Are we really fearful apes, and are fearful apes more prosocial?
Grossmann proposes that humans are both more likely to express fear than other ape species and particularly sensitive to conspecifics' fear. This heightened fearfulness and fear detection, then, serves to elicit approach from caregivers, and increases one's own tendency to approach needy conspecifics. These tendencies ultimately ensure that humans receive, and deliver, the nurturance that uniquely characterizes our species. In what follows, we argue that these claims, although interesting and provocative, are currently unsupported by the broader literature in at least three ways. First, the assertion that humans are particularly fearful requires further evidence. Second, infants' sensitivity to fear (“fear bias”) may not actually reflect responsiveness to fear specifically, but rather to a broad range of distress signals. Finally, associations between both the expression and perception of fear and children's own prosocial behaviors are empirically tenuous. We detail our concerns below, ultimately concluding that the “fearful ape” may not be a useful description of the human species.
One aspect of Grossmann's hypothesis is that humans experience and express higher levels of fear than do other nonhuman primates. Support for this assertion comes from a single study (Herrmann, Hare, Cissewski, & Tomasello, Reference Herrmann, Hare, Cissewski and Tomasello2011) in which human infants demonstrated relatively more avoidance toward novel objects and people than did nonhuman ape species. These findings, however, suffer from some important confounds. First, there were significant age differences between the human (2.5 years) and nonhuman (6.4, 6.5, and 6.8 years) primate participants, a difference exacerbated by the faster rate of maturation in nonhumans. These age differences resulted in significant body size differences between participants and experimenters across species. That is, compared with 6.5-year-old nonhuman apes, 2.5-year-old children are much smaller than an adult experimenter, and might have displayed more fear because of the vulnerabilities this size difference entailed. Indeed, although this condition was not without interpretive difficulties, a control condition in which the experimenter was absent from the scene revealed no increased avoidance in humans. Thus, claiming that heightened fearfulness is an essential feature of the human species may be premature.
Another facet of Grossmann's hypothesis posits that infants are particularly sensitive to others' fear. We propose, instead, that the literature is more consistent with a general sensitivity to distress (Decety et al., Reference Decety, Bartal, Uzefovsky and Knafo-Noam2016; de Waal & Preston, Reference de Waal and Preston2017). While human infants do demonstrate sensitivity to fear (Leppänen & Nelson, Reference Leppänen and Nelson2012), they are also highly receptive to distress signals, including pain and sadness (Addabbo, Bolognini, & Turati, Reference Addabbo, Bolognini and Turati2021; Davidov et al., Reference Davidov, Paz, Roth-Hanania, Uzefovsky, Orlitsky, Mankuta and Zahn-Waxler2021; Dondi, Simion, & Caltran, Reference Dondi, Simion and Caltran1999). The fear bias literature typically compares infants' responses to fear solely with anger and happiness (Grossmann, Missana, & Krol, Reference Grossmann, Missana and Krol2018; Peltola, Leppänen, Mäki, & Hietanen, Reference Peltola, Leppänen, Mäki and Hietanen2009; Xie, McCormick, Westerlund, Bowman, & Nelson, Reference Xie, McCormick, Westerlund, Bowman and Nelson2019); these comparisons do not distinguish the “fearful ape” from the “distressed ape.” Indeed, some studies have shown that infants respond comparably and/or inconsistently to any arousing and/or threatening emotions (fear, anger, pain) relative to happy/neutral stimuli (Cheng, Lee, Chen, Wang, & Decety, Reference Cheng, Lee, Chen, Wang and Decety2012; Hunnius, de Wit, Vrins, & von Hofsten, Reference Hunnius, de Wit, Vrins and von Hofsten2011; Vanderwert et al., Reference Vanderwert, Westerlund, Montoya, McCormick, Miguel and Nelson2015). Thus, more evidence is required to show that infants are uniquely sensitive to fear.
Further, Grossmann suggests that the enhanced experience and privileged perception of fear make humans more caring and cooperative. To support the link between heightened fearfulness and prosociality, he draws from evidence suggestive that heightened fearfulness enhances children's guilt proneness, which in turn reduces rule violation (Kochanska, Gross, Lin, & Nichols, Reference Kochanska, Gross, Lin and Nichols2002). He also cites research showing that heightened fearfulness in children is associated with enhanced theory-of-mind skills (Wellman, Lane, LaBounty, & Olson, Reference Wellman, Lane, LaBounty and Olson2011), thought to be important for cooperation (Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, & Moll, Reference Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne and Moll2005). Although guilt proneness and social-cognitive competencies are clearly relevant to prosociality, they are at best indirect measures. Further, direct evidence seems to contradict Grossmann's claims. For example, shyness is a core dimension of fearfulness traits (Henderson, Reference Henderson2010; Poole, Tang, & Schmidt, Reference Poole, Tang, Schmidt, Pérez-Edgar and Fox2018). Many studies have shown that higher levels of shyness is associated with reduced, rather than enhanced, prosocial behavior (Beier, Terrizzi, Woodward, & Larson, Reference Beier, Terrizzi, Woodward and Larson2017; Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Fabes, Karbon, Murphy, Carlo and Wosinski1996; Karasewich, Kuhlmeier, Beier, & Dunfield, Reference Karasewich, Kuhlmeier, Beier and Dunfield2019; Tan, Mikami, Luzhanska, & Hamlin, Reference Tan, Mikami, Luzhanska and Hamlin2021; Young, Fox, & Zahn-Waxler, Reference Young, Fox and Zahn-Waxler1999), presumably because prosocial behavior requires social interaction with unfamiliar others. In addition, exhibiting fearfulness and inhibited responses early in life has consistently been associated with less adaptive social functioning (e.g., fewer close friends, higher loneliness, less prosocial behavior) and greater risk for developing social anxiety later in life (Clauss & Blackford, Reference Clauss and Blackford2012; Eisenberg, Spinrad, Taylor, & Liew, Reference Eisenberg, Spinrad, Taylor and Liew2019; Liew et al., Reference Liew, Eisenberg, Spinrad, Eggum, Haugen, Kupfer and Baham2011; Sandstrom, Uher, & Pavlova, Reference Sandstrom, Uher and Pavlova2020; Tang et al., Reference Tang, Crawford, Morales, Degnan, Pine and Fox2020). Thus, rather than promoting prosociality, enhanced fearfulness appears to have a negative impact on prosocial behavior specifically and social functioning broadly.
To support the assertion that sensitivity to fear facilitates children's prosociality, Grossmann offers limited evidence that children who exhibit a fear bias are more helpful and generous (Grossmann et al., Reference Grossmann, Missana and Krol2018; Rajhans, Altvater-Mackensen, Vaish, & Grossmann, Reference Rajhans, Altvater-Mackensen, Vaish and Grossmann2016). However, different means of measuring fear bias across these studies (e.g., latency to fixate, duration of first look) make this argument less convincing. Further, it seems that responsiveness to distress signals generally, not fear specifically, elicits caring behavior (Davidov, Zahn-Waxler, Roth-Hanania, & Knafo, Reference Davidov, Zahn-Waxler, Roth-Hanania and Knafo2013; Decety, Reference Decety, Bartal, Uzefovsky and Knafo-Noam2016). For instance, individual differences in 3–10-month-olds' empathic concern for peers and adults feigning injury (i.e., pained, not fearful) predicts caring responses at 12–18 months (Davidov et al., Reference Davidov, Paz, Roth-Hanania, Uzefovsky, Orlitsky, Mankuta and Zahn-Waxler2021; Roth-Hanania, Davidov, & Zahn-Waxler, Reference Roth-Hanania, Davidov and Zahn-Waxler2011), suggesting a role for processing others' pain in facilitating prosocial responses. Even past work from Grossmann's own lab suggests that viewing painful (vs. angry) expressions activates approach mechanisms during infancy (i.e., left hemisphere activation; Missana, Grigutsch, & Grossmann, Reference Missana, Grigutsch and Grossmann2014). Thus, evidence supporting a unique relationship between responsiveness to fear and caring behaviors appears tenuous at best.
Although Grossmann's hypothesis is intriguing, it contradicts existing work or requires further evidence to support three of its major claims: That humans are particularly fearful, highly receptive to fear, and that these tendencies promote prosociality. Instead, it is more likely that human caregiving and prosocial behavior result from the expression of, and sensitivity to, a broad range of distress cues.
Are we really fearful apes, and are fearful apes more prosocial?
Grossmann proposes that humans are both more likely to express fear than other ape species and particularly sensitive to conspecifics' fear. This heightened fearfulness and fear detection, then, serves to elicit approach from caregivers, and increases one's own tendency to approach needy conspecifics. These tendencies ultimately ensure that humans receive, and deliver, the nurturance that uniquely characterizes our species. In what follows, we argue that these claims, although interesting and provocative, are currently unsupported by the broader literature in at least three ways. First, the assertion that humans are particularly fearful requires further evidence. Second, infants' sensitivity to fear (“fear bias”) may not actually reflect responsiveness to fear specifically, but rather to a broad range of distress signals. Finally, associations between both the expression and perception of fear and children's own prosocial behaviors are empirically tenuous. We detail our concerns below, ultimately concluding that the “fearful ape” may not be a useful description of the human species.
One aspect of Grossmann's hypothesis is that humans experience and express higher levels of fear than do other nonhuman primates. Support for this assertion comes from a single study (Herrmann, Hare, Cissewski, & Tomasello, Reference Herrmann, Hare, Cissewski and Tomasello2011) in which human infants demonstrated relatively more avoidance toward novel objects and people than did nonhuman ape species. These findings, however, suffer from some important confounds. First, there were significant age differences between the human (2.5 years) and nonhuman (6.4, 6.5, and 6.8 years) primate participants, a difference exacerbated by the faster rate of maturation in nonhumans. These age differences resulted in significant body size differences between participants and experimenters across species. That is, compared with 6.5-year-old nonhuman apes, 2.5-year-old children are much smaller than an adult experimenter, and might have displayed more fear because of the vulnerabilities this size difference entailed. Indeed, although this condition was not without interpretive difficulties, a control condition in which the experimenter was absent from the scene revealed no increased avoidance in humans. Thus, claiming that heightened fearfulness is an essential feature of the human species may be premature.
Another facet of Grossmann's hypothesis posits that infants are particularly sensitive to others' fear. We propose, instead, that the literature is more consistent with a general sensitivity to distress (Decety et al., Reference Decety, Bartal, Uzefovsky and Knafo-Noam2016; de Waal & Preston, Reference de Waal and Preston2017). While human infants do demonstrate sensitivity to fear (Leppänen & Nelson, Reference Leppänen and Nelson2012), they are also highly receptive to distress signals, including pain and sadness (Addabbo, Bolognini, & Turati, Reference Addabbo, Bolognini and Turati2021; Davidov et al., Reference Davidov, Paz, Roth-Hanania, Uzefovsky, Orlitsky, Mankuta and Zahn-Waxler2021; Dondi, Simion, & Caltran, Reference Dondi, Simion and Caltran1999). The fear bias literature typically compares infants' responses to fear solely with anger and happiness (Grossmann, Missana, & Krol, Reference Grossmann, Missana and Krol2018; Peltola, Leppänen, Mäki, & Hietanen, Reference Peltola, Leppänen, Mäki and Hietanen2009; Xie, McCormick, Westerlund, Bowman, & Nelson, Reference Xie, McCormick, Westerlund, Bowman and Nelson2019); these comparisons do not distinguish the “fearful ape” from the “distressed ape.” Indeed, some studies have shown that infants respond comparably and/or inconsistently to any arousing and/or threatening emotions (fear, anger, pain) relative to happy/neutral stimuli (Cheng, Lee, Chen, Wang, & Decety, Reference Cheng, Lee, Chen, Wang and Decety2012; Hunnius, de Wit, Vrins, & von Hofsten, Reference Hunnius, de Wit, Vrins and von Hofsten2011; Vanderwert et al., Reference Vanderwert, Westerlund, Montoya, McCormick, Miguel and Nelson2015). Thus, more evidence is required to show that infants are uniquely sensitive to fear.
Further, Grossmann suggests that the enhanced experience and privileged perception of fear make humans more caring and cooperative. To support the link between heightened fearfulness and prosociality, he draws from evidence suggestive that heightened fearfulness enhances children's guilt proneness, which in turn reduces rule violation (Kochanska, Gross, Lin, & Nichols, Reference Kochanska, Gross, Lin and Nichols2002). He also cites research showing that heightened fearfulness in children is associated with enhanced theory-of-mind skills (Wellman, Lane, LaBounty, & Olson, Reference Wellman, Lane, LaBounty and Olson2011), thought to be important for cooperation (Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, & Moll, Reference Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne and Moll2005). Although guilt proneness and social-cognitive competencies are clearly relevant to prosociality, they are at best indirect measures. Further, direct evidence seems to contradict Grossmann's claims. For example, shyness is a core dimension of fearfulness traits (Henderson, Reference Henderson2010; Poole, Tang, & Schmidt, Reference Poole, Tang, Schmidt, Pérez-Edgar and Fox2018). Many studies have shown that higher levels of shyness is associated with reduced, rather than enhanced, prosocial behavior (Beier, Terrizzi, Woodward, & Larson, Reference Beier, Terrizzi, Woodward and Larson2017; Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Fabes, Karbon, Murphy, Carlo and Wosinski1996; Karasewich, Kuhlmeier, Beier, & Dunfield, Reference Karasewich, Kuhlmeier, Beier and Dunfield2019; Tan, Mikami, Luzhanska, & Hamlin, Reference Tan, Mikami, Luzhanska and Hamlin2021; Young, Fox, & Zahn-Waxler, Reference Young, Fox and Zahn-Waxler1999), presumably because prosocial behavior requires social interaction with unfamiliar others. In addition, exhibiting fearfulness and inhibited responses early in life has consistently been associated with less adaptive social functioning (e.g., fewer close friends, higher loneliness, less prosocial behavior) and greater risk for developing social anxiety later in life (Clauss & Blackford, Reference Clauss and Blackford2012; Eisenberg, Spinrad, Taylor, & Liew, Reference Eisenberg, Spinrad, Taylor and Liew2019; Liew et al., Reference Liew, Eisenberg, Spinrad, Eggum, Haugen, Kupfer and Baham2011; Sandstrom, Uher, & Pavlova, Reference Sandstrom, Uher and Pavlova2020; Tang et al., Reference Tang, Crawford, Morales, Degnan, Pine and Fox2020). Thus, rather than promoting prosociality, enhanced fearfulness appears to have a negative impact on prosocial behavior specifically and social functioning broadly.
To support the assertion that sensitivity to fear facilitates children's prosociality, Grossmann offers limited evidence that children who exhibit a fear bias are more helpful and generous (Grossmann et al., Reference Grossmann, Missana and Krol2018; Rajhans, Altvater-Mackensen, Vaish, & Grossmann, Reference Rajhans, Altvater-Mackensen, Vaish and Grossmann2016). However, different means of measuring fear bias across these studies (e.g., latency to fixate, duration of first look) make this argument less convincing. Further, it seems that responsiveness to distress signals generally, not fear specifically, elicits caring behavior (Davidov, Zahn-Waxler, Roth-Hanania, & Knafo, Reference Davidov, Zahn-Waxler, Roth-Hanania and Knafo2013; Decety, Reference Decety, Bartal, Uzefovsky and Knafo-Noam2016). For instance, individual differences in 3–10-month-olds' empathic concern for peers and adults feigning injury (i.e., pained, not fearful) predicts caring responses at 12–18 months (Davidov et al., Reference Davidov, Paz, Roth-Hanania, Uzefovsky, Orlitsky, Mankuta and Zahn-Waxler2021; Roth-Hanania, Davidov, & Zahn-Waxler, Reference Roth-Hanania, Davidov and Zahn-Waxler2011), suggesting a role for processing others' pain in facilitating prosocial responses. Even past work from Grossmann's own lab suggests that viewing painful (vs. angry) expressions activates approach mechanisms during infancy (i.e., left hemisphere activation; Missana, Grigutsch, & Grossmann, Reference Missana, Grigutsch and Grossmann2014). Thus, evidence supporting a unique relationship between responsiveness to fear and caring behaviors appears tenuous at best.
Although Grossmann's hypothesis is intriguing, it contradicts existing work or requires further evidence to support three of its major claims: That humans are particularly fearful, highly receptive to fear, and that these tendencies promote prosociality. Instead, it is more likely that human caregiving and prosocial behavior result from the expression of, and sensitivity to, a broad range of distress cues.
Financial support
RD was supported by a Joseph-Armand Bombardier Canada Graduate Scholarship under award 767-2019-2852. ET was supported by a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC) Postdoctoral Fellowship under award 756-2022-0589. JKH was supported by an SSHRC Insight Grant under award F18-04495.
Competing interest
None.