Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T12:06:21.675Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

When dyadic interaction is the context: Mimicry behaviors on the origin of imitation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 December 2017

Ruth Campos
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology, Campus de Cantoblanco, 28049 Madrid, Spain. [email protected]@uam.eshttps://www.uam.es/ss/Satellite/Psicologia/en/1242653130931/1242653125813/persona/detallePDI/Campos_Garciahttps://www.uam.es/ss/Satellite/Psicologia/es/1242653130931/1242653126657/persona/detallePDI/Nieto_Vizcaino,_Carmen.htm
Carmen Nieto
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology, Campus de Cantoblanco, 28049 Madrid, Spain. [email protected]@uam.eshttps://www.uam.es/ss/Satellite/Psicologia/en/1242653130931/1242653125813/persona/detallePDI/Campos_Garciahttps://www.uam.es/ss/Satellite/Psicologia/es/1242653130931/1242653126657/persona/detallePDI/Nieto_Vizcaino,_Carmen.htm

Abstract

Keven & Akins (K&A) redefine some of the neonatal imitation (NI) behaviors as developmental stereotypes. From a neuroconstructivist framework, those early gestures are also far from being considered as imitative behaviors. The cognitive substrate of imitation requires an interactive context to develop. Prior to intentional imitation, the dyad shows mimicry behaviors, which are automatic, but do not fade through development.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bahrick, L. E., Lickliter, R. & Flom, R. (2004) Intersensory redundancy guides the development of selective attention, perception, and cognition in infancy. Current Directions in Psychological Science 13(3):99102. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00283.x.Google Scholar
Carpenter, M. & Tomasello, M. (2005) Intention reading and imitative learning. In: Perspectives on imitation: From neuroscience to social science, ed. Chater, N. & Hurley, S., pp. 133–48. MIT.Google Scholar
Carpenter, M., Uebel, J. & Tomasello, M. (2013) Being mimicked increases prosocial behavior in 18-month-old infants. Child Development 84(5):1511–18. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12083.Google Scholar
Chartrand, T. L. & Lakin, J. L. (2013) The antecedents and consequences of human behavioral mimicry. Annual Review of Psychology 64:285308. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143754.Google Scholar
Duffy, K. A. & Chartrand, T. L. (2015) Mimicry: Causes and consequences. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 3:112–16. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.03.002.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. H., Senju, A. & Tomalski, P. (2015) The two-process theory of face processing: Modifications based on two decades of data from infants and adults. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 50:169–79. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.009.Google Scholar
Keven, N. & Akins, K. (2016) Neonatal imitation in context: Sensory-motor development in the perinatal period. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1107. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X16000911.Google Scholar
Lakin, J. L., Jefferis, V. E., Cheng, C. M. & Chartrand, T. L. (2003) The chameleon effect as social glue: Evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 27(3):145–62. Available at:https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025389814290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lavelli, M. & Fogel, A. (2005) Developmental changes in the relationship between the infant's attention and emotion during early face-to-face communication: The 2-month transition. Developmental Psychology 41(1):265–80. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.41.1.265.Google Scholar
Legerstee, M. & Varghese, J. (2001) The role of maternal affect mirroring on social expectancies in three-month-old infants. Child Development 72(5):1301–13. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00349.Google Scholar
Lewkowicz, D. J. (2014) Early experience and multisensory perceptual narrowing. Developmental Psychobiology 56(2):292315. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21197.Google Scholar
Mareschal, D., Johnson, M. H., Sirois, S., Spratling, M., Thomas, M. & Westermann, G. (2007) Neuroconstructivism, vol. I: How the brain constructs cognition. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. & Moore, M. K. (1977) Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198(4312):7578. Available at: http://science.sciencemag.org/content/198/4312/75.Google Scholar
Moody, E. J. & McIntosh, D. N. (2006) Autism and mimicry: Bases and consequences of rapid, automatic matching behavior. In: Imitation and the social mind: Autism and typical development, ed. Rogers, S. J. & Williams, J., pp. 7195. Guilford.Google Scholar
Morton, J. & Johnson, M. H. (1991) CONSPEC and CONLERN: A two-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychological Review 98(2):164. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.164.Google Scholar
Oostenbroek, J., Suddendorf, T., Nielsen, M., Redshaw, J., Kennedy-Costantini, S., Davis, J., Clark, S. & Slaughter, V. (2016) Comprehensive longitudinal study challenges the existence of neonatal imitation in humans. Current Biology 26(10):1334–38. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.03.047.Google Scholar
Soussignan, R., Nadel, J., Canet, P. & Gerardin, P. (2006) Sensitivity to social contingency and positive emotion in 2-month-olds. Infancy 10(2):123–44. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in1002_2.Google Scholar
Thelen, E. (1981b) Rhythmical behavior in infancy: An ethological perspective. Developmental Psychology 17(3):237–57. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.17.3.237.Google Scholar
Volpe, J. (2008) Neurology of the newborn infant, 5th ed. Saunders/Elsevier.Google Scholar
Wan, M. W., Green, J., Elsabbagh, M., Johnson, M., Charman, T. & Plummer, F. (2013) Quality of interaction between at-risk infants and caregiver at 12–15 months is associated with 3-year autism outcome. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 54(7):763–71. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12032.Google Scholar
Wörmann, V., Holodynski, M., Kärtner, J. & Keller, H. (2012) A cross-cultural comparison of the development of the social smile: A longitudinal study of maternal and infant imitation in 6-and 12-week-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development 35(3):335–47. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.03.002.Google Scholar