As affective scientists, we appreciate the target article's focus on how emotions emerge within early social development and are influenced by aspects of social functioning, including cooperation. The basic premise of the article should be called into question, however, when viewed through the lens of published, empirical evidence that was underemphasized, overlooked, or misinterpreted in the article's discussion of fearfulness in early childhood.
First, there is a robust and growing literature demonstrating that heightened fearfulness in children is not an adaptive trait, and is instead an important predictor of negative behavioral outcomes. The abundance of research on the relation between fearful behavior and mental health problems was greatly minimized in the target article, even though some of it was mentioned (e.g., Fox et al., Reference Fox, Buzzell, Morales, Valadez, Wilson and Henderson2021; Sandstrom, Uher, & Pavlova, Reference Sandstrom, Uher and Pavlova2020). In fact, a large body of research indicates that perceived fearfulness in children is associated with a variety of long-term negative outcomes (e.g., Buss & McDoniel, Reference Buss and McDoniel2016; Coplan, Wilson, Frohlick, & Zelenski, Reference Coplan, Wilson, Frohlick and Zelenski2006; Van Brakel, Muris, Bogels, & Thomassen, Reference Van Brakel, Muris, Bogels and Thomassen2006), contrary to the author's claims regarding the benefits of early fearfulness. It has been known for some time that the extent to which infants and young children demonstrate a fearful temperament (commonly defined as behavioral inhibition, or children's tendency to excessively avoid or withdraw from novel situations) prospectively predicts internalizing behaviors and adverse social and mental health outcomes (e.g., Chronis-Tuscano et al., Reference Chronis-Tuscano, Dengan, Pine, Perez-Edgar, Henderson, Diaz and Fox2009; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Degnan, Perez-Edgar, Henderson, Rubin, Pine and Fox2009). In fact, fearful temperament is the strongest predictor of social anxiety in later childhood, with approximately 40% of behaviorally inhibited children going on to develop anxiety disorders compared to roughly 12% in children with other temperaments (Clauss & Blackford, Reference Clauss and Blackford2012; Fox, Barker, White, Suway, & Pine, Reference Fox, Barker, White, Suway and Pine2013). Importantly, the link between fearful temperament and anxiety disorders has also been documented cross-culturally (Howard, Muris, Loxton, & Wege, Reference Howard, Muris, Loxton and Wege2017; Vreeke, Muris, Mayer, Huijding, & Rapee, Reference Vreeke, Muris, Mayer, Huijding and Rapee2013), suggesting that this relation is unlikely to be the function of an evolutionary mismatch with Western culture as suggested by the author.
Second, research suggests that parents are unlikely to differentiate between negative emotions in their infants, and contrary to the author's claim that parental responses to fear are uniquely beneficial, parents are responsive to the intensity of both positive and negative affective behaviors. Many (if not all) evocative behaviors in infants capture caregiver attention, regardless of valence or perceived emotion category (e.g., Thompson-Booth et al., Reference Thompson-Booth, Viding, Mayes, Rutherford, Hodsoll and McCrory2013). For example, caregivers are highly sensitive to behaviors that suggest positive affect in infants (Kivijarvi et al., Reference Kivijarvi, Voeten, Niemela, Raiha, Lertola and Piha2001). Additionally, caregiver sensitivity to infant distress relies more on integrating information about context and the intensity of infant distress than on making inferences about whether an infant is fearful, sad, angry, and so on (Mesman, Oster, & Camras, Reference Mesman, Oster and Camras2012). This makes sense given evidence that, across both Western and non-Western cultures, infants do not show distinct facial configurations in fear- and anger-eliciting situations (Camras et al., Reference Camras, Oster, Bakeman, Meng, Ujiie and Campos2007) and that caregivers respond similarly to bouts of child distress cross-culturally (Bornstein et al., Reference Bornstein, Tamis-LeMonda, Tal, Ludemann, Toda, Rahn and Vardi1992, Reference Bornstein, Putnick, Rigo, Esposito, Swain, Suwalsky and Venuti2017). More generally, many of the arguments offered in the target article about the adaptiveness of fear in infants and young children would also hold for caregivers' perceptions of other emotions, such as sadness or happiness, or intense affective experiences.
