We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
To save content items to your account,
please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies.
If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account.
Find out more about saving content to .
To save content items to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected]
is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings
on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part
of your Kindle email address below.
Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations.
‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi.
‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
It is half a century since Maxwell investigated the chromatic relations of the spectral colours and exhibited the results on Newton's diagram. The curve “forms two sides of a triangle with doubtful fragments of the third side. Now, if three colours in Newton's diagram lie in a straight line, the middle one is a compound of the two others. Hence all the colours of the spectrum may be compounded of those which lie at the angles of this triangle. These correspond to the following—scarlet, wave-length (in Fraunhofer's measure), 2328; green, wave-length, 1914; blue, wave-length, 1717. All the other colours of the spectrum may be produced by combinations of these; and since all natural colours are compounded of the colours of the spectrum, they may be compounded of these three primary colours. I [Maxwell] have strong reason to believe that these are the three primary colours corresponding to three modes of sensation in the organ of vision, on which the whole system of colour, as seen by the normal eye, depends.”
Later observations, such as those of König and Dieterici, have in the main confirmed Maxwell's conclusions. The green corner is indeed more rounded off than he supposed. It is with regard to the “doubtful fragments of the third side” that I have something to say.
My subject is shadows, in the literal sense of the word—shadows thrown by light, and shadows thrown by sound. The ordinary shadow thrown by light is familiar to all. When a fairly large obstacle is placed between a small source of light and a white screen, a well-defined shadow of the obstacle is thrown on the screen. This is a simple consequence of the approximately rectilinear path of light. Optical shadows may be thrown over great distances, if the light is of sufficient intensity: in a lunar eclipse the shadow of the earth is thrown on the moon: in a solar eclipse the shadow of the moon is thrown on the earth. Acoustic shadows, or shadows thrown by sound, are not so familiar to most people; they are less perfect than optical shadows, although their imperfections are usually over-estimated in ordinary observations. The ear is able to adjust its sensitiveness over a wide range, so that, unless an acoustic shadow is very complete, it often escapes detection by the unaided ear, the sound being sufficiently well heard in all positions. In certain circumstances, however, acoustic shadows may be very pronounced, and capable of easy observation.
The difference between acoustic and optical shadows was considered of so much importance by Newton, that it prevented him from accepting the wave theory of light.
The fact that by the aid of a spectroscope interferences may be observed with light originally white used to be regarded as a proof of the existence of periodicities in the original radiation; but it seems now to be generally agreed that these periodicities are due to the spectroscope. When a pulse strikes a grating, it is obvious that the periodicity and its variation in different directions are the work of the grating. The assertion that Newton's experiments prove the colours to be already existent in white light, is usually made in too unqualified a form.
When a prism, which has no periodicities of figure, is substituted for a grating, the modus operandi is much less obvious. This question has been especially considered by Schuster (Phil. Mag. XXXVII. p. 509, 1894; VII. p. 1, 1904), and quite recently Ames has given an “Elementary Discussion of the Action of a Prism upon White Light” (Astrophysical Journal, July 1905). The aim of the present note is merely to illustrate the matter further.
I commence by remarking that, so far as I see, there is nothing faulty or specially obscure in the traditional treatment founded upon the consideration of simple, and accordingly infinite, trains of waves. By Fourier's theorem any arbitrary disturbance may be thus compounded; and the method suffices to answer any question that may be raised, so long at least as we are content to take for granted the character of the dispersive medium—the relation of velocity to wave-length—without enquiring further as to its constitution.
The nature of the clue by which we are able to pronounce whether a sound of low pitch reaches us from the right or from the left was long a mystery, seeing that in such cases the difference of intensities at the two ears, used singly, is inappreciable. By some special laboratory experiments conducted about three years ago, I was able to show that the discrimination depends upon the phase-difference at the two ears, and that the sound is judged to be on that side where the phase is in advance. When the pitch is higher (much above g′) no distinct lateral effect accompanies a phase-difference, and the discrimination of right and left in ordinary hearing undoubtedly depends upon intensities. Commenting on these results, I remarked [p. 356]: “The conclusion, no longer to be resisted, that when a sound of low pitch reaches the two ears with approximately equal intensities, but with a phase-difference of a quarter of a period, we are able so easily to distinguish at which ear the phase is in advance, must have far-reaching consequences in the theory of audition. It seems no longer possible to hold that the vibratory character of sound terminates at the outer ends of the nerves along which the communication with the brain is established.
It is some thirty years ago since I executed a rather extensive series of experiments in order to ascertain more precisely what are the capabilities of the ears in estimating the direction of sounds. It appeared that when the alternative was between right and left, the discrimination could be made with certainty and without moving the head, even although the sounds were pure tones. Nor was any difficulty introduced by the requirement that the ears should be stopped at the moment when the sounds commenced.
On the other hand, if the question was whether a sound were situated in front or behind the observer, no pronouncement could be made in the case of pure tones. The impossibility of distinguishing front and back carries with it further confusions relating to cases where the sound may be obliquely situated. But with sounds of other character and notably with the speaking voice, front and back could often be distinguished. It is understood, of course, that the head was kept still. A slight rotation, bringing a pure tone (originally situated exactly in front or exactly behind) to the right or the left, gives the information that was previously lacking.
The discrimination between right and left is usually supposed to be explicable by the greater intensity of sensation experienced by the ear which lies nearer to the sound. When the pitch is pretty high, there is no doubt that this explanation is adequate.
The importance which these rings have acquired in recent years, owing to the researches of [Lummer], Michelson, and of Fabry and Perot, lends interest to the circumstances of their discovery. It seems to be usually supposed that Haidinger merely observed the rings, without a full appreciation of the mode of formation. Thus Mascart writes: “C'est par ce procédé, que Haidinger les a observées le premier avec une lame de mica, mais sans en donner la véritable explication.” A reference to the original papers will, I think, show that Haidinger, in spite of one or two slips, understood the character of the rings very well, and especially the distinction between them and the rings usually named after Newton and dependent upon a variable thickness in the thin plate.
In the first memoir (Pogg. Ann. LXXVII. p. 219, 1849) the bands formed by reflexion are especially discussed. A spirit-flame with salted wick, seen by reflexion at considerable obliquity in a mica plate, is traversed by approximately straight bands running perpendicularly to the plane of incidence. Talbot had observed phenomena in many respects similar…. But the yellow and black lines, observed by Talbot in thin blown glass, differ in character from the lines from mica, though both are dependent upon the interference of light. In the case of the glass the interference is due to the fact that the thickness of the glass is variable, and the lines are localised at the plate.
Dr Edridge-Green has introduced a method of classifying colour-vision by determining the number of separate parts or divisions in the spectrum within each of which the observer can perceive no colour difference. Movable screens are provided in the focal plane of the spectroscopic telescope, by which the part admitted to the eye is limited and the limits measured in terms of wave-length. Beginning at the extreme visible red, more and more of the spectrum is admitted until a change of colour (not merely of brightness) is just perceptible. This gives the first division. The second division starts from the place just determined, and is limited in the direction of shorter wave-length by the same condition. In this way the whole spectrum is divided into a number of contiguous divisions, or patches, which Dr Edridge-Green terms monochromatic. It will be observed that the delimitation of these patches includes an arbitrary element depending on the point from which the start is made—in this case the extreme red.
In the memoir, above reprinted from the Philosophical Magazine, I discussed the theories of Abbe and Helmholtz, and endeavoured to show their correlation. It appeared that the method of the former, while ingenious and capable of giving interesting results in certain directions, was inapplicable to many of the problems which it is necessary to attack. As an example of this, it may suffice to mention the case of a self-luminous object.
The work of Helmholtz, to which attention has recently been recalled by Mr. J. W. Gordon in a lively criticism (p. 381), was founded upon the processes already developed by Airy, Verdet, and others for the performance of the telescope. The theories both of Abbe and Helmholtz pointed to a tolerably definite limit to the powers of the Microscope, dependent, however, upon the wave-length of the light employed and upon the medium in which the object is imbedded. It appeared that two neighbours, whether constituting a single pair of points or forming part of an extended series of equidistant points, could not be properly distinguished if the distance were less than half the wave-length of the light employed. The importance of this conclusion, as imposing a limit upon our powers of direct observation, can hardly be overestimated; but there has been in some quarters a tendency to ascribe to it a more precise character than it can bear, or even to mistake its meaning altogether.
The boiling-point of argon being intermediate between those of nitrogen and oxygen, it may be expected that any operations of evaporation and condensation which increase the oxygen relatively to the nitrogen will at the same time increase the argon relatively to the nitrogen and diminish it relatively to the oxygen. In the experiments about to be detailed the gas analysed was that given off from liquid air, either freshly collected, or after standing (with evaporation) for some time—from a day to a week. The analyses were for oxygen and for argon, and were made upon different, though similar, samples. Thus after an analysis of a sample for oxygen by Hempel's method with copper and ammonia, 4 or 5 litres would be collected in a graduated holder, and then the first analysis confirmed on a third sample. In no case, except one to be specified later, was the quantity of gas withdrawn sufficient to disturb sensibly the composition. The liquid was held in Dewar's vessels, but the evolution of gas from below was always sufficient to keep the mass well mixed.
The examination for argon was made in a large test-tube inverted over alkali, into which the gas was fed intermittently from the holder. The nitrogen was gradually oxidized by the electric discharge from a Ruhmkorff coil in connexion with the public supply of alternating current, the proportion of oxygen being maintained suitably by additions of oxygen or hydrogen as might be required.