Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-02T18:52:04.287Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Vicarious contagion decreases differentiation – and comes with costs

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 October 2016

Ovul Sezer
Affiliation:
Harvard Business School, Boston, MA 02163. [email protected]@hbs.edu
Michael I. Norton
Affiliation:
Harvard Business School, Boston, MA 02163. [email protected]@hbs.edu

Abstract

Baumeister et al. propose that individual differentiation is a crucial determinant of group success. We apply their model to processes lying in between the individual and the group – vicarious processes. We review literature in four domains – attitudes, emotions, moral behavior, and self-regulation – showing that group identification can lead to vicarious contagion, reducing individual differentiation and inducing negative consequences.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackerman, J. M., Goldstein, N. J., Shapiro, J. R. & Bargh, J. A. (2009) You wear me out: The vicarious depletion of self-control. Psychological Science 20(3):326–32.Google Scholar
Egan, P. M., Hirt, E. R. & Karpen, S. C. (2012) Taking a fresh perspective: Vicarious restoration as a means of recovering self-control. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 48(2):457–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finkel, E. J., Campbell, W. K., Brunell, A. B., Dalton, A. N., Scarbeck, S. J. & Chartrand, T. L. (2006) High-maintenance interaction: Inefficient social coordination impairs self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 91(3):456–75.Google Scholar
Gino, F. & Galinsky, A. D. (2012) Vicarious dishonesty: When psychological closeness creates distance from one's moral compass. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 119(1):1526.Google Scholar
Goldstein, N. J. & Cialdini, R. B. (2007) The spyglass self: A model of vicarious self-perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 92(3):402–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldstein, N. J. & Hays, N. A. (2011) Illusory power transference: The vicarious experience of power. Administrative Science Quarterly 56(4):593–21.Google Scholar
Gunia, B. C., Sivanathan, N. & Galinsky, A. D. (2009) Vicarious entrapment: Your sunk costs, my escalation of commitment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 45(6):1238–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heatherton, T. F. & Vohs, K. D. (1998) Why is it so difficult to inhibit behavior? Psychological Inquiry 9:212–16.Google Scholar
Kouchaki, M. (2011) Vicarious moral licensing: The influence of others' past moral actions on moral behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 101(4):702–15.Google Scholar
Krach, S., Cohrs, J. C., de Echeverria Loebell, N. C., Kircher, T., Sommer, J., Jansen, A. & Paulus, F. M. (2011) Your flaws are my pain: Linking empathy to vicarious embarrassment. PLoS One 6(4):e18675.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lickel, B., Schmader, T., Curtis, M., Scarnier, M. & Ames, D. R. (2005) Vicarious shame and guilt. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations 8(2):145–57.Google Scholar
Monin, B., Norton, M. I., Cooper, J. & Hogg, M. A. (2004) Reacting to an assumed situation vs. conforming to an assumed reaction: The role of perceived speaker attitude in vicarious dissonance. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations 7(3):207–20.Google Scholar
Norton, M. I., Monin, B., Cooper, J. & Hogg, M. A. (2003) Vicarious dissonance: Attitude change from the inconsistency of others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 85(1):4762.Google Scholar
Turner, J. & Tajfel, H. (1982) Social identity and intergroup relations. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wesselmann, E. D., Bagg, D. & Williams, K. D. (2009) “I feel your pain”: The effects of observing ostracism on the ostracism detection system. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 45(6):1308–11.Google Scholar