Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T22:37:43.026Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

From the bottom up: The roots of social neuroscience at risk of running dry?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 July 2013

Cindy Hamon-Hill
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Neuroscience; Neuroscience Institute, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS B3H 4R2, Canada. [email protected]@dal.cawww.gadbois.org/simon
Simon Gadbois
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Neuroscience; Neuroscience Institute, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS B3H 4R2, Canada. [email protected]@dal.cawww.gadbois.org/simon

Abstract

A second-person neuroscience, as an emerging area of neuroscience and the behavioral sciences, cannot afford to avoid a bottom-up, subcortical, and conative-affective perspective. An example with canid social play and a modern motivational behavioral neursocience will illustrate our point.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bauer, E. B. & Smuts, B. B. (2007) Cooperation and competition during dyadic play in domestic dogs, Canis familiaris . Animal Behaviour 73:489–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bekoff, M. (2004) Wild justice and fair play: Cooperation, forgiveness, and morality in animals. Biology and Philosophy 19:489520.Google Scholar
Bekoff, M. (2008) The emotional lives of animals. New World Library.Google Scholar
Berridge, K. C. (2003) Pleasures of the brain. Brain and Cognition 52:106–28.Google Scholar
Burgdorf, J., Panksepp, J. & Moskal, J. R. (2011) Frequency-modulated 50 kHz ultrasonic vocalizations: A tool for uncovering the molecular substrates of positive affect. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35:1831–36.Google Scholar
Burgdorf, J., Wood, P. L., Kroes, R. A., Moskal, J. R. & Panksepp, J. (2007) Neurobiology of 50 kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in rats: Electrode mapping, lesion, and pharmacology studies. Behavioural Brain Research 182:274–83.Google ScholarPubMed
Burghardt, G. M. (2005) The genesis of animal play. MIT Press.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. (2008) Putting the altruism back into altruism: The evolution of empathy. Annual Review Psychology 59:279300. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625.Google Scholar
Fentress, J. C. & Gadbois, S. (2001) The development of action sequences. In: Handbook of behavioral neurobiology: Developmental psychobiology, vol. 13, ed. Blass, E. M., pp. 393430. Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Fentress, J. C. & McLeod, P. J. (1986) Motor patterns in development. In: Handbook of behavioral neurobiology: Developmental psychobiology, vol. 8, ed. Blass, E. M., pp. 3597. Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Kringelbach, M. L. & Berridge, K. C. (2009) Towards a functional neuroanatomy of pleasure and happiness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13:479–87.Google Scholar
Miklosi, A. (2008) Dog behaviour, evolution, and cognition. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Palagi, E. & Cordoni, G. (2009) Postconflict third-party affiliation in Canis lupus: Do wolves share similarities with the great apes? Animal Behaviour 78:979–86.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1998) Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2011) The basic emotional circuits of mammalian brains: Do animals have affective lives? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35:1791–804.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panksepp, J., Normansell, L., Cox, J. F. & Siviy, S. M. (1994) Effects of neonatal decortication on the social play of juvenile rats. Physiology and Behavior 56:429–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parvizi, J. (2009) Corticocentric myopia: Old bias in new cognitive sciences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13:354–59. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.04.008.Google Scholar
Pellis, S. M., Pellis, V. C. & Whitshaw, I. Q. (1992) The role of the cortex in play fighting by rats: Developmental and evolutionary implications. Brain, Behaviour and Evolution 39:270–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siviy, S. M. (1998) Neurobiological substrates of play behavior. In: Animal play: Evolutionary, comparative and ecological perspectives, ed. Bekoff, M. & Beyers, J. A., pp. 221–42. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Siviy, S. M. & Panksepp, J. (2011) In search of the neurobiological substrates for social playfulness in mammalian brains. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35:1821–30. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.03.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, K. S., Berridge, K. C. & Aldridge, J. W. (2011) Disentangling pleasure from incentive salience and learning signals in the brain reward circuitry. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 10:10935–36. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1101920108.Google Scholar
Smith, L. & Thelen, E. (2003) Development as a dynamic system. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7:343–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thelen, E. & Smith, L. B. (1994) A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. MIT Press.Google Scholar