No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Innateness of magnitude perception? Skill can be acquired and mastered at all ages
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 27 July 2017
Abstract
We agree with Leibovich et al.'s argument that the number sense theory should be re-evaluated. However, we argue that highly efficient skills (i.e., fluent and highly accurate, “automatic,” performance) can be acquired and mastered at all ages. Hence, evidence for primacy or fluency in perceiving continuous magnitudes is insufficient for supporting strong conclusions about the innateness of this aptitude.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017
References
De Boysson-Bardies, B. (1993) Ontogeny of language-specific syllabic productions. In: Developmental neurocognition: Speech and face processing in the first year of life, ed. De Boysson-Bardies, B., de Schonen, S., Jusczyk, P., MacNeilage, P. & Morton, J., pp. 353–63. Kluwer.Google Scholar
Eimas, P. D. (1975) Auditory and phonetic coding of the cues for speech: Discrimination of the [rl] distinction by young infants. Perception & Psychophysics
18(5):341–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, E. R., Jusczyk, P. & Vigorito, J. (1971) Speech perception in infants. Science
171(3968):303–306.Google Scholar
Girelli, L., Lucangeli, D. & Butterworth, B. (2000) The development of automaticity in accessing number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
76:104–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Held, R. & Hein, A. (1963) Movement-produced stimulation in the development of visually guided behavior. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology
56(5):872–76.Google Scholar
Jarjoura, W. & Karni, A. (2016) A novel tactile Braille–Stroop test (TBSt). British Journal of Visual Impairment
34(1):72–82.Google Scholar
Karni, A. (1996) The acquisition of perceptual and motor skills: A memory system in the adult human cortex. Cognitive Brain Research
5(1):39–48.Google Scholar
Karni, A. & Bertini, G. (1997) Learning perceptual skills: Behavioral probes into adult cortical plasticity. Current Opinion in Neurobiology
7(4):530–35.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K. (2010) Brain mechanisms in early language acquisition. Neuron
67(5):713–27.Google Scholar
Lasky, R. E., Syrdal-Lasky, A. & Klein, R. E. (1975) VOT discrimination by four to six and a half month old infants from Spanish environments. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
20(2):215–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Logan, G. D. (1997) The automaticity of academic life: Unconscious applications of an implicit theory. In: The automaticity of everyday life: Advances in social cognition, vol. 10, ed. Wyer, R. S. Jr., pp. 157–79. Psychology Press, Taylor and Francis.Google Scholar
MacLeod, C. M. (1991) Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin
109:163–203.Google Scholar
Poldrack, R. A. & Logan, G. D. (1998) What is the mechanism for fluency in successive recognition?
Acta Psychologica
98(2):167–81.Google Scholar
Rubinsten, O., Henik, A., Berger, A. & Shahar-Shalev, S. (2002) The development of internal representations of magnitude and their association with Arabic numerals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
81:74–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, L. B., Jones, S. S. & Landau, B. (1996) Naming in young children: A dumb attentional mechanism?
Cognition
60(2):143–71.Google Scholar
Starr, A. & Brannon, E. M. (2015) Evidence against continuous variables driving numerical discrimination in infancy. Frontiers in Psychology
6:923.Google Scholar
Stroop, J. R. (1935) Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology
18:643–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treisman, A. M. & Gelade, G. (1980) A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology
12(1): 97–136.Google Scholar
Tzelgov, J., Yehene, V., Kotler, L. & Alon, A. (2000) Automatic comparisons of artificial digits never compared: Learning linear ordering relations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition
26(1):1–18.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F. & Lalonde, C. E. (1988) Cross-language speech perception: Initial capabilities and developmental change. Developmental Psychology
24(5):672–83.Google Scholar
Target article
From “sense of number” to “sense of magnitude”: The role of continuous magnitudes in numerical cognition
Related commentaries (29)
A “sense of magnitude” requires a new alternative for learning numerical symbols
Approximate number sense theory or approximate theory of magnitude?
Commentary on Leibovich et al.: What next?
Computational foundations of the visual number sense
Controlling for continuous variables is not futile: What we can learn about number representation despite imperfect control
Direct and rapid encoding of numerosity in the visual stream
Evidence for a number sense
From continuous magnitudes to symbolic numbers: The centrality of ratio
How not to develop a sense of number
Infants discriminate number: Evidence against the prerequisite of visual object individuation and the primacy of continuous magnitude
Infants, animals, and the origins of number
Innateness of magnitude perception? Skill can be acquired and mastered at all ages
Is the ANS linked to mathematics performance?
Magnitude rather than number: More evidence needed
Magnitude, numerosity, and development of number: Implications for mathematics disabilities
Multitudes are adaptable magnitudes in the estimation of number
Number faculty is alive and kicking: On number discriminations and number neurons
Numerical intuitions in infancy: Give credit where credit is due
Numerical magnitude evaluation as a foundation for decision making
Perceiving numerosity from birth
Right idea, wrong magnitude system
Selecting the model that best fits the data
The contribution of fish studies to the “number sense” debate
The contributions of non-numeric dimensions to number encoding, representations, and decision-making factors
The evolvement of discrete representations from continuous stimulus properties: A possible overarching principle of cognition
The number sense is neither last resort nor of primary import
What is a number? The interplay between number and continuous magnitudes
What is the precise role of cognitive control in the development of a sense of number?
Why try saving the ANS? An alternative proposal
Author response
Toward an integrative approach to numerical cognition