Crossref Citations
This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by Crossref.
Robenalt, Clarice
and
Goldberg, Adele E.
2015.
Judgment evidence for statistical preemption: It is relatively better to vanish than to disappear a rabbit, but a lifeguard can equally well backstroke or swim children to shore.
Cognitive Linguistics,
Vol. 26,
Issue. 3,
p.
467.
GOLDBERG, ADELE E.
2016.
Partial productivity of linguistic constructions: Dynamic categorization and statistical preemption.
Language and Cognition,
Vol. 8,
Issue. 3,
p.
369.
Luke, Steven G.
and
Asplund, Anna
2018.
Prereaders’ eye movements during shared storybook reading are language-mediated but not predictive.
Visual Cognition,
Vol. 26,
Issue. 5,
p.
351.
Larsen-Freeman, Diane
2018.
Learning Language through Task Repetition.
Vol. 11,
Issue. ,
p.
311.
Peter, Michelle S.
and
Rowland, Caroline F.
2019.
Aligning Developmental and Processing Accounts of Implicit and Statistical Learning.
Topics in Cognitive Science,
Vol. 11,
Issue. 3,
p.
555.
Gavard, Elisa
and
Ziegler, Johannes C.
2022.
The Effects of Semantic and Syntactic Prediction on Reading Aloud.
Experimental Psychology,
Vol. 69,
Issue. 6,
p.
308.
Gavard, Elisa
and
Ziegler, Johannes C.
2024.
Semantic and Syntactic Predictions in Reading Aloud: Are Good Predictors Good Statistical Learners?.
Journal of Cognition,
Vol. 7,
Issue. 1,
Target article
An integrated theory of language production and comprehension
Related commentaries (32)
A developmental perspective on the integration of language production and comprehension
An ecological alternative to a “sad response”: Public language use transcends the boundaries of the skin
Are forward models enough to explain self-monitoring? Insights from patients and eye movements
Cascading and feedback in interactive models of production: A reflection of forward modeling?
Communicative intentions can modulate the linguistic perception-action link
Does what you hear predict what you will do and say?
Evidence for, and predictions from, forward modeling in language production
Forward modelling requires intention recognition and non-impoverished predictions
How do forward models work? And why would you want them?
Inner speech as a forward model?
Integrate, yes, but what and how? A computational approach of sensorimotor fusion in speech
Intentional strategies that make co-actors more predictable: The case of signaling
Intermediate representations exclude embodiment
Is there any evidence for forward modeling in language production?
It ain't what you do (it's the way that you do it)
Memory and cognitive control in an integrated theory of language processing
Prediction in processing is a by-product of language learning
Prediction is no panacea: The key to language is in the unexpected
Prediction plays a key role in language development as well as processing
Predictive coding? Yes, but from what source?
Preparing to be punched: Prediction may not always require inference of intentions
Seeking predictions from a predictive framework
The complexity-cost factor in bilingualism
The neurobiology of receptive-expressive language interdependence
The poor helping the rich: How can incomplete representations monitor complete ones?
The role of action in verbal communication and shared reality
Toward a unified account of comprehension and production in language development
Towards a complete multiple-mechanism account of predictive language processing
What does it mean to predict one's own utterances?
What is the context of prediction?
When to simulate and when to associate? Accounting for inter-talker variability in the speech signal
“Well, that's one way”: Interactivity in parsing and production
Author response
Forward models and their implications for production, comprehension, and dialogue