., Beauchaine, T. P. & Dawson, G. (2013) Reward-based decision making and electrodermal responding by young children with autism spectrum disorders during a gambling task. Autism Research 6(6):494505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fauconnier, G. & Turner, M. (2008) The way we think: Conceptual blending and the mind's hidden complexities. Basic Books.
Festinger, L. (1962) Cognitive dissonance. Scientific American.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Field, D. J. (1994) What is the goal of sensory coding. Neural Computation 6(4):559601. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1162/neco.1994.6.4.559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1975) The language of thought, vol. 5. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1998) Concepts. Where cognitive science went wrong: Clarendon Press/Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (2014) Minds without meanings: An essay on the content of concepts. Mit Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frege, G. (1892/1952a) On concept and object. In: Translations from the philosophical writings of Gottlob Frege, ed. Geach, P. T. & Black, M., pp. 4255. Basil Blackwell. (Original work published in 1892)Google Scholar
Friston, K. (2005) A theory of cortical responses. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 360(1456):815–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, K. (2013) Life as we know it. Journal of the Royal Society, Interface 10(86):20130475.Google Scholar
Friston, K. J. (2005) A theory of cortical responses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 360(1456):815–36. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2005.1622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, K. J. (2010) The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11(2):127–38. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2787.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friston, K. J., Daunizeau, J. & Kiebel, S. J. (2009) Reinforcement learning or active inference? PLOS ONE 4(7):13. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0006421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friston, K. J., Stephan, K. E., Montague, R. & Dolan, R. J. (2014) Computational psychiatry: The brain as a phantastic organ. Lancet Psychiatry 1(2):148–58. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/s2215-0366(14)70275-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, C. D. & Frith, U. (1999) Interacting minds – A biological basis. Science 286(5445):1692–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fujita, K., Trope, Y., Liberman, N. & Levin-Sagi, M. (2006) Construal levels and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 90(3):351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallese, V. & Goldman, A. (1998) Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2(12):493501. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(98)01262-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D. (1983) Structure-mapping: A theoretical framework for analogy. Cognitive Science 7(2): 155–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D. & Markman, A. B. (1997) Structure mapping in analogy and similarity. American Psychologist 52(1):45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D. & Medina, J. (1998) Similarity and the development of rules. Cognition 65(2-3):263–97. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(98)00002-x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gershman, S. J., Blei, D. M. & Niv, Y. (2010) Context, learning, and extinction. Psychological review, 117(1):197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, D. T. & Wilson, T. D. (2007) Prospection: Experiencing the future. Science 317(5843): 1351–54. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1144161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilead, M., Boccagno, C., Silverman, M., Hassin, R. R., Weber, J. & Ochsner, K. N. (2016) Self-regulation via neural simulation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 113(36):10037–42. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1600159113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilead, M., Liberman, N. & Maril, A. (2012) Construing counterfactual worlds: The role of abstraction. European Journal of Social Psychology 42(3):391–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilead, M., Liberman, N. & Maril, A. (2013) The language of future-thought: An fMRI study of embodiment and tense processing. Neuroimage 65:267–79. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.073.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goff, L. M. & Roediger, H. L. (1998) Imagination inflation for action events: Repeated imaginings lead to illusory recollections. Memory and Cognition 26(1):2033.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldsmith, M., Koriat, A. & Weinberg-Eliezer, A. (2002) Strategic regulation of grain size in memory reporting. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General 131(1):7395. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.131.173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, N. (1972) Seven strictures on similarity. In: Goodman, N., Problems and projects, pp. 437–47. Indianapolis: Bobbs-Merrill.Google Scholar
Goodwin, G. P. & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2013) The acquisition of Boolean concepts. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17(3):128–33. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.01.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottlieb, L. J., Wong, J., de Chastelaine, M. & Rugg, M. D. (2012) Neural correlates of the encoding of multimodal contextual features. Learning & Memory 19(12):605–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, R. (1986) Folk psychology as simulation. Mind and Language 1(2):158–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gregory, R. L. (1980) Perceptions as hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 290(1038):181–97. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1980.0090.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffiths, T. L., Chater, N., Kemp, C., Perfors, A. & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2010) Probabilistic models of cognition: Exploring representations and inductive biases. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14(8):357–64. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halamish, V. & Liberman, N. (2017) How much information to sample before making a decision? It's a matter of psychological distance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 71:111–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hahn, U. & Chater, N. (1998) Understanding similarity: A joint project for psychology, case-based reasoning, and law. Artificial Intelligence Review 12(5):393427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H. & Phillips, S. (2010) Relational knowledge: The foundation of higher cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14(11):497505. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.08.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haslam, N. & Loughnan, S. (2014) Dehumanization and infrahumanization. In: Annual review of psychology, vol. 65, ed. Fiske, S. T., pp. 399423. Annual Reviews.Google Scholar
Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D. & Maguire, E. A. (2007) Using imagination to understand the neural basis of episodic memory. Journal of Neuroscience 27(52):14365-374. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.4549-07.2007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauk, O., Johnsrude, I. & Pulvermüller, F. (2004) Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron 41(2):301307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heider, F. (1958) The psychology of interpersonal relations. Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helmholtz, H. von (1856/1961) Treatise of physiological optics: Concerning the perceptions in general. In: Classics in psychology, ed. Shipley, T., pp. 79127. Philosophy Library. (Original work published in 1856)Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2015) The secret of our success: How culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinton, G. E., Dayan, P., Frey, B. J. & Neal, R. M. (1995) The wake-sleep algorithm for unsupervised neural networks. Science 268(5214):1158–61. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1126/science.7761831.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinton, G. E., Osindero, S. & Teh, Y. W. (2006) A fast learning algorithm for deep belief nets. Neural Computation 18(7):1527–54. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1162/neco.2006.18.7.1527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hintzman, D. L. (1986) Schema abstraction in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychological Review 93(4):411–28. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295x.93.4.411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Høffding, H. (1892) Outlines of psychology. Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobeika, L., Diard-Detoeuf, C., Garcin, B., Levy, R. & Volle, E. (2016) General and specialized brain correlates for analogical reasoning: A meta-analysis of functional imaging studies. Human Brain Mapping 37(5):1953–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hohwy, J. (2013) The predictive mind. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hommel, B., Musseler, J., Aschersleben, G. & Prinz, W. (2001) The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24(5):849–78. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525×01000103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopfield, J. J. (1982) Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America-Biological Sciences 79(8):2554–58. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.79.8.2554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holland, P. C. (2008) Cognitive versus stimulus-response theories of learning. Learning & Behavior 36(3):227–41. doi: 10.3758/lb.36.3.227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hubel, D. H. & Wiesel, T. N. (1962) Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex. The Journal of Physiology 160:106–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hummel, J. E. & Holyoak, K. J. (1997) Distributed representations of structure: A theory of analogical access and mapping. Psychological Review 104(3):427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hubel, D. H. & Wiesel, T. N. (1968) Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex. Journal of Physiology-London 195(1):215–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janis, I. L. (1972) Victims of groupthink: A psychological study of foreign-policy decisions and fiascoes. Oxford, England: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Johnson, A. & Redish, A. D. (2007) Neural ensembles in CA3 transiently encode paths forward of the animal at a decision point. Journal of Neuroscience 27(45):12176–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. K. & Raye, C. L. (1981) Reality monitoring. Psychological Review 88(1):67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, E. E. & Davis, K. E. (1965) From acts to dispositions: The attribution process in person perception. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 2(4):219–66.Google Scholar
Kahana, M. J. (1996) Associative retrieval processes in free recall. Memory and Cognition 24(1):103109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kandel, E. R. (2001) The molecular biology of memory storage: a dialogue between genes and synapses. Science, 294(5544):1030–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanwisher, N. (2010) Functional specificity in the human brain: A window into the functional architecture of the mind. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107(25):11163–70. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1005062107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelley, H. H. (1973) Processes of causal attribution. American Psychologist 28(2):107–28. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1037/h0034225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kernberg, O. (1975/1985) Borderline conditions and pathological narcissism. Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson.Google Scholar
Kiefer, M. & Pulvermüller, F. (2012) Conceptual representations in mind and brain: Theoretical developments, current evidence and future directions. Cortex 48(7):805–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, H. (2016) Default network activation during episodic and semantic memory retrieval: A selective meta-analytic comparison. Neuropsychologia 80:3546. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.11.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirchhoff, M., Parr, T., Palacios, E., Friston, K. & Kiverstein, J. (2018) The Markov blankets of life: Autonomy, active inference and the free energy principle. Journal of The Royal Society Interface 15(138):20170792.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kratzer, A. & Heim, I. (1998) Semantics in generative grammar, vol. 1185. Blackwell Oxford.Google Scholar
Kruger, T., Fiedler, K., Koch, A. S. & Alves, H. (2014) Response category width as a psychophysical manifestation of construal level and distance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 40(4):501–12. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1177/0146167213517009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kruschke, J. K. (1992) ALCOVE: An exemplar-based connectionist model of category learning. Psychological Review 99(1):22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuczaj, S. A. & Daly, M. J. (1979) The development of hypothetical reference in the speech of young children. Journal of Child Language 6(03):563–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuffler, S. W. (1953) Discharge patterns and functional organization of mammalian retina. Journal of Neurophysiology 16(1):3768.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kwan, D., Craver, C. F., Green, L., Myerson, J., Boyer, P. & Rosenbaum, R. S. (2012) Future decision-making without episodic mental time travel. Hippocampus 22(6):1215–19. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.20981.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lah, S. & Miller, L. (2008) Effects of temporal lobe lesions on retrograde memory: A critical review. Neuropsychology Review 18(1):24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lake, B. M., Ullman, T. D., Tenenbaum, J. B. & Gershman, S. J. (2017) Building machines that learn and think like people. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 40:25. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525×16001837.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LeCun, Y., Bengio, Y. & Hinton, G. (2015) Deep learning. Nature 521(7553):436–44. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1038/nature14539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leslie, A. M. (1987) Pretense and representation: The origins of theory of mind. Psychological Review 94(4):412–26. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.94.4.412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, N. & Trope, Y. (1998) The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal construal theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 75(1):5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, N., Sagristano, M. D. & Trope, Y. (2002) The effect of temporal distance on level of mental construal. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 38(6):523–34. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-1031(02)00535-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, N. & Trope, Y. (2008) The psychology of transcending the here and now. Science 322(5905):12011205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, N. & Trope, Y. (2014) Traversing psychological distance. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 18(7):364–69. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.03.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macleod, C. M. (1991) Half a century of research on the stroop effect: An Integrative review. Psychological Bulletin 109(2):163203. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.109.2.163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Madore, K. P., Szpunar, K. K., Addis, D. R. & Schacter, D. L. (2016) Episodic specificity induction impacts activity in a core brain network during construction of imagined future experiences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 113(38):10696–701. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1612278113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahler, W. (1933) Ersatzhandlungen verschiedenen realitätsgrades. Psychologische Forschung 18(1):2789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Majdandzic, J., Amashaufer, S., Hummer, A., Windischberger, C. & Lamm, C. (2016) The selfless mind: How prefrontal involvement in mentalizing with similar and dissimilar others shapes empathy and prosocial behavior. Cognition 157:2438. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.08.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malle, B. F. & Holbrook, J. (2012) Is there a hierarchy of social inferences? The likelihood and speed of inferring intentionality, mind, and personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 102(4):661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcus, G. (2009) Kluge: The haphazard evolution of the human mind. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Marcus, G. F. (2001) The algebraic mind: Integrating connectionism and cognitive science. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcus, G. F., Vijayan, S., Rao, S. B. & Vishton, P. M. (1999) Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. Science 283(5398):7780.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markman, A. B. & Stilwell, C. H. (2001) Role-governed categories. Journal of Experimental and Theoretical Artificial Intelligence 13(4):329–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marr, D. (1982) Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Martinelli, P., Sperduti, M. & Piolino, P. (2013) Neural substrates of the self-memory system: New insights from a meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping 34(7):1515–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martinho, A. & Kacelnik, A. (2016) Ducklings imprint on the relational concept of “same or different.” Science 353(6296):286–88. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaf4247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L. & O'Reilly, R. C. (1995) Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychological Review 102(3):419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, J. L., Rumelhart, D. E. & Research Group, PDP. (1986) Parallel distributed processing. MIT Press.Google Scholar
