Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T07:45:04.479Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Body ownership as a proxy for individual and social separation and connection

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 February 2021

Marina Scattolin
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Medicine and Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, 00185Rome, [email protected]://agliotilab.org/lab-staff/phd-students/3rd-year/marina-scattolin#anchor IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, 00179Rome, [email protected]://agliotilab.org/lab-staff/senior-fellows/maria-serena-panasiti
Maria Serena Panasiti
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Medicine and Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, 00185Rome, [email protected]://agliotilab.org/lab-staff/phd-students/3rd-year/marina-scattolin#anchor IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, 00179Rome, [email protected]://agliotilab.org/lab-staff/senior-fellows/maria-serena-panasiti
Salvatore Maria Aglioti
Affiliation:
IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, 00179Rome, [email protected]://agliotilab.org/lab-staff/senior-fellows/maria-serena-panasiti Sapienza University of Rome and Center for Life Nano Science, Italian Institute of Technology, 00161Rome, Italy. [email protected]://agliotilab.org/lab-staff/principal-investigator

Abstract

Lee and Schwartz procedures of separation offer a much needed interpretation of the literature on moral cleansing. However, body ownership as a grounded mechanism of separation and connection has been neglected. We argue that embodiment may be employed to connect the self to desirable aspects of cognitive and emotional interactions and disembodiment to disconnect from undesirable elements.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aglioti, S., Smania, N., Manfredi, M., & Berlucchi, G. (1996). Disownership of left hand and objects related to it in a patient with right brain damage. NeuroReport, 8(1), 293296. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-199612200-00058.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aymerich-Franch, L., Kishore, S., & Slater, M. (2020). When your robot avatar misbehaves you are likely to apologize: An exploration of guilt during robot embodiment. International Journal of Social Robotics, 12, 217226. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-019-00556-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Banakou, D., Groten, R., & Slater, M. (2013). Illusory ownership of a virtual child body causes overestimation of object sizes and implicit attitude changes. PNAS, 110(31), 1284612851. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1306779110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berlucchi, G., & Aglioti, S. M. (1997). The body in the brain: Neural bases of corporeal awareness. Trends in Neurosciences, 20(12), 560564. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(97)01136-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanke, O. (2012). Multisensory brain mechanisms of bodily self-consciousness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13, 556571. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botvinick, M., & Cohen, J. (1998). Rubber hands “feel” touch that eyes see. Nature, 391, 756. https://doi.org/10.1038/35784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bourdin, P., Barberia, I., Oliva, R., & Slater, M. (2017). A virtual out-of-body experience reduces fear of death. PLoS ONE, 12(1), e0169343. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brugger, P., Lenggenhager, B., & Giummarra, M. J. (2013). Xenomelia: A social neuroscience view of altered bodily self-consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 204. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bufalari, I., Lenggenhager, B., Porciello, G., Holmes, B. S., & Aglioti, S. M. (2014). Enfacing others but only if they are nice to you. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 102. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fusaro, M., Tieri, G., & Aglioti, S. M. (2016). Seeing pain and pleasure on self and others: Behavioral and psychophysiological reactivity in immersive virtual reality. Journal of Neurophysiology, 116(6), 26562662. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00489.2016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giummarra, M. J., Gibson, S. J., Georgiou-Karistianis, N., & Bradshaw, J. L. (2008). Mechanisms underlying embodiment, disembodiment and loss of embodiment. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32(1), 143160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.07.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gonzalez-Liencres, C., Zapata, L. E., Iruretagoyena, G., Seinfeld, S., Perez-Mendez, L., Arroyo-Palacios, J., … Sanchez-Vives, M. V. (2020). Being the victim of intimate partner violence in virtual reality: First- versus third-person perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 820. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00820.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guterstam, A., Abdulkarim, Z., & Ehrsson, H. H. (2015). Illusory ownership of an invisible body reduces autonomic and subjective social anxiety responses. Scientific Reports, 5, 9831. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep09831.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofer, M., Hüsser, A., & Prabhu, S. (2017). The effect of an avatar's emotional expressions on players’ fear reactions: The mediating role of embodiment. Computers in Human Behavior, 75, 883890. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.06.024.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ma, K., Sellaro, R., Lippelt, D. P., & Hommel, B. (2016). Mood migration: How enfacing a smile makes you happier. Cognition, 151, 5262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.02.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maister, L., Slater, M., Sanchez-Vives, M. V., & Tsakiris, M. (2015). Changing bodies changes minds: Owning another body affects social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(1), 612. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.11.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martini, M., Kilteni, K., Maselli, A., & Sanchez-Vives, M. V. (2015). The body fades away: Investigating the effects of transparency of an embodied virtual body on pain threshold and body ownership. Scientific Reports, 5, 13948. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep13948.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monti, A., Porciello, G., Tieri, G., & Aglioti, S. M. (2020). The “embreathment” illusion highlights the role of breathing in corporeal awareness. Journal of Neurophysiology, 123(1), 420427. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00617.2019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paladino, M. P., Mazzurega, M., Pavani, F., & Schubert, T. W. (2010). Synchronous multisensory stimulation blurs self-other boundaries. Psychological Science, 21(9), 12021207. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610379234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peck, T. C., Seinfeld, S., Aglioti, S. M., & Slater, M. (2013). Putting yourself in the skin of a black avatar reduces implicit racial bias. Consciousness and Cognition, 22, 779787. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2013.04.016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pezzetta, R., Nicolardi, V., Tidoni, E., & Aglioti, S. M. (2018). Error, rather than its probability, elicits specific electrocortical signatures: A combined EEG-immersive virtual reality study of action observation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 120(3), 11071118. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00130.2018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porciello, G., Bufalari, I., Minio-Paluello, I., di Pace, E., & Aglioti, S. M. (2018). The “enfacement” illusion: A window on the plasticity of the self. Cortex, 104, 261275. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.01.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Provenzano, L., Porciello, G., Ciccarone, S., Lenggenhager, B., Tieri, G., Marucci, M., Dazzi, F., Loriedo, C., & Bufalari, I. (2020). Characterizing body image distortion and bodily self-plasticity in anorexia nervosa via visuo-tactile stimulation in virtual reality. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 9(98), 117. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9010098.Google Scholar
Sforza, A., Bufalari, I., Haggard, P., & Aglioti, S. M. (2010). My face in yours: Visuo-tactile facial stimulation influences sense of identity. Social Neuroscience, 5(2), 148162. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470910903205503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slater, M., Perez-Marcos, D., Ehrsson, H. H., & Sanchez-Vives, M. V. (2009). Inducing illusory ownership of a virtual body. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 3(2), 214220. https://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.01.029.2009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tieri, G., Tidoni, E., Pavone, E. F., & Aglioti, S. M. (2015). Body visual discontinuity affects feeling of ownership and skin conductance responses. Scientific Reports, 5, 17139. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep17139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yee, N., Bailenson, J. N., & Ducheneaut, N. (2009). The Proteus effect: Implications of transformed digital self-representation on online and offline behavior. Communication Research, 36(2), 285312. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650208330254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar