No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Shared intentionality may have been favored by persistence hunting in Homo erectus
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 14 January 2025
Abstract
Shared intentionality is the derived hominin motivation and skills to align mental states. Research on the role of interdependence in the phylogeny of shared intentionality has only considered the archeological record of Homo heidelbergensis. But ethnographic and fossil data must be considered, too. Doing so suggests that shared intentionality may have been favored in Homo erectus to support persistence hunting.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Alexander, R. M. (1991). Energy-saving mechanisms in walking and running. Journal of Experimental Biology, 160(1), 55–69. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.160.1.55CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bramble, D. M., & Lieberman, D. E. (2004). Endurance running and the evolution of Homo. Nature, 432(7015), 345–352. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature03052CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caravaggi, P., Pataky, T., Günther, M., Savage, R., & Crompton, R. (2010). Dynamics of longitudinal arch support in relation to walking speed: Contribution of the plantar aponeurosis. Journal of Anatomy, 217(3), 254–261. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7580.2010.01261.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carrier, D. R. (1984) The energetic paradox of human running and hominid evolution. Current Anthropology 25(4), 483–495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatala, K. G., Roach, N. T., Ostrofsky, K. R., Wunderlich, R. E., Dingwall, H. L., Villmoare, B. A., … Richmond, B. G. (2016). Footprints reveal direct evidence of group behavior and locomotion in Homo erectus. Scientific Reports, 6(1), 28766. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep28766CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holowka, N. B., & Lieberman, D. E. (2018). Rethinking the evolution of the human foot: Insights from experimental research. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 221(Pt 17), jeb174425. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.174425CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holowka, N. B., Richards, A., Sibson, B. E., & Lieberman, D. E. (2021). The human foot functions like a spring of adjustable stiffness during running. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 224(Pt 1), jeb219667. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.219667Google Scholar
Hrdy, S. B. (2009). Mothers and others: The evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Huang, C. K., Kitaoka, H. B., An, K. N., & Chao, E. Y. (1993). Biomechanical evaluation of longitudinal arch stability. Foot & Ankle, 14(6), 353–357. https://doi.org/10.1177/107110079301400609CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ker, R. F., Bennett, M. B., Bibby, S. R., Kester, R. C., & Alexander, R. M. (1987). The spring in the arch of the human foot. Nature, 325(6100), 147–149. doi: https://doi.org/10.1038/325147a0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liebenberg, L. (1990). The art of tracking: The origin of science. David Phillip Publishers.Google Scholar
Liebenberg, L. (2006) Persistence hunting by modern hunter-gatherers. Current Anthropology 47(6), 1017–1026.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, D. E., Bramble, D. M., Raichlen, D. A., & Shea, J. J. (2007). The evolution of endurance running and the tyranny of ethnography: A reply to Pickering and Bunn (2007). Journal of Human Evolution, 53(4), 439–442. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2007.07.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, D. E., Kistner, T. M., Richard, D., Lee, I.-M., & Baggish, A. L. (2021). The active grandparent hypothesis: Physical activity and the evolution of extended human healthspans and lifespans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(50), e2107621118. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2107621118CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, D. E., Mahaffey, M., Cubesare Quimare, S., Holowka, N. B., Wallace, I. J., & Baggish, A. L. (2020). Running in Tarahumara (Rarámuri) culture: Persistence hunting, footracing, dancing, work, and the fallacy of the athletic savage. Current Anthropology, 61(3), 356–379. https://doi.org/10.1086/708810CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morin, E., & Winterhalder, B. (2024). Ethnography and ethnohistory support the efficiency of hunting through endurance running in humans. Nature Human Behaviour, 8, 1065–1075. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-024-01876-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (1999). Grandmothering and the evolution of Homo erectus. Journal of Human Evolution, 36(5), 461–485. https://doi.org/10.1006/jhev.1998.0285CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pontzer, H. (2017). Economy and endurance in human evolution. Current Biology, 27(12), R613–R621. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.031CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, G. (2005). Cooperation through interdependence. Animal Behaviour, 70(4), 901–908. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.02.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skyrms, B. (2004). The stag hunt and the evolution of social structure. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stearne, S. M., McDonald, K. A., Alderson, J. A., North, I., Oxnard, C. E., & Rubenson, J. (2016). The foot's arch and the energetics of human locomotion. Scientific Reports, 6(1), 19403. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep19403CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tindale, N. B. (1974). Aboriginal tribes of Australia: Their terrain, environmental controls, distribution, limits, and proper names. Australian National University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2014). A natural history of human thinking. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2022). The evolution of agency: Behavioral organization from lizards to humans. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Hare, B., Lehmann, H., & Call, J. (2007) Reliance on head versus eyes in the gaze following of great apes and human infants: The cooperative eye hypothesis. Journal of Human Evolution 52(3), 314–320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., Melis, A. P., Tennie, C., Wyman, E., & Herrmann, E. (2012) Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation: The interdependence hypothesis. Current Anthropology 53(6), 673–692.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vasil, J. (2023). A new look at young children's referential informativeness. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 18(3), 624–648. https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916221112072CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Venkadesan, M., Yawar, A., Eng, C. M., Dias, M. A., Singh, D. K., Tommasini, S. M., … Mandre, S. (2020). Stiffness of the human foot and evolution of the transverse arch. Nature, 579(7797), 97–100. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-020-2053-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Target article
Reconsidering the link between past material culture and cognition in light of contemporary hunter–gatherer material use
Related commentaries (21)
Advancing paleoanthropology beyond default nulls
All that glitters is not gold: The false-symbol problem in archaeology
Animal artefacts challenge archaeological standards for tracing human symbolic cognition
Archaeology retains a central role for studying the behavioral and cognitive evolution of our species and genus
Are we jingling modern hunter-gatherers and early Homo sapiens?
Behavioural modernity is dead: Long live behavioural modernity
Beyond the binary: Inferential challenges and solutions in cognitive archaeology
Cultural innovation is not only a product of cognition but also of cultural context
Don't ignore cognitive evolution during the three million years that preceded the archaeological record of material culture!
Inferences from absences
Material culture both reflects and causes human cognitive evolution
Negative priors and inferences from absence of evidence in cognitive and linguistic archaeology: Epistemically sound and scientifically strategic
Not just symbolism: Technologies may also have a less than direct connection with cognition
Perishable material choice indicates symbolic and representational capacities
Proposing the DN(C)-model of material evidence for well-calibrated claims about past cultures
Revising the null model in language evolution research
Shared intentionality may have been favored by persistence hunting in Homo erectus
Sports, team games, and physical skill competitions as an important source of symbolic material culture with low preservation probability
The cognitive and evolutionary science of behavioural modernity goes beyond material chronology
The Mbuti people still reproduce a 75,000 years old recursive pattern
What would be pre-modern human cognition?
Author response
Hominin cognition: The null hypothesis