McClelland, J. L., Botvinick, M. M., Noelle, D. C., Plaut, D. C., Rogers, T. T., Seidenberg, M. S. & Smith, L. B. (2010) Letting structure emerge: Connectionist and dynamical systems approaches to cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14(8):348–56. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.06.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Medin, D. L. & Schaffer, M. M. (1978) Context theory of classification learning. Psychological review 85(3):207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mead, G. H. (1934) Mind, self, and society: From the standpoint of a social behaviorist: Works of George Herbert Mead, vol. 1. 111. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Medin, D. L., Goldstone, R. L. & Gentner, D. (1993) Respects for similarity. Psychological Review 100(2):254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milligan, K., Astington, J. W. & Dack, L. A. (2007) Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development 78(2):622–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montefinese, M. (2019) Semantic representation of abstract and concrete words: A minireview of neural evidence. Journal of neurophysiology 121(5):1585–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mormann, F., Kornblith, S., Quiroga, R. Q., Kraskov, A., Cerf, M., Fried, I. & Koch, C. (2008) Latency and selectivity of single neurons indicate hierarchical processing in the human medial temporal lobe. Journal of Neuroscience 28(36):8865–72. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1640-08.2008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moulton, S. T. & Kosslyn, S. M. (2009) Imagining predictions: Mental imagery as mental emulation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 364(1521):1273–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, G. L. & Medin, D. L. (1985) The role of theories in conceptual coherence. Psychological Review 92(3):289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newell, A. & Simon, H. A. (1972) Human problem solving. Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Ng, A. Y. & Jordan, M. I. (2002) On discriminative vs. generative classifiers: A comparison of logistic regression and naive Bayes. In: Advances in neural information processing systems, ed. Dietterich, T. G., Becker, S. & Ghahramani, Z., pp. 841–48.Google Scholar
Nichols, S. & Stich, S. (2000) A cognitive theory of pretense. Cognition 74(2):115–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Niv, Y., Joel, D. & Dayan, P. (2006) A normative perspective on motivation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10(8):375–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norenzayan, A. (2013) Big gods: How religion transformed cooperation and conflict. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
O'Reilly, R. C. & McClelland, J. L. (1994) Hippocampal conjunctive encoding, storage, and recall: Avoiding a trade-off. Hippocampus 4(6):661–82. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.450040605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Packer, D. J. & Cunningham, W. A. (2009) Neural correlates of reflection on goal states: The role of regulatory focus and temporal distance. Social Neuroscience 4(5):412–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearce, J. M. (1987) A model for stimulus-generalization in Pavlovian conditioning. Psychological Review 94(1):6173. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295x.94.1.61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, J., Naselaris, T., Holmes, E. A. & Kosslyn, S. M. (2015) Mental imagery: Functional mechanisms and clinical applications. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 19(10):590602. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.08.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peirce, C. S. (1931) Collected papers of Charles Sanders Peirce. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Penn, D. C., Holyoak, K. J. & Povinelli, D. J. (2008) Darwin's mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31(2):109–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peters, J. & Büchel, C. (2010) Episodic future thinking reduces reward delay discounting through an enhancement of prefrontal-mediotemporal interactions. Neuron 66(1):138–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pezzulo, G., Kemere, C. & van der Meer, M. A. A. (2017) Internally generated hippocampal sequences as a vantage point to probe future-oriented cognition. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1396(1):144–65. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.13329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J. (1954) The construction of reality in the child. Basic Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. (2007) The stuff of thought: Language as a window into human nature. Penguin.Google Scholar
Pick-Alony, R., Liberman, N. & Trope, Y. (2014) High level of construal and psychological distance reduce melioration. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 27(4):291300.Google Scholar
Pothos, E. M. & Chater, N. (2002) A simplicity principle in unsupervised human categorization. Cognitive Science 26(3):303–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Powers, A. R., Mathys, C. & Corlett, P. R. (2017) Pavlovian conditioning-induced hallucinations result from overweighting of perceptual priors. Science 357(6351):596600. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aan3458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prinz, J. (2012a) Regaining composure: A defense of prototype compositionality. The Oxford Handbook of Compositionality 437–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pylkkänen, L., Brennan, J. & Bemis, D. K. (2011) Grounding the cognitive neuroscience of semantics in linguistic theory. Language and Cognitive Processes 26(9):1317–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1978) When is attribution of beliefs justified? [P&W]. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1(4):592–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quiroga, R. Q., Kraskov, A., Koch, C. & Fried, I. (2009) Explicit encoding of multimodal percepts by single neurons in the human brain. Current Biology 19(15):1308–13. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.06.060.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quiroga, R. Q., Reddy, L., Kreiman, G., Koch, C. & Fried, I. (2005) Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain. Nature 435(7045):11021107. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1038/nature03687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A. & Shulman, G. L. (2001) A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 98(2):676–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rao, R. P. N. & Ballard, D. H. (1999) Predictive coding in the visual cortex: A functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nature Neuroscience 2(1):7987. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1038/4580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raposo, A., Mendes, M. & Marques, J. F. (2012) The hierarchical organization of semantic memory: Executive function in the processing of superordinate concepts. Neuroimage 59(2):1870–78. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.072.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Redish, A. D. (2013) The mind within the brain: How we make decisions and how those decisions go wrong. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Redish, A. D. (2016) Vicarious trial and error. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 17(3):147–59. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2015.30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redish, A. D., Jensen, S., Johnson, A. & Kurth-Nelson, Z. (2007) Reconciling reinforcement learning models with behavioral extinction and renewal: Implications for addiction, relapse, and problem gambling. Psychological Review 114(3):784805. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.114.3.784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reeves, L. M. & Weisberg, R. W. (1994) The role of content and abstract information in analogical transfer. Psychological Bulletin 115(3):381400. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.115.3.381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodriguez, M. L., Mischel, W. & Shoda, Y. (1989) Cognitive person variables in the delay of gratification of older children at risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 57(2):358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roelofs, A. (1992) A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking. Cognition 42(1–3): 107–42. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(92)90041-f.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosch, E. & Mervis, C. B. (1975) Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology 7(4):573605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M. & Boyesbraem, P. (1976) Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology 8(3):382439. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(76)90013-x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruffman, T., Slade, L. & Crowe, E. (2002) The relation between children's and mothers' mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. Child Development 73(3):734–51. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00435.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sargolini, F., Fyhn, M., Hafting, T., McNaughton, B. L., Witter, M. P., Moser, M. B. & Moser, E. I. (2006) Conjunctive representation of position, direction, and velocity in entorhinal cortex. Science: 312(5774):758–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Rumbaugh, D. M. & Boysen, S. (1978) Symbolic communication between two chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Science 201(4356):641–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R. & Buckner, R. L. (2007) Remembering the past to imagine the future: The prospective brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8(9):657–61. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schank, R. C. & Abelson, R. P. (1975) Scripts, plans, and knowledge. Yale University.Google Scholar
Schultz, W., Dayan, P. & Montague, P. R. (1997) A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275(5306):1593–99. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1126/science.275.5306.1593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartenbeck, P., FitzGerald, T., Dolan, R. J. & Friston, K. J. (2013) Exploration, novelty, surprise, and free energy minimization. Frontiers in Psychology 4:5. Available at: http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00710.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1979) What is an intentional state? Mind 88(349):7492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1983) Intentionality: An essay in the philosophy of mind. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. R. (2010) Making the social world: The structure of human civilization. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Semin, G. R. & Fiedler, K. (1991) The linguistic category model, its bases, applications and range. European Review of Social Psychology 2(1):130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shapira, O., Liberman, N., Trope, N. & Rim, S. (2012) Levels of mental construal. Sage Handbook of Social Cognition, 229–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepard, R. N. (1987) Toward a universal law of generalization for psychological science. Science 237(4820):1317–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1938) The behavior of organisms: An experimental analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Smith, E. R. (1998) Mental representation and memory. In: Gilbert, D. T., Fiske, S. T., Lindzey, G. (eds). The handbook of social psychology, vol. 2, pp. 391445. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Smith, E. R. & Semin, G. R. (2007) Situated social cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science 16(3):132–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snefjella, B. & Kuperman, V. (2015) Concreteness and psychological distance in natural language use. Psychological Science 26(9):1449–60. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615591771.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spreng, R. N., Mar, R. A. & Kim, A. S. (2009) The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: A quantitative meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21(3):489510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spreng, R. N. & Grady, C. L. (2010) Patterns of brain activity supporting autobiographical memory, prospection, and theory of mind, and their relationship to the default mode network. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22(6):1112–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanovich, K. E. & West, R. F. (2000) Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23(5):645–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stark, C., Stark, S. & Gordon, B. (2005) New semantic learning and generalization in a patient with amnesia. Neuropsychology 19(2):139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suddendorf, T. (2013) The gap: The science of what separates us from other animals. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T. & Corballis, M. C. (2007) The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30(3):299313. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525×07001975.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamir, D. I. & Mitchell, J. P. (2010) Neural correlates of anchoring-and-adjustment during mentalizing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107(24):10827–832. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1003242107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamir, D. I. & Mitchell, J. P. (2011) The default network distinguishes construals of proximal versus distal events. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 23(10):2945–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamir, D. I., Bricker, A. B., Dodell-Feder, D. & Mitchell, J. P. (2015) Reading fiction and reading minds: The role of simulation in the default network. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 11(2):215–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tenenbaum, J. B., Kemp, C., Griffiths, T. L. & Goodman, N. D. (2011) How to grow a mind: Statistics, structure, and abstraction. Science 331(6022):1279–85. doi: 10.1126/science.1192788.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, C. K., Bonakdarpour, B., Fix, S. C., Blumenfeld, H. K., Parrish, T. B., Gitelman, D. R. & Mesulam, M. M. (2007) Neural correlates of verb argument structure processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19(11):1753–67. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.11.1753.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tolman, E. C. (1948) Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review 55(4):189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trope, Y. (1986) Identification and inferential processes in dispositional attribution. Psychological Review 93(3):239–57. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295x.93.3.239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y. & Liberman, N. (2000) Temporal construal and time-dependent changes in preference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 79(6):876.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trope, Y. & Liberman, N. (2010) Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review 117(2):440–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tulving, E. (1984) Precis of Tulving elements of episodic memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7(2):223–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E. (1985) How many memory systems are there? American Psychologist 40(4):385–98. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.40.4.385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E. (1986) What kind of a hypothesis is the distinction between episodic semantic memory? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 12(2):307–11. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.12.2.307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E., Hayman, C. & Macdonald, C. A. (1991) Long-lasting perceptual priming and semantic learning in amnesia: A case experiment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 17(4):595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, J. C. (1991) Social influence. Thomson Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
Tversky, A. (1977) Features of similarity. Psychological Review 84(4):327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vallacher, R. R. & Wegner, D. M. (1987) What do people think they're doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psychological Review 94(1):3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Meer, M. A., Johnson, A., Schmitzer-Torbert, N. C. & Redish, A. D. (2010) Triple dissociation of information processing in dorsal striatum, ventral striatum, and hippocampus on a learned spatial decision task. Neuron 67(1):2532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Velleman, J. D. (1992) The guise of the good. Nous 26(1):326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, J., Conder, J. A., Blitzer, D. N. & Shinkareva, S. V. (2010) Neural representation of abstract and concrete concepts: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping 31(10):1459–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weingarten, E., Chen, Q. J., McAdams, M., Yi, J., Hepler, J. & Albarracin, D. (2016) From primed concepts to action: A meta-analysis of the behavioral effects of incidentally presented words. Psychological Bulletin 142(5):472–97. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000030.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitaker, K. J., Vendetti, M. S., Wendelken, C. & Bunge, S. A. (2018) Neuroscientific insights into the development of analogical reasoning. Developmental Science 21(2): e12531.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, A., Reddigari, S. & Pylkkänen, L. (2017) Early sensitivity of left perisylvian cortex to relationality in nouns and verbs. Neuropsychologia 100:131–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, J. N. & Grafman, J. (2003) Human prefrontal cortex: processing and representational perspectives. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4(2):139–47. doi: 10.1038/nrn1033.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yudkin, D. A., Pick, R., Hur, E. Y., Liberman, N. & Trope, Y. (2019) Psychological distance promotes exploration in search of a global maximum. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 45(6):893906.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ylinen, A., Bragin, A., Nadasdy, Z., Jando, G., Szabo, I., Sik, A. & Buzsaki, G. (1995) Sharp wave associated high-frequency oscillation (200-hz) in the intact hippocampus: Network and intracellular mechanisms. Journal of Neuroscience 15(1):3046.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zarate, M. A. & Smith, E. R. (1990) Person categorization and stereotyping. Social Cognition 8(2):161–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmerer, V. C., Varley, R. A., Deamer, F. & Hinzen, W. (2019) Factive and counterfactive interpretation of embedded clauses in aphasia and its relationship with lexical, syntactic and general cognitive capacities. Journal of Neurolinguistics 49:2944. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2018.08.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frege, G. (1892/1952b) On sense and reference. In: Translations from the philosophical writings of Gottlob Frege, ed. Geach, P. T. & Black, M., pp. 5678. Basil Blackwell. (Original work published in 1892)Google Scholar
Lømo, T (1966) Frequency potentiation of excitatory synaptic activity in the dentate area of the hippocampal formation. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica. 68:128.Google Scholar