Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T06:05:37.341Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

It's a matter of (executive) load: Separation as a load-dependent resetting procedure

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 February 2021

Giorgia Ponsi
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, 00185Rome, [email protected]://dippsi.psi.uniroma1.it/users/[email protected]://dippsi.psi.uniroma1.it/users/era-vanessa IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, 00142Rome, Italy
Vanessa Era
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, 00185Rome, [email protected]://dippsi.psi.uniroma1.it/users/[email protected]://dippsi.psi.uniroma1.it/users/era-vanessa IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, 00142Rome, Italy
Chiara Fini
Affiliation:
Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, 00185Rome, [email protected]://web.uniroma1.it/dip42/dip42/node/5757/fini-chiara
Ilenia Falcinelli
Affiliation:
ISTC, Istituto di Scienze e Tecnologie della Cognizione – CNR, 00185Rome, Italy. [email protected]

Abstract

Lee and Schwarz made considerable theoretical advances in the psychology of cleansing by proposing that cleaning actions might serve as separation procedures between two psychological entities. Here, we propose that the effectiveness of the separation process may be modulated by the available amount of executive resources, and that separation may operate as a load-dependent resetting procedure.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

Shared first authorship.

References

Abramovitch, A., Dar, R., Hermesh, H., & Schweiger, A. (2012). Comparative neuropsychology of adult obsessive-compulsive disorder and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Implications for a novel executive overload model of OCD. Journal of Neuropsychology, 6(2), 161191. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1748-6653.2011.02021.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Obsessive compulsive and related disorders. In Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). American Psychiatric Association. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.dsm05.Google Scholar
Armstrong, T., Zald, D. H., & Olatunji, B. O. (2011). Attentional control in OCD and GAD: Specificity and associations with core cognitive symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49(11), 756762. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2011.08.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bucci, P., Mucci, A., Volpe, U., Merlotti, E., Galderisi, S., & Maj, M. (2004). Executive hypercontrol in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Electrophysiological and neuropsychological indices. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115(6), 13401348. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2003.12.031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burguière, E., Monteiro, P., Mallet, L., Feng, G., & Graybiel, A. M. (2015). Striatal circuits, habits, and implications for obsessive-compulsive disorder. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 30, 5965. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2014.08.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chum, M., Bekkering, H., Dodd, M. D., & Pratt, J. (2007). Motor and visual codes interact to facilitate visuospatial memory performance. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14(6), 11891193. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, N., Mor, N., & Henik, A. (2015). Linking executive control and emotional response: A training procedure to reduce rumination. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(1), 1525. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702614530114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curci, A., Lanciano, T., Soleti, E., & Rimé, B. (2013). Negative emotional experiences arouse rumination and affect working memory capacity. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 13(5), 867880. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daches, S., Mor, N., Winquist, J., & Gilboa-Schechtman, E. (2010). Brooding and attentional control in processing self-encoded information: Evidence from a modified Garner task. Cognition and Emotion, 24(5), 876885. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930902954825.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darke, S. (1988). Anxiety and working memory capacity. Cognition and Emotion, 2(2), 145154. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699938808408071.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Los Reyes, A., Aldao, A., Kundey, S. M. A., Lee, B. G., & Molina, S. (2012). Compromised decision making and the effects of manipulating physical states on human judgments. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 68(1), 17. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20851.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 7(2), 336353. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forster, S., Elizalde, A. O. N., Castle, E., & Bishop, S. J. (2015). Unraveling the anxious mind: Anxiety, worry, and frontal engagement in sustained attention versus off-task processing. Cerebral Cortex, 25(3), 609618. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gehring, W. J., Himle, J., & Nisenson, L. G. (2000). Action-monitoring dysfunction in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychological Science, 11(1), 16. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillan, C. M., Apergis-Schoute, A. M., Morein-Zamir, S., Urcelay, G. P., Sule, A., Fineberg, N. A., … Robbins, T. W. (2015). Functional neuroimaging of avoidance habits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 172(3), 284293. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14040525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillan, C. M., Papmeyer, M., Morein-Zamir, S., Sahakian, B. J., Fineberg, N. A., Robbins, T. W., & De Wit, S. (2011). Disruption in the balance between goal-directed behavior and habit learning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(7), 718726. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10071062.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillan, C. M., & Robbins, T. W. (2014). Goal-directed learning and obsessive – compulsive disorder. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, Biological Sciences, 369(1655), 111. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graybiel, A. M., & Rauch, S. L. (2000). Toward a neurobiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuron, 28, 343347. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(00)00113-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haith, A. M., & Krakauer, J. W. (2018). The multiple effects of practice: Skill, habit and reduced cognitive load. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 20, 196201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.01.