No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Vocal communication is multi-sensorimotor coordination within and between individuals
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 17 December 2014
Abstract
Speech is an exquisitely coordinated interaction among effectors both within and between individuals. No account of human communication evolution that ignores its foundational multisensory characteristics and cooperative nature will be satisfactory. Here, we describe two additional capacities – rhythmic audiovisual speech and cooperative communication – and suggest that they may utilize the very same or similar circuits as those proposed for vocal learning.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014
References
Ackermann, H. & Riecker, A. (2004) The contribution of the insula to motor aspects of speech production: A review and a hypothesis. Brain and Language
89:320–28.Google Scholar
Bauernfiend, A. L., de Sousa, A. A., Avasthi, T., Dobson, S. D., Raghanti, M. A., Lewandowski, A. H., Zilles, K., Semendeferi, K., Allman, J. M., Craig, A. D., Hof, P. R. & Sherwood, C. C. (2013) A volumetric comparison of the insular cortex and its subregions in primates. Journal of Human Evolution
64:263–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bergman, T. J. (2013) Speech-like vocalized lip-smacking in geladas. Current Biology
23(7):R268–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bohland, J. W. & Guenther, F. H. (2006) An fMRI investigation of syllable sequence production. NeuroImage
2:821–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burkart, J. M., Hrdy, S. B. & van Schaik, C. P. (2009a) Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology
18:175–86.Google Scholar
Caruana, F., Jezzini, A., Sbriscia-Fioretti, B., Rizzolatti, G. & Gallese, V. (2011) Emotional and social behaviors elicited by electrical stimulation of the insula in the macaque monkey. Current Biology
21:195–99.Google Scholar
Chandrasekaran, C., Lemus, L., Trubanova, A., Gondan, M. & Ghazanfar, A. A. (2011) Monkeys and humans share a common computation for face/voice integration. PLOS Computational Biology
7(9):e1002165.Google Scholar
Chandrasekaran, C., Trubanova, A., Stillittano, S., Caplier, A. & Ghazanfar, A. A. (2009) The natural statistics of audiovisual speech. PLOS Computational Biology
5:e1000436.Google Scholar
Dronkers, N. F. (1996) A new brain region for coordinating speech articulation. Nature
384(6605):159–61. doi: 10.1038/384159a0.Google Scholar
Fernandez-Duque, E., Valeggia, C. R. & Mendoza, S. P. (2009) The biology of paternal care in human and nonhuman primates. Annual Review of Anthropology
38:115–30.Google Scholar
Ferrari, P. F., Paukner, A., Ionica, C. & Suomi, S. J. (2009) Reciprocal face-to-face communication between rhesus macaque mothers and their newborn infants. Current Biology
19:1768–72.Google Scholar
Ghazanfar, A. A. (2013) Multisensory vocal communication in primates and the evolution of rhythmic speech. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology
67(9):1441–48.Google Scholar
Ghazanfar, A. A., Chandrasekaran, C. & Morrill, R. J. (2010) Dynamic, rhythmic facial expressions and the superior temporal sulcus of macaque monkeys: Implications for the evolution of audiovisual speech. European Journal of Neuroscience
31:1807–17.Google Scholar
Ghazanfar, A. A. & Logothetis, N. K. (2003) Facial expressions linked to monkey calls. Nature
423(6943):937–38.Google Scholar
Ghazanfar, A. A. & Poeppel, D. (2014) The neurophysiology and evolution of the speech rhythm. In: The cognitive neurosciences V (5th edition), ed. Gazzaniga, M. S. & Mangun, G. R., pp. 629–38. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ghazanfar, A. A. & Rendall, D. (2008) Evolution of human vocal production. Current Biology
18(11):R457–60.Google Scholar
Ghazanfar, A. A., Takahashi, D. Y., Mathur, N. & Fitch, W. T. (2012) Cineradiography of monkey lipsmacking reveals the putative origins of speech dynamics. Current Biology
22:1176–82.Google Scholar
Ghazanfar, A. A., Turesson, H. K., Maier, J. X., van Dinther, R., Patterson, R. D. & Logothetis, N. K. (2007) Vocal tract resonances as indexical cues in rhesus monkeys. Current Biology
17:425–30.Google Scholar
Greenberg, S., Carvey, H., Hitchcock, L. & Chang, S. (2003) Temporal properties of spontaneous speech – a syllable-centric perspective. Journal of Phonetics
31
(3–4):465–85.Google Scholar
Habbershon, H. M., Ahmed, S. Z. & Cohen, Y. E. (2013) Rhesus macaques recognize unique multimodal face-voice relations of familiar individuals and not of unfamiliar ones. Brain, Behavior, and Evolution
81:219–25.Google Scholar
Hasson, U., Ghazanfar, A. A., Galantucci, B., Garrod, S. & Keysers, C. (2012) Brain-to-brain coupling: A mechanism for creating and sharing a social world. Trends in Cognitive Sciences
16(2):114–21.Google Scholar
Jordan, K. E., Brannon, E. M., Logothetis, N. K. & Ghazanfar, A. A. (2005) Monkeys match the number of voices they hear with the number of faces they see. Current Biology
15:1034–38.Google Scholar
Lund, J. P. & Kolta, A. (2006) Brainstem circuits that control mastication: Do they have anything to say during speech?
Journal of Communication Disorders
39:381–90.Google Scholar
MacNeilage, P. F. (1998) The frame/content theory of evolution of speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences
21(4):499–511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrill, R. J., Paukner, A., Ferrari, P. F. & Ghazanfar, A. A. (2012) Monkey lip-smacking develops like the human speech rhythm. Developmental Science
15:557–68.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G. & Arbib, M. A. (1998) Language within our grasp. Trends in Neurosciences
21:188–94.Google Scholar
Sliwa, J., Duhamel, J. R., Pascalis, O. & Wirth, S. (2011) Spontaneous voice-face identity matching by rhesus monkeys for familiar conspecifics and humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA
108:1735–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stivers, T., Enfield, N. J., Brown, P., Englert, C., Hayashi, M., Heinemann, T., Hoymann, G., Rossano, F., de Ruiter, J. P., Yoon, K. E. & Levinson, S. C. (2009) Universals and cultural variation in turn-taking in conversation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA
106:10587–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Syal, S. & Finlay, B. L. (2011) Thinking outside the cortex: Social motivation in the evolution and development of language. Developmental Science
14:417–30.Google Scholar
Takahashi, D. Y., Narayanan, D. Z. & Ghazanfar, A. A. (2013) Coupled oscillator dynamics of vocal turn-taking in monkeys. Current Biology
23:2162–68.Google Scholar
Van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1962) Facial expressions of higher primates. Symposium of the Zoological Society, London
8:97–125.Google Scholar
Target article
Brain mechanisms of acoustic communication in humans and nonhuman primates: An evolutionary perspective
Related commentaries (30)
Beyond cry and laugh: Toward a multilevel model of language production
Comparative analyses of speech and language converge on birds
Contribution of the basal ganglia to spoken language: Is speech production like the other motor skills?
Differences in auditory timing between human and nonhuman primates
Does it talk the talk? On the role of basal ganglia in emotive speech processing
Early human communication helps in understanding language evolution
En route to disentangle the impact and neurobiological substrates of early vocalizations: Learning from Rett syndrome
Environments organize the verbal brain
Evolution of affective and linguistic disambiguation under social eavesdropping pressures
Functional neuroimaging of human vocalizations and affective speech
Functions of the cortico-basal ganglia circuits for spoken language may extend beyond emotional-affective modulation in adults
Modification of spectral features by nonhuman primates
Neanderthals did speak, but FOXP2 doesn't prove it
Perceptual elements in brain mechanisms of acoustic communication in humans and nonhuman primates
Phonation takes precedence over articulation in development as well as evolution of language
Physical mechanisms may be as important as brain mechanisms in evolution of speech
Speech as a breakthrough signaling resource in the cognitive evolution of biological complex adaptive systems
Speech prosody, reward, and the corticobulbar system: An integrative perspective
Speech, vocal production learning, and the comparative method
The basal ganglia within a cognitive system in birds and mammals
The evolution of coordinated vocalizations before language
The forgotten role of consonant-like calls in theories of speech evolution
The sensorimotor and social sides of the architecture of speech
The sound of one hand clapping: Overdetermination and the pansensory nature of communication
Very young infants' responses to human and nonhuman primate vocalizations
Vocal communication is multi-sensorimotor coordination within and between individuals
Vocal learning, prosody, and basal ganglia: Don't underestimate their complexity1
Voluntary and involuntary processes affect the production of verbal and non-verbal signals by the human voice
Why vocal production of atypical sounds in apes and its cerebral correlates have a lot to say about the origin of language
Why we can talk, debate, and change our minds: Neural circuits, basal ganglia operations, and transcriptional factors
Author response
Phylogenetic reorganization of the basal ganglia: A necessary, but not the only, bridge over a primate Rubicon of acoustic communication