Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T20:07:00.243Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Is a wandering mind a novelty-seeking mind? The curious case of incubation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 May 2024

Myrthe Faber*
Affiliation:
Department of Cognitive Science and Artificial Intelligence, Tilburg School of Humanities and Digital Sciences, Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands [email protected] Donders Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
Alwin de Rooij
Affiliation:
Department of Communication and Cognition, Tilburg School of Humanities and Digital Sciences, Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands [email protected] Centre of Applied Research for Art, Design and Technology, Avans University of Applied Sciences, ‘s-Hertogenbosch, The Netherlands
*
*Corresponding author.

Abstract

The Novelty-Seeking Model can explain incubation's effect on creativity by assuming an adaptive decision threshold. During an impasse, the threshold for novelty becomes too high and biased to previous neural activity, hindering progress. Incubation “resets” this threshold through attentional decoupling, allowing for spontaneous ideas to emerge from subsequent mind wandering or other activities that attract attention, facilitating progress.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Ivancovsky et al. propose that curiosity and creativity “are manifestations of a unified process that underlies novelty-seeking” (p. 45). They suggest that curiosity and creativity reinforce each other such that the more curious and creative a person is, the more attuned they become to novel stimuli. This heightened sensitivity to novelty, the authors argue, is a result of being in a novelty-seeking mode, which raises neural activity associated with (spontaneous) mental events above a certain decision threshold. This process might explain “how certain stimulation can attract our attention and be perceived as novel and interesting” (p. 45). We propose how this idea can be expanded to the state of incubation, in which people tend to refrain from deliberate thoughts about a task or problem and instead do something else and/or let their minds wander.

Incubation is an interesting state in terms of the Novelty-Seeking Model. Creative thinking requires people to flexibly shift along a continuum of exploitation (which is linked to goal-directed, convergent thinking, specific curiosity, and deprivation) to exploration (which is linked to open-ended, divergent thinking, diverse curiosity, and interest) to arrive at outcomes that are useful and original. When attention shifts from being focused to becoming defocused, irrelevant information is allowed to “leak in” (Ivancovsky, Baror, & Bar). Incubation, however, appears to be even further along this continuum: attention becomes decoupled from the task at hand, and often refocused on something else (Ritter & Dijksterhuis, Reference Ritter and Dijksterhuis2014), such as self-generated cognition (e.g., mind wandering; Faber, Krasich, Bixler, Brockmole, & D'Mello, Reference Faber, Krasich, Bixler, Brockmole and D'Mello2020). We argue that in addition to focused and defocused attention, decoupled or refocused attention might play an important role in novelty-seeking that could explain the often-reported positive relationship between decoupled processes such as incubation and mind wandering on the one hand, and creativity on the other.

Indeed, many famous anecdotes support this strategy's effectiveness for overcoming impasse (Wallas, Reference Wallas1926). Mathematician Poincaré, for example, had one of his most important theoretical breakthroughs during a geological excursion, because, as he claimed, “the changes of travel made me forget my mathematical work” (Poincaré, Reference Poincaré2022, p. 387). When an impasse occurs, the threshold for what is novel and interesting has become too high and too biased to previous neural activity to make progress (Beda & Smith, Reference Beda and Smith2022; Gauselmann, Frings, Schmidt, & Tempel, Reference Gauselmann, Frings, Schmidt and Tempel2023). In line with the Novelty-Seeking Model, during incubation, this threshold might be lowered adaptively, allowing for ideas that arise spontaneously to attract attention. Lowering the threshold might enhance the salience of spontaneous ideas. This increases the probability that one can make progress after an impasse, but also comes at the cost of accuracy. In turn, when progress is being made the threshold is raised to restore accuracy accordingly, through more deliberate modes of creative thought.

The idea of such an adaptive threshold finds its origins in adaptive gain theory, which may offer further insight into how incubation supports creativity. This theory postulates that the dynamic shift between focus and defocus in response to changing environmental demands is regulated by the locus coeruleus norepinephrine system (Aston-Jones & Cohen, Reference Aston-Jones and Cohen2005). Increases in tonic norepinephrine (NE) – the slow release of NE – boosts overall signal strength in neural circuitry, enabling sensitivity to novel and task-irrelevant information. Phasic NE – fast bursts of NE release – enhances responses to salient events (Mittner, Hawkins, Boekel, & Forstmann, Reference Mittner, Hawkins, Boekel and Forstmann2016). In view of the Novelty-Seeking Model, moderately heightened tonic NE may help facilitate creativity through defocusing while maintaining a suitable level of alertness (de Rooij, Vromans, & Dekker, Reference de Rooij, Vromans and Dekker2018), whereas phasic responses facilitate focus in response to salience (Salvi, Simoncini, Grafman, & Beeman, Reference Salvi, Simoncini, Grafman and Beeman2020), which can help to select an idea as a basis for further creative thinking (de Rooij, Reference de Rooij2023; Simonton, Reference Simonton2023).

Attentional decoupling is facilitated by an upshift in tonic NE, which enhances neural gain and functional connectivity within and between networks, allowing for the brain to go into a more exploratory mode. In addition, when tonic NE levels become high, it reduces phasic responses to the extent that attentional decoupling occurs (Mittner et al., Reference Mittner, Hawkins, Boekel and Forstmann2016). The upshift in tonic NE in response to an impasse might therefore facilitate the “reset” needed to reduce the bias to previous neural activity that has led to the impasse. We suggest that this upshift in tonic NE also effectively lowers the threshold for what is novel and interesting. The behaviors that a person subsequently engages in during incubation, whether it is letting the mind wander or refocusing on something else, then may promote the attentional shift toward spontaneous ideas, which are more likely to become salient due to the lowered threshold.

Behaviorally, this resonates with our own work, in which we have recently observed that there is indeed a bias such that ideas that arise spontaneously are perceived as more novel than those that are deliberate, even if objectively, there is no difference (de Rooij, Atef, & Faber, Reference de Rooij, Atef and Faber2023). We would therefore like to argue that the activation threshold might be dynamic: when one is stuck on a problem and searching for a specific idea, the threshold might be higher, allowing for selectivity. Incubation might provide a “reset” of the threshold, allowing for (potentially not particularly novel or interesting) ideas to attract attention such that progress can be made. With this proposed theoretical expansion of the Novelty-Seeking Model, we aim to capture an important feature of creative idea generation, namely how the dynamic regulation of attention to spontaneous and deliberate thoughts together enables creativity.

Financial support

This work was supported by the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) grant number VI.Veni.191G.001 award to Myrthe Faber.

Competing interest

None.

References

Aston-Jones, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2005). An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: Adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 403450. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beda, Z., & Smith, S. M. (2022). Unfixate your creative mind: Forgetting fixation and its applications. Translational Issues in Psychological Science, 8(1), 6678. https://doi.org/10.1037/tps0000290CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Rooij, A. (2023). Inner speaking and uncertainty during idea generation. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 57(3), 376396. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.584CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Rooij, A., Atef, A., & Faber, M. (2023, November 7). A wandering mind is not always a creative mind: How thought dynamics explain the relationship between mind wandering and creativity. Retrieved from https://osf.io/preprints/psyarxiv/w45hfCrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Rooij, A., Vromans, R. D., & Dekker, M. (2018). Noradrenergic modulation of creativity: Evidence from pupillometry. Creativity Research Journal, 30(4), 339351. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2018.1530533Google Scholar
Faber, M., Krasich, K., Bixler, R. E., Brockmole, J. R., & D'Mello, S. K. (2020). The eye–mind wandering link: Identifying gaze indices of mind wandering across tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 46(10), 1201. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000743Google ScholarPubMed
Gauselmann, P., Frings, C., Schmidt, M., & Tempel, T. (2023). Protecting against mental impasses: Evidence of selective retrieval mitigating the impact of fixation in creative problem solving. Cognition, 239, 105547. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2023.105547CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mittner, M., Hawkins, G. E., Boekel, W., & Forstmann, B. U. (2016). A neural model of mind wandering. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(8), 570578. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.06.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poincaré, H. (2022). The foundations of science: Science and hypothesis, the value of science, science and method. Digicat. (Original work published 1904).Google Scholar
Ritter, S. M., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2014). Creativity – the unconscious foundations of the incubation period. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 000215. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00215CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salvi, C., Simoncini, C., Grafman, J., & Beeman, M. (2020). Oculometric signature of switch into awareness? Pupil size predicts sudden insight whereas microsaccades predict problem-solving via analysis. NeuroImage, 217, 116933. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116933CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simonton, D. K. (2023). The blind-variation and selective-retention theory of creativity: Recent developments and current status of BVSR. Creativity Research Journal, 35(3), 304323. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2022.2059919CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallas, G. (1926). The art of thought. Harcourt, Brace.Google Scholar