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hajcak, G., & Simons, R. F. (2002). Error-related brain activity in obsessive-compulsive undergraduates. Psychiatry Research, 110(1), 6372. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-1781(02)00034-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauser, T. U., Moutoussis, M., Iannaccone, R., Brem, S., Walitza, S., Drechsler, R., … Dolan, R. J. (2017). Increased decision thresholds enhance information gathering performance in juvenile obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). PLoS Computational Biology, 13(4), 119. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johansen, T., & Dittrich, W. H. (2013). Cognitive performance in a subclinical obsessive-compulsive sample 1: Cognitive functions. Psychiatry Journal, 110. https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/565191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joormann, J. (2006). Differential effects of rumination and dysphoria on the inhibition of irrelevant emotional material: Evidence from a negative priming task. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 30(2), 149160. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-006-9035-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joormann, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2008). Updating the contents of working memory in depression: Interference from irrelevant negative material. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117(1), 182192. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.117.1.182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joormann, J., Levens, S. M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2011). Sticky thoughts: Depression and rumination are associated with difficulties manipulating emotional material in working memory. Psychological Science, 22(8), 979983. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611415539.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalanthroff, E., Aslan, C., & Dar, R. (2017). Washing away your sins will set your mind free: physical cleansing modulates the effect of threatened morality on executive control. Cognition and Emotion, 31(1), 185192. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2015.1086313CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lavie, N. (2010). Attention, distraction, and cognitive control under load. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(3), 143148. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721410370295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milliery, M., Bouvard, M., Aupetit, J., & Cottraux, J. (2000). Sustained attention in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder: A controlled study. Psychiatry Research, 96(3), 199209. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-1781(00)00210-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moran, T. P. (2016). Anxiety and working memory capacity: A meta-analysis and narrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 142(8), 831864. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000051.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, A. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(5), 400424. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reuven, O., Liberman, N., & Dar, R. (2014). The effect of physical cleaning on threatened morality in individuals with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Clinical Psychological Science, 2(2), 224229. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702613485565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaefer, S., Lövdén, M., Wieckhorst, B., & Lindenberger, U. (2010). Cognitive performance is improved while walking: Differences in cognitive-sensorimotor couplings between children and young adults. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7(3), 371389. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405620802535666.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmeichel, B. J. (2007). Attention control, memory updating, and emotion regulation temporarily reduce the capacity for executive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136(2), 241255. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.136.2.241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shields, G. S., Moons, W. G., Tewell, C. A., & Yonelineas, A. P. (2016). Anxiety not anger impairs executive functioning. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 16(6), 792797. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.antiviral.2015.06.014.Chronic.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skulmowski, A., & Rey, G. D. (2017). Bodily effort enhances learning and metacognition: Investigating the relation between physical effort and cognition using dual-process models of embodiment. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 13(1), 310. https://doi.org/10.5709/acp-0202-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Starcevic, V., Berle, D., Brakoulias, V., Sammut, P., Moses, K., Milicevic, D., & Hannan, A. (2011). Functions of compulsions in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 45(6), 449457. https://doi.org/10.3109/00048674.2011.567243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Randenborgh, A., Hüffmeier, J., LeMoult, J., & Joormann, J. (2010). Letting go of unmet goals: Does self-focused rumination impair goal disengagement? Motivation and Emotion, 34(4), 325332. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-010-9190-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner Cook, S., Yip, T. K., & & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2012). Gestures, but not meaningless movements, lighten working memory load when explaining math. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27(4), 594610. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.567074.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watkins, E., & Brown, R. G. (2002). Rumination and executive function in depression: An experimental study. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 72(3), 400402. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.72.3.400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitmer, A. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2013). An attentional scope model of rumination. Psychological Bulletin, 139(5), 10361061. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychometric Bulletin & Review, 9(4), 625636. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolff, N., Chmielewski, W., Buse, J., Roessner, V., & Beste, C. (2019). Paradoxical response inhibition advantages in adolescent obsessive compulsive disorder result from the interplay of automatic and controlled processes. NeuroImage: Clinical, 23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101893.Google ScholarPubMed
Yang, Y., Cao, S., Shields, G. S., Teng, Z., & Liu, Y. (2017). The relationships between rumination and core executive functions: A meta-analysis. Depression and Anxiety, 34(1), 3750. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22539.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed