No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Negative priors and inferences from absence of evidence in cognitive and linguistic archaeology: Epistemically sound and scientifically strategic
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 14 January 2025
Abstract
The article provides an important warning but its general conclusions should be nuanced: (i) When there is no evidence for it, we should depart from the hypothesis that a species lacks a particular cognitive capacity, and (ii) inferences from absence of evidence can be epistemically sound and scientifically strategic in cognitive and linguistic archaeology.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Beckers, G. J. L, Bolhuis, J. J., Okanoya, K., & Berwick, R. C. (2012). Birdsong neurolinguistics: Songbird context-free grammar claim is premature. NeuroReport, 23(3), 139–145. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32834f1765CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyno, G., & Demir, S. (2022). Plant-mycorrhiza communication and mycorrhizae in inter-plant communication. Symbiosis, 86, 155–168. doi: 10.1007/s13199-022-00837-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camps, M., & Uriagereka, J. (2006). The Gordian knot of linguistic fossils. In Rosselló, J. & Martín, J. (Eds.), The biolinguistic turn. Issues on language and biology (pp. 34–65). PPU.Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T., & Hauser, M. D. (2004). Computational constraints on syntactic processing in a nonhuman primate. Science, 303, 377–380. doi: 10.1126/science.108940CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallistel, C. R. (1998). Symbolic processes in the brain: The case of insect navigation. In Scarborough, D. & Sternberg, S. (Eds.), Invitation to cognitive science (Vol. 4, pp. 1–51). MIT Press. doi: 10.7551/mitpress/3967.003.0004Google Scholar
Gallistel, C. R. (2011). Prelinguistic thought. Language Learning and Development, 7(4), 253–262. doi: 10.1080/15475441.2011.578548CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298(5598), 1569–1579. doi: 10.1126/science.298.5598.1569CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howson, C., & Urbach, P. (1989). Scientific reasoning: The Bayesian approach. Open Court.Google Scholar
Oaksford, M., & Hahn, U. (2004). A Bayesian approach to the argument from ignorance. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58(2), 75–85. doi: 10.1037/h0085798CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephens, C. (2011). A Bayesian approach to absent evidence reasoning. Informal Logic, 31(1), 56–65. doi: 10.22329/il.v31i1.2967CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, R., & Darvill, T. (2022). What haven't we found? Recognising the value of negative evidence in archaeology. Antiquity, 96(388), 955–967. doi: 10.15184/aqy.2022.69CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallach, E. (2019). Inference from absence: The case of archaeology. Palgrave Communications, 5, 94. doi: 10.1057/s41599-019-0307-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Target article
Reconsidering the link between past material culture and cognition in light of contemporary hunter–gatherer material use
Related commentaries (21)
Advancing paleoanthropology beyond default nulls
All that glitters is not gold: The false-symbol problem in archaeology
Animal artefacts challenge archaeological standards for tracing human symbolic cognition
Archaeology retains a central role for studying the behavioral and cognitive evolution of our species and genus
Are we jingling modern hunter-gatherers and early Homo sapiens?
Behavioural modernity is dead: Long live behavioural modernity
Beyond the binary: Inferential challenges and solutions in cognitive archaeology
Cultural innovation is not only a product of cognition but also of cultural context
Don't ignore cognitive evolution during the three million years that preceded the archaeological record of material culture!
Inferences from absences
Material culture both reflects and causes human cognitive evolution
Negative priors and inferences from absence of evidence in cognitive and linguistic archaeology: Epistemically sound and scientifically strategic
Not just symbolism: Technologies may also have a less than direct connection with cognition
Perishable material choice indicates symbolic and representational capacities
Proposing the DN(C)-model of material evidence for well-calibrated claims about past cultures
Revising the null model in language evolution research
Shared intentionality may have been favored by persistence hunting in Homo erectus
Sports, team games, and physical skill competitions as an important source of symbolic material culture with low preservation probability
The cognitive and evolutionary science of behavioural modernity goes beyond material chronology
The Mbuti people still reproduce a 75,000 years old recursive pattern
What would be pre-modern human cognition?
Author response
Hominin cognition: The null hypothesis