Further, throughout the target article, the description of infant behavior is not clearly distinguished from inferences about its cause. Infant behaviors were frequently described as “fearful” without clearly demonstrating that the situations were reasonably likely to evoke fear. Indeed, instances of a given emotion category are situationally expressed with a variety of facial configurations and behaviors (e.g., in anger, infants and adults furrow their brows in a scowl less than 30% of the time; Barrett, Adolphs, Marsella, Martinez, & Pollak, Reference Barrett, Adolphs, Marsella, Martinez and Pollak2019; Bennett, Bendersky, & Lewis, Reference Bennett, Bendersky and Lewis2002; Sears, Repetti, Reynolds, & Sperling, Reference Sears, Repetti, Reynolds and Sperling2014). Given this situated variation in expressive behaviors, some infant behaviors (such as crying) will be interpreted by adults in more than one way, depending on the context (Chóliz, Fernández-Abascal, & Martínez-Sánchez, Reference Chóliz, Fernández-Abascal and Martínez-Sánchez2013). Scientists must take care to separate their descriptions of physical actions (e.g., facial movements such as widened eyes and gasping mouth) from their inferences about the causes of such actions (such as an inference that the movements are an expression of emotion, or an expression of fear more specifically). Thus, in line with our second point it is more scientifically accurate to conclude that intense or affectively evocative behaviors, rather than fearful behaviors per se, elicit caregiving responses.
Third, parental presence reduces perceived fearfulness early in life at both the behavioral and neural levels; it does not support perceived fearfulness as one would expect if this were, indeed, adaptive. Recent developmental research suggests that a caregiver's presence reduces the development of behaviors commonly associated with perceived fearfulness, such as startle responses (e.g., Van Rooij et al., Reference Van Rooij, Cross, Stevens, Vance, Kim, Bradley and Jovanovic2017). There is also evidence in both human and nonhuman animals that the presence of a caregiver promotes exploration and learning in children, buffering against behaviors that are perceived as fearful (e.g., Callaghan et al., Reference Callaghan, Meyer, Opendak, Van Tieghem, Harmon, Li and Tottenham2019). Although the original article proposes the role of amygdala activity in supporting fearfulness, caregiver presence typically functions to buffer rapid amygdala development and premature amygdala engagement (Tottenham, Reference Tottenham2012). Further, maternal presence allows child amygdala-prefrontal circuitry to function more similarly to adolescent connectivity, thereby allowing for more mature regulatory behavior (Gee et al., Reference Gee, Gabard-Durnam, Telzer, Humphreys, Goff, Shapiro and Tottenham2014). These findings suggest that exploration, and not fearful behavior, is what is most adaptive for infants.
In our view, the target article falls short of demonstrating that heightened fearfulness in infants and young children is evolutionarily adaptive. This hypothesis is called into serious doubt by several hidden inferences that are not supported by existing research, specifically that which demonstrates long-term mental health outcomes of perceived fearfulness as well as parental buffering. Instead, intense affect more generally elicits responses from caregivers. These affective behaviors, and not fearfulness per se, may be what elicits responsive caregiving, ultimately leading to improved functionality within the lifespan of an individual human and improved adaptation of the species.
As affective scientists, we appreciate the target article's focus on how emotions emerge within early social development and are influenced by aspects of social functioning, including cooperation. The basic premise of the article should be called into question, however, when viewed through the lens of published, empirical evidence that was underemphasized, overlooked, or misinterpreted in the article's discussion of fearfulness in early childhood.
First, there is a robust and growing literature demonstrating that heightened fearfulness in children is not an adaptive trait, and is instead an important predictor of negative behavioral outcomes. The abundance of research on the relation between fearful behavior and mental health problems was greatly minimized in the target article, even though some of it was mentioned (e.g., Fox et al., Reference Fox, Buzzell, Morales, Valadez, Wilson and Henderson2021; Sandstrom, Uher, & Pavlova, Reference Sandstrom, Uher and Pavlova2020). In fact, a large body of research indicates that perceived fearfulness in children is associated with a variety of long-term negative outcomes (e.g., Buss & McDoniel, Reference Buss and McDoniel2016; Coplan, Wilson, Frohlick, & Zelenski, Reference Coplan, Wilson, Frohlick and Zelenski2006; Van Brakel, Muris, Bogels, & Thomassen, Reference Van Brakel, Muris, Bogels and Thomassen2006), contrary to the author's claims regarding the benefits of early fearfulness. It has been known for some time that the extent to which infants and young children demonstrate a fearful temperament (commonly defined as behavioral inhibition, or children's tendency to excessively avoid or withdraw from novel situations) prospectively predicts internalizing behaviors and adverse social and mental health outcomes (e.g., Chronis-Tuscano et al., Reference Chronis-Tuscano, Dengan, Pine, Perez-Edgar, Henderson, Diaz and Fox2009; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Degnan, Perez-Edgar, Henderson, Rubin, Pine and Fox2009). In fact, fearful temperament is the strongest predictor of social anxiety in later childhood, with approximately 40% of behaviorally inhibited children going on to develop anxiety disorders compared to roughly 12% in children with other temperaments (Clauss & Blackford, Reference Clauss and Blackford2012; Fox, Barker, White, Suway, & Pine, Reference Fox, Barker, White, Suway and Pine2013). Importantly, the link between fearful temperament and anxiety disorders has also been documented cross-culturally (Howard, Muris, Loxton, & Wege, Reference Howard, Muris, Loxton and Wege2017; Vreeke, Muris, Mayer, Huijding, & Rapee, Reference Vreeke, Muris, Mayer, Huijding and Rapee2013), suggesting that this relation is unlikely to be the function of an evolutionary mismatch with Western culture as suggested by the author.
Second, research suggests that parents are unlikely to differentiate between negative emotions in their infants, and contrary to the author's claim that parental responses to fear are uniquely beneficial, parents are responsive to the intensity of both positive and negative affective behaviors. Many (if not all) evocative behaviors in infants capture caregiver attention, regardless of valence or perceived emotion category (e.g., Thompson-Booth et al., Reference Thompson-Booth, Viding, Mayes, Rutherford, Hodsoll and McCrory2013). For example, caregivers are highly sensitive to behaviors that suggest positive affect in infants (Kivijarvi et al., Reference Kivijarvi, Voeten, Niemela, Raiha, Lertola and Piha2001). Additionally, caregiver sensitivity to infant distress relies more on integrating information about context and the intensity of infant distress than on making inferences about whether an infant is fearful, sad, angry, and so on (Mesman, Oster, & Camras, Reference Mesman, Oster and Camras2012). This makes sense given evidence that, across both Western and non-Western cultures, infants do not show distinct facial configurations in fear- and anger-eliciting situations (Camras et al., Reference Camras, Oster, Bakeman, Meng, Ujiie and Campos2007) and that caregivers respond similarly to bouts of child distress cross-culturally (Bornstein et al., Reference Bornstein, Tamis-LeMonda, Tal, Ludemann, Toda, Rahn and Vardi1992, Reference Bornstein, Putnick, Rigo, Esposito, Swain, Suwalsky and Venuti2017). More generally, many of the arguments offered in the target article about the adaptiveness of fear in infants and young children would also hold for caregivers' perceptions of other emotions, such as sadness or happiness, or intense affective experiences.
