Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T00:58:16.676Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neuropsychology still needs to model organismic processes “from within”

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 June 2015

Juan Pascual-Leone
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada. [email protected]://tcolab.blog.yorku.ca/
Antonio Pascual-Leone
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Windsor, Windsor, Ontario, Canada. [email protected]://www1.uwindsor.ca/people/apl/
Marie Arsalidou
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada. [email protected]://tcolab.blog.yorku.ca/ Department of Psychology, National Research University Higher School of Economics, Moscow, 101000Russia. [email protected]://www.hse.ru/en/org/persons/133641316

Abstract

Four issues are discussed: (1) differences between cognition and emotion; (2) affect, emotion, and motivation differentials, including a neuropsychological model of motivation; (3) mental attention (working memory) as a resource neither affective nor cognitive, but applicable to both; and (4) explication of neuropsychological scheme units, which have neuronal circuits as functional infrastructure, thus helping to clarify the semantics of functional connectivity.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Arbib, M. A., Erdi, P. & Szentagothai, J. (1998) Neural organization: Structure, function and dynamics. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Arsalidou, M., Morris, D. & Taylor, M. J. (2011) Converging evidence for the advantage of dynamic facial expressions. Brain Topography 24:149–63. doi: 10.1007/s10548-011-0171-4.Google Scholar
Arsalidou, M., Pascual-Leone, J. & Johnson, J. (2010) Misleading cues improve developmental assessment of attentional capacity: The color matching task. Cognitive Development 25:262–77.Google Scholar
Arsalidou, M., Pascual-Leone, J., Johnson, J., Morris, D. & Taylor, M. J. (2013) A balancing act of the brain: Activations and deactivations driven by cognitive load. Brain and Behavior 3:273385. doi: 10.1002/brb3.128.Google Scholar
Beckmann, M., Hohansen-Berg, H. & Rushworth, M. F. S. (2009) Connectivity-based parcellation of human cingulate cortex and its relation to functional specialization. Journal of Neuroscience 29(4):1175–90.Google Scholar
Cromheeke, S. & Mueller, S. C. (2014) Probing emotional influences on cognitive control: An ALE meta-analysis of cognition emotion interactions. Brain Structure and Function 219:9951008. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0549-z.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (1999) The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. Harcourt.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (2010) Self comes to mind: Constructing the conscious brain. Pantheon.Google Scholar
Greenberg, L. S. (2002) Emotion-focused therapy: Coaching clients to work through their feelings. APA Press.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. & Biven, L. (2012) The archaeology of mind: Neuroevolutionary origins of human emotions. Norton.Google Scholar
Pascual-Leone, A., Greenberg, L. S. & Pascual-Leone, J. (2009) Developments in task analysis: New methods to study change. Psychotherapy Research 19(4):52542. doi: 10.1080/10503300902897797 Google Scholar
Pascual-Leone, J. (1984) Attentional dialectic and mental effort: Towards an organismic theory of life stages. In: Beyond formal operations, ed. Commons, M. L., Richards, F. A. & Armon, C., pp. 182215. Praeger.Google Scholar
Pascual-Leone, J. (2005) Not a bridge, but an organismic (general & causal) neuro-psychology should make a difference in emotion theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28(2):213–14.Google Scholar
Pascual-Leone, J. (2012) Piaget as a pioneer of dialectical constructivism: Seeking dynamic processes for human science. In: After Piaget, ed. Marti, E. & Rodriguez, C., pp. 1541. Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Pascual-Leone, J. (2013) Can we model organismic causes of working memory, efficiency, and fluid intelligence? A meta-subjective perspective. Intelligence 41:738–43.Google Scholar
Pascual-Leone, J. (2014) Dialectics. In: Encyclopedia of critical psychology, ed. Teo, T., pp. 421–28. Springer, Science+Business Media.Google Scholar
Pascual-Leone, J. & Johnson, J. (2004) Affect, self-motivation, and cognitive development: A dialectical constructivist view. In: Motivation, emotion, and cognition: Perspectives on intellectual development and functioning, ed. Dai, D. & