Crossref Citations
This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by Crossref.
Mochizuki, Akito Azumane
Sudo, Michiko Mochizuki
Kirino, Eiji
and
Itoh, Kenji
2014.
Brain activation associated with motor imagery of coordination exercises and social abilities.
European Journal of Sport Science,
Vol. 14,
Issue. 7,
p.
671.
Grèzes, Julie
and
Dezecache, Guillaume
2014.
How do shared-representations and emotional processes cooperate in response to social threat signals?.
Neuropsychologia,
Vol. 55,
Issue. ,
p.
105.
Kaufmann, Laurence
and
Clément, Fabrice
2014.
Wired for Society: Cognizing Pathways to Society and Culture.
Topoi,
Vol. 33,
Issue. 2,
p.
459.
Grèzes, J.
Dezecache, G.
and
Eskenazi, T.
2015.
Brain Mapping.
p.
1027.
Sartori, Luisa
2016.
Shared Representations.
p.
392.
Hamilton, Antonia
2016.
Shared Representations.
p.
313.
Jensen, Jeppe Sinding
2016.
How Institutions Work in Shared Intentionality and ‘We-Mode’ Social Cognition.
Topoi,
Vol. 35,
Issue. 1,
p.
301.
Betti, Sonia
Castiello, Umberto
Guerra, Silvia
Granziol, Umberto
Zani, Giovanni
Sartori, Luisa
and
Longo, Matthew
2019.
Gaze and body cues interplay during interactive requests.
PLOS ONE,
Vol. 14,
Issue. 10,
p.
e0223591.
Betti, Sonia
Zani, Giovanni
Guerra, Silvia
Granziol, Umberto
Castiello, Umberto
Begliomini, Chiara
and
Sartori, Luisa
2023.
When Corticospinal Inhibition Favors an Efficient Motor Response.
Biology,
Vol. 12,
Issue. 2,
p.
332.
Target article
Toward a second-person neuroscience1
Related commentaries (28)
A mature second-person neuroscience needs a first-person (plural) developmental foundation
A second-person approach cannot explain intentionality in social understanding
Advancing the neuroscience of social emotions with social immersion
Brain games: Toward a neuroecology of social behavior
From synthetic modeling of social interaction to dynamic theories of brain–body–environment–body–brain systems
From the bottom up: The roots of social neuroscience at risk of running dry?
Further steps toward a second-person neuroscience
Interaction versus observation: A finer look at this distinction and its importance to autism
It takes two to talk: A second-person neuroscience approach to language learning
Merging second-person and first-person neuroscience
Mirror neurons are central for a second-person neuroscience: Insights from developmental studies
On projecting grammatical persons into social neurocognition: A view from linguistics
Parameterising ecological validity and integrating individual differences within second-person neuroscience
Reciprocity between second-person neuroscience and cognitive robotics
Second person neuroscience needs theories as well as methods
Second-person neuroscience: Implications for Wittgensteinian and Vygotskyan approaches to psychology
Second-person social neuroscience: Connections to past and future theories, methods, and findings
Social affordances in context: What is it that we are bodily responsive to?
Social affordances: Is the mirror neuron system involved?
Social cognition is not a special case, and the dark matter is more extensive than recognized
Social perception and “spectator theories” of other minds
Talking to each other and talking together: Joint language tasks and degrees of interactivity
The brain as part of an enactive system
The second person in “I”-“you”-“it” triadic interactions
The use of non-interactive scenarios in social neuroscience
Toward a neuroscience of interactive parent–infant dyad empathy
What we can learn from second animal neuroscience
Why not the first-person plural in social cognition?
Author response
A second-person neuroscience in interaction1