Further, throughout the target article, the description of infant behavior is not clearly distinguished from inferences about its cause. Infant behaviors were frequently described as “fearful” without clearly demonstrating that the situations were reasonably likely to evoke fear. Indeed, instances of a given emotion category are situationally expressed with a variety of facial configurations and behaviors (e.g., in anger, infants and adults furrow their brows in a scowl less than 30% of the time; Barrett, Adolphs, Marsella, Martinez, & Pollak, Reference Barrett, Adolphs, Marsella, Martinez and Pollak2019; Bennett, Bendersky, & Lewis, Reference Bennett, Bendersky and Lewis2002; Sears, Repetti, Reynolds, & Sperling, Reference Sears, Repetti, Reynolds and Sperling2014). Given this situated variation in expressive behaviors, some infant behaviors (such as crying) will be interpreted by adults in more than one way, depending on the context (Chóliz, Fernández-Abascal, & Martínez-Sánchez, Reference Chóliz, Fernández-Abascal and Martínez-Sánchez2013). Scientists must take care to separate their descriptions of physical actions (e.g., facial movements such as widened eyes and gasping mouth) from their inferences about the causes of such actions (such as an inference that the movements are an expression of emotion, or an expression of fear more specifically). Thus, in line with our second point it is more scientifically accurate to conclude that intense or affectively evocative behaviors, rather than fearful behaviors per se, elicit caregiving responses.
Third, parental presence reduces perceived fearfulness early in life at both the behavioral and neural levels; it does not support perceived fearfulness as one would expect if this were, indeed, adaptive. Recent developmental research suggests that a caregiver's presence reduces the development of behaviors commonly associated with perceived fearfulness, such as startle responses (e.g., Van Rooij et al., Reference Van Rooij, Cross, Stevens, Vance, Kim, Bradley and Jovanovic2017). There is also evidence in both human and nonhuman animals that the presence of a caregiver promotes exploration and learning in children, buffering against behaviors that are perceived as fearful (e.g., Callaghan et al., Reference Callaghan, Meyer, Opendak, Van Tieghem, Harmon, Li and Tottenham2019). Although the original article proposes the role of amygdala activity in supporting fearfulness, caregiver presence typically functions to buffer rapid amygdala development and premature amygdala engagement (Tottenham, Reference Tottenham2012). Further, maternal presence allows child amygdala-prefrontal circuitry to function more similarly to adolescent connectivity, thereby allowing for more mature regulatory behavior (Gee et al., Reference Gee, Gabard-Durnam, Telzer, Humphreys, Goff, Shapiro and Tottenham2014). These findings suggest that exploration, and not fearful behavior, is what is most adaptive for infants.
In our view, the target article falls short of demonstrating that heightened fearfulness in infants and young children is evolutionarily adaptive. This hypothesis is called into serious doubt by several hidden inferences that are not supported by existing research, specifically that which demonstrates long-term mental health outcomes of perceived fearfulness as well as parental buffering. Instead, intense affect more generally elicits responses from caregivers. These affective behaviors, and not fearfulness per se, may be what elicits responsive caregiving, ultimately leading to improved functionality within the lifespan of an individual human and improved adaptation of the species.
Financial support
This paper was supported by funding from the National Institutes of Health (F32HD105316 to MO), National Science Foundation (BCS 1947972 to LFB), the National Institute of Mental Health (R21MH129902 to LFB), the US Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences (W911NF-16-1-019 to LFB), the Elizabeth R. Koch Foundation (through its Unlikely Collaborators Fund to LFB), a Marie Skłodowska–Curie Individual Fellowship from the European Commission (892379 to KH) under the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation program, and a James McDonnell Foundation Scholar Award for Understanding Human Cognition (to VL). This paper reflects only the authors' views; the European Commission is not liable for any use that may be made of the contained information. The views, opinions, and/or findings contained in this review are those of the authors and shall not be construed as an official Department of the Army position, policy, or decision, unless so designated by other documents; nor do they necessarily reflect the views of the Elizabeth R. Koch Foundation.
Competing interest
None.