No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Art as emotional exploration
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 29 November 2017
Abstract
The Roman poet Horace said poetry gives pleasure and instructs. A more informative theory is that poetry and art, in general, are less about pleasure than about exploration of emotions. Literary authors concentrate on negative emotions, seemingly to try and understand them. In two studies, reading literary art enabled the transformation of selfhood, not by being instructed but by people changing in their own ways.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017
References
Chekhov, A. (1899) The lady with the toy dog. In: Anton Chekhov: Five great short stories, trans. Koteliansky, S. S. & Cannan, G.. pp. 81–94. Dover (current edition 1990).Google Scholar
Djikic, M. & Oatley, K. (2014) The art in fiction: From indirect communication to changes of the self. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts
8:498–505.Google Scholar
Djikic, M., Oatley, K. & Carland, M. (2012) Genre or artistic merit: The effect of literature on personality. Scientific Study of Literature
2:25–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Djikic, M., Oatley, K. & Peterson, J. B. (2006) The bitter-sweet labor of emoting: The linguistic comparison of writers and physicists. Creativity Research Journal
18(2):191–97. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj1802_5.Google Scholar
Djikic, M., Oatley, K., Zoeterman, S. & Peterson, J. B. (2009) On being moved by art: How reading fiction transforms the self. Creativity Research Journal
21(1):24–29. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1080/10400410802633392.Google Scholar
Horace (19 BCE/1932)
Ars poetica [The art of poetry]. In: Horace: Satires, epistles and ars poetica, ed. & trans. Fairclough, H. R.. Heineman (current edition 1932).Google Scholar
Oatley, K. (2016) Fiction: Simulation of social worlds. Trends in Cognitive Sciences
20:618–28.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W., Francis, M. E. & Booth, R. J. (2001) Linguistic inquiry and word count: LIWC 2001. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Vanhaerren, M., d'Errico, F., Stronger, C., James, S. L., Todd, J. A. & Mienis, H. K. (2006) Middle palaeolithic shell beads in Israel and Algeria. Science
312:1785.Google Scholar
Target article
The Distancing-Embracing model of the enjoyment of negative emotions in art reception
Related commentaries (32)
A social dimension to enjoyment of negative emotion in art reception
Art and fiction are signals with indeterminate truth values
Art as emotional exploration
Art enhances meaning by stimulating integrative complexity and aesthetic interest
Art reception as an interoceptive embodied predictive experience
Artistic misunderstandings: The emotional significance of historical learning in the arts
Being moved is a positive emotion, and emotions should not be equated with their vernacular labels
Boredom in art
Considering the filmmaker: Intensified continuity, narrative structure, and the Distancing-Embracing model
Context matters: How macroeconomic forces may alter the reception of negative emotions in art
Distancing, not embracing, the Distancing-Embracing model of art reception
Does art expertise facilitate distancing?
Embracing nonfiction: How to extend the Distancing-Embracing model
Emotional granularity and the musical enjoyment of sadness itself
Empathy as a guide for understanding the balancing of Distancing-Embracing with negative art
Explaining the enjoyment of negative emotions evoked by the arts: The need to consider empathy and other underlying mechanisms of emotion induction
Fiction as a bridge to action
Genre scripts and appreciation of negative emotion in the reception of film
Individual differences in embracing negatively valenced art: The roles of openness and sensation seeking
Live theatre as exception and test case for experiencing negative emotions in art
Orange is the new aesthetic
Parental response to baby cry involves brain circuits for negative emotion Distancing-Embracing
Positivity versus negativity is a matter of timing
Psychological models of art reception must be empirically grounded
Reconciling an underlying contradiction in the Distancing-Embracing model
The enjoyment of negative emotions in the experience of magic
The paradox of tragedy and emotional response to simulation
The urge to judge: Why the judgmental attitude has anything to do with the aesthetic enjoyment of negative emotions
Tuning in to art: A predictive processing account of negative emotion in art
What is art and how does it differ from aesthetics?
You are not alone – Social sharing as a necessary addition to the Embracing factor
“Negative emotions” live in stories, not in the hearts of readers who enjoy them
Author response
Negative emotions in art reception: Refining theoretical assumptions and adding variables to the Distancing-Embracing model