Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-02T19:51:47.317Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mutualism is only a part of human morality

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 February 2013

Herbert Gintis*
Affiliation:
Santa Fe Institute, Santa Fe, NM 87501, and Department of Economics, Central European University, 1051 Budapest, Hungary. [email protected]://people.umass.edu/gintis

Abstract

Baumard et al. mischaracterize our model of individual and social choice behavior. We model individuals who maximize preferences given their beliefs, and subject to their informational and material constraints (Fehr & Gintis 2007). Individuals thus must make trade-offs among self-regarding, other-regarding, and character virtue goals. Two genetic predispositions are particularly crucial. The first is strong reciprocity. The second is the capacity to internalize norms through the socialization process. Our model includes the authors' model as a subset.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bingham, P. M. (1999) Human uniqueness: A general theory. Quarterly Review of Biology 74(2):133–69.Google Scholar
Boehm, C. (1999) Hierarchy in the forest: The evolution of egalitarian behavior. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boehm, C. (2011) Moral origins: The evolution of virtue, altruism, and shame. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bowles, S. & Gintis, H. (2011) A cooperative species: Human reciprocity and its evolution. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., Gintis, H. & Bowles, S. (2010) Coordinated punishment of defectors sustains cooperation and can proliferate when rare. Science 328:617–20.Google Scholar
Durkheim, E. (1915) The elementary forms of religious life. The Free Press.Google Scholar
Fehr, E. & Gintis, H. (2007) Human motivation and social cooperation: Experimental and analytical foundations. Annual Review of Sociology 33:4364.Google Scholar
Gintis, H. (2003) The hitchhiker's guide to altruism: Genes, culture, and the internalization of norms. Journal of Theoretical Biology 220(4):407–18.Google Scholar
Gintis, H., Bowles, S., Boyd, R. & Fehr, E. (2005) Moral sentiments and material interests: On the foundations of cooperation in economic life. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., Gintis, H., McElreath, R., Alvard, M., Barr, A., Ensminger, J., Hill, K., Gil-White, F., Gurven, M., Marlowe, F., Patton, J. Q., Smith, N. & Tracer, D. (2005) “Economic man” in cross-cultural perspective: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28(6):795815; discussion: 815–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richerson, P. J. & Boyd, R. (2000) Climate, culture, and the evolution of cognition. In: The evolution of cognition, ed. Heyes, C. & Huber, L., pp. 329–46. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Savage, L. J. (1972) The foundations of statistics. Dover.Google Scholar
Wiessner, P. (2005) Norm enforcement among the Ju/'hoansi Bushmen: A case of strong reciprocity? Human Nature 16(2):115–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed