Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T03:04:05.890Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

We know what stops you from thinking forever: A metacognitive perspective

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 July 2023

Rakefet Ackerman
Affiliation:
Faculty of Data and Decision Sciences, Technion – Israel Institute of Technology, Haifa, Israel [email protected]; technion.ac.il
Kinga Morsanyi
Affiliation:
Centre for Mathematical Cognition, Loughborough University, Loughborough, UK [email protected]; lboro.ac.uk

Abstract

This commentary addresses omissions in De Neys's model of fast-and-slow thinking from a metacognitive perspective. We review well-established meta-reasoning monitoring (e.g., confidence) and control processes (e.g., rethinking) that explain mental effort regulation. Moreover, we point to individual, developmental, and task design considerations that affect this regulation. These core issues are completely ignored or mentioned in passing in the target article.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

This commentary addresses several major omissions in De Neys's “working model.” We predominantly focus on gaps in the conceptualization of the “switch feature” and stopping deliberative processes (S2).

Metacognitive research deals with the monitoring and control of thinking processes (Nelson & Narens, Reference Nelson, Narens and Bower1990). More than 30 years of research have dealt with the processes that inform subjective assessments of success (e.g., confidence) and the subsequent decisions (e.g., to rethink, see Fiedler, Ackerman, & Scarampi, Reference Fiedler, Ackerman, Scarampi, Sternberg and Funke2019). Of particular relevance is the meta-reasoning framework (Ackerman & Thompson, Reference Ackerman and Thompson2017), which is mentioned briefly in section 4.4. By using well-established metacognitive concepts, this framework opens the “black box” of mental effort regulation. It details monitoring and control processes that take place in the early intuitive reasoning stages (S1) separately from the deliberative stages (S2), including processes discussed in the target article and more.

First, the processes covered by the “switch feature” are discussed in length in the literature initiated by Thompson, Prowse Turner, and Pennycook (Reference Thompson, Prowse Turner and Pennycook2011) using the two-response paradigm with feeling of rightness judgment (FOR, mentioned in sect. 4.4; Ackerman & Thompson, Reference Ackerman and Thompson2017). FOR is the metacognitive judgment that accompanies the initial response that comes to mind. It has been considered to trigger the switch between S1 and S2 and found to predict S2 engagement (e.g., Thompson et al., Reference Thompson, Prowse Turner, Pennycook, Ball, Brack, Ophir and Ackerman2013).

A further issue is that the proposed model is incomplete in that the alleged “switch mechanism” is considered to depend entirely on the relative activation levels of competing intuitions and the mysterious “deliberation threshold.” In fact, a variety of situational and personal factors have been found to affect metacognitive control decisions, such as reasoning time and response choice. Specifically, task design, such as instructions to reason logically (e.g., Ferreira, Garcia-Marques, Sherman, & Sherman, Reference Ferreira, Garcia-Marques, Sherman and Sherman2006; Morsanyi, Primi, Chiesi, & Handley, Reference Morsanyi, Primi, Chiesi and Handley2009), cognitive load (De Neys, Reference De Neys2006; Morsanyi, Busdraghi, & Primi, Reference Morsanyi, Busdraghi and Primi2014), and time pressure (Sidi, Shpigelman, Zalmanov, & Ackerman, Reference Sidi, Shpigelman, Zalmanov and Ackerman2017), as well as individual characteristics, such as thinking dispositions (Cacioppo, Petty, Feinstein, & Jarvis, Reference Cacioppo, Petty, Feinstein and Jarvis1996), cognitive ability (e.g., Stanovich & West, Reference Stanovich and West2000), task-relevant knowledge (e.g., Chiesi, Primi, & Morsanyi, Reference Chiesi, Primi and Morsanyi2011; Stanovich & West, Reference Stanovich and West2008), and anxiety levels (e.g., Beilock & DeCaro, Reference Beilock and DeCaro2007; Primi, Donati, Chiesi, & Morsanyi, Reference Primi, Donati, Chiesi and Morsanyi2018) affect reasoning time and response choice. Thus, any model explaining the “switch feature” should incorporate and account for the contextual and individual factors that influence the reasoning process.

Second, the target article discusses stopping deliberative processes (S2) and reverting to S1. An overlooked issue, though, is when to stop S2 and provide a response. Within the metacognitive literature, several models address stopping effortful thinking: The discrepancy reduction models (Nelson & Narens, Reference Nelson, Narens and Bower1990), the region of proximal learning (Metcalfe & Kornell, Reference Metcalfe and Kornell2005), and the diminishing criterion model (DCM, Ackerman, Reference Ackerman2014; see Ackerman, Yom-Tov, & Torgovitsky, Reference Ackerman, Yom-Tov and Torgovitsky2020, for a review). According to the most recent model, the DCM, stopping thinking efforts is guided by a combination of two stopping criteria: (a) Confidence in each considered answer is compared to a desired confidence level. Importantly, this stopping criterion dynamically drops as people deliberate longer, reflecting compromising on expected success. (b) A time limit for thinking about each task item, beyond which people are reluctant to think any further (see also Hawkins & Heathcote, Reference Hawkins and Heathcote2021).

Third, based on the suggested model, “System 2 deliberation will extend for as long as the uncertainty remains above the threshold” (target article, sect. 3.4, para. 3). Thus, under substantial uncertainty people are doomed to think forever. Nevertheless, a totally overlooked aspect is when people opt out (e.g., “I don't know”) or turn to external help (see Ackerman, Reference Ackerman2014; Undorf, Livneh, & Ackerman, Reference Undorf, Livneh and Ackerman2021). In particular, considering children and novices brings to the fore that people looking at unfamiliar problems may not have any available heuristics to activate. Developmentally, there is a blurry line between deliberative and intuitive processes (Osman & Stavy, Reference Osman and Stavy2006) in that responses that can be given quasi automatically by adults may require cognitive effort for children (Morsanyi & Handley, Reference Morsanyi and Handley2008) and may become established by learning (Fischbein, Reference Fischbein1987; Gauvrit & Morsanyi, Reference Gauvrit and Morsanyi2014). De Neys briefly considers lack of S1 response (sect. 2.1.5). Another possibility is that people may activate a series of distantly related heuristics, but none of these would be sufficiently strong to offer an answer. In contrast, according to the DCM, when people get to a pre-set time limit, they may prefer opting out over providing a low confidence response. This topic was discussed in metacognitive research already in the 1990s (Koriat & Goldsmith, Reference Koriat and Goldsmith1996) and was further developed since then (see Undorf et al., Reference Undorf, Livneh and Ackerman2021). Thus, there are processes that prevent people from thinking forever.

Fourth, De Neys asks in the Introduction “how do we know that we can rely on an intuitively cued problem solution” (target article, para. 4 in the Introduction) and mentions that “the internal switch decision is itself intuitive in nature” (target article, para. 4 in the Introduction). In metacognitive terms, these intuitions are based on heuristic cues that underlie all metacognitive judgments (Koriat, Reference Koriat1997). Metacognitive judgments combine an extensive amount of features (Undorf & Bröder, Reference Undorf and Bröder2021), including individual self-perceptions and beliefs (“beyond my expertise”), task characteristics (time pressure), and item characteristics (conclusion believability) that may influence, and sometimes mislead, metacognitive judgments (see Ackerman, Reference Ackerman2019). Given the wide-spread biases in judgments like FOR and confidence (Thompson et al., Reference Thompson, Prowse Turner, Pennycook, Ball, Brack, Ophir and Ackerman2013), considering potential misleading factors must be incorporated in any model of switch and stopping mechanisms.

Finally, from a developmental perspective, adults have a larger repertoire of heuristics and better ability to integrate them into their cognitive and metacognitive processes than children (Koriat, Ackerman, Adiv, Lockl, & Schneider, Reference Koriat, Ackerman, Adiv, Lockl and Schneider2014). However, in the proposed model, the more heuristics are considered, the longer the thinking process that deals with potential conflicts among them. This contrasts with the traditional role assigned to reasoning heuristics – that they offer immediately available (and highly compelling) responses immediately (e.g., Evans, Reference Evans2006), which is why they are considered to be adaptive and essential parts of the cognitive architecture.

In sum, the proposed model ignores well-established bodies of literature that address the central issues it was meant to cover. Particularly, metacognitive research offers switch and stopping rules, heuristic processes, individual characteristics, and developmental trajectories required for describing the complex processes underlying reasoning.

Financial support

This work was funded by the Israel Science Foundation (grant reference: 234/18) to R.A. and by the Economic and Social Research Council, UK (grant reference: ES/W002914/1) to K.M.

Competing interest

None.

References

Ackerman, R. (2014). The diminishing criterion model for metacognitive regulation of time investment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(3), 13491368.10.1037/a0035098CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ackerman, R. (2019). Heuristic cues for meta-reasoning judgments: Review and methodology. Psychological Topics, 28(1), 120.Google Scholar
Ackerman, R., & Thompson, V. (2017). Meta-reasoning: Monitoring and control of thinking and reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(8), 607617.10.1016/j.tics.2017.05.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ackerman, R., Yom-Tov, E., & Torgovitsky, I. (2020). Using confidence and consensuality to predict time invested in problem solving and in real-life web searching. Cognition, 199, 104248.10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104248CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beilock, S. L., & DeCaro, M. S. (2007). From poor performance to success under stress: Working memory, strategy selection, and mathematical problem solving under pressure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(6), 983998.Google ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Feinstein, J. A., & Jarvis, W. B. G. (1996). Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 197253.10.1037/0033-2909.119.2.197CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chiesi, F., Primi, C., & Morsanyi, K. (2011). Developmental changes in probabilistic reasoning: The role of cognitive capacity, instructions, thinking styles, and relevant knowledge. Thinking & Reasoning, 17(3), 315350.10.1080/13546783.2011.598401CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Neys, W. (2006). Dual processing in reasoning: Two systems but one reasoner. Psychological Science, 17(5), 428433.10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01723.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J. S. B. (2006). The heuristic-analytic theory of reasoning: Extension and evaluation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(3), 378395.10.3758/BF03193858CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferreira, M. B., Garcia-Marques, L., Sherman, S. J., & Sherman, J. W. (2006). Automatic and controlled components of judgment and decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 797813.10.1037/0022-3514.91.5.797CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiedler, K., Ackerman, R., & Scarampi, C. (2019). Metacognition: Monitoring and controlling one's own knowledge, reasoning and decisions. In Sternberg, R. J. & Funke, J. (Eds.), Introduction to the psychology of human thought (pp. 89111). Heidelberg University Publishing.Google Scholar
Fischbein, E. (1987). Intuition in science and mathematics. Reidel.Google Scholar
Gauvrit, N., & Morsanyi, K. (2014). The equiprobability bias from a mathematical and psychological perspective. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 10, 119130.10.5709/acp-0163-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkins, G. E., & Heathcote, A. (2021). Racing against the clock: Evidence-based versus time-based decisions. Psychological Review, 128(2), 222263.10.1037/rev0000259CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koriat, A. (1997). Monitoring one's own knowledge during study: A cue-utilization approach to judgments of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126, 349370.10.1037/0096-3445.126.4.349CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koriat, A., Ackerman, R., Adiv, S., Lockl, K., & Schneider, W. (2014). The effects of goal-driven and data-driven regulation on metacognitive monitoring during learning: A developmental perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(1), 386403.10.1037/a0031768CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koriat, A., & Goldsmith, M. (1996). Monitoring and control processes in the strategic regulation of memory accuracy. Psychological Review, 103(3), 490517.10.1037/0033-295X.103.3.490CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Metcalfe, J., & Kornell, N. (2005). A region of proximal learning model of study time allocation. Journal of Memory and Language, 52(4), 463477.10.1016/j.jml.2004.12.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morsanyi, K., Busdraghi, C., & Primi, C. (2014). Mathematical anxiety is linked to reduced cognitive reflection: A potential road from discomfort in the mathematics classroom to susceptibility to biases. Behavioral and Brain Functions 2014, 10, 31.10.1186/1744-9081-10-31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morsanyi, K., & Handley, S. J. (2008). How smart do you need to be to get it wrong? The role of cognitive capacity in the development of heuristic-based judgment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 99, 1836.10.1016/j.jecp.2007.08.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morsanyi, K., Primi, C., Chiesi, F., & Handley, S. J. (2009). The effects and side-effects of statistics education. Psychology students’ (mis-)conceptions of probability. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34, 210220.10.1016/j.cedpsych.2009.05.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings. In Bower, G. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 26, pp. 125173). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Osman, M., & Stavy, M. O. R. (2006). Development of intuitive rules: Evaluating the application of the dual-system framework to understanding children’s intuitive reasoning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 935953.10.3758/BF03213907CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Primi, C., Donati, M., Chiesi, F., & Morsanyi, K. (2018). Are there gender differences in cognitive reflection? Invariance and differences related to mathematics. Thinking & Reasoning, 24, 258279.10.1080/13546783.2017.1387606CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sidi, Y., Shpigelman, M., Zalmanov, H., & Ackerman, R. (2017). Understanding metacognitive inferiority on screen by exposing cues for depth of processing. Learning and Instruction, 51, 6173.10.1016/j.learninstruc.2017.01.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(5), 645665.10.1017/S0140525X00003435CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2008). On the relative independence of thinking biases and cognitive ability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(4), 672695.10.1037/0022-3514.94.4.672CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, V., Prowse Turner, J., Pennycook, G., Ball, L., Brack, H., Ophir, Y., & Ackerman, R. (2013). The role of answer fluency and perceptual fluency as metacognitive cues for initiating analytic thinking. Cognition, 128, 237251.10.1016/j.cognition.2012.09.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, V. A., Prowse Turner, J. A., & Pennycook, G. (2011). Intuition, reason, and metacognition. Cognitive Psychology, 63(3), 107140.10.1016/j.cogpsych.2011.06.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Undorf, M., & Bröder, A. (2021). Metamemory for pictures of naturalistic scenes: Assessment of accuracy and cue utilization. Memory & Cognition, 49(7), 14051422.10.3758/s13421-021-01170-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Undorf, M., Livneh, I., & Ackerman, R. (2021). Metacognitive control processes in question answering: Help seeking and withholding answers. Metacognition & Learning, 16, 431458.10.1007/s11409-021-09259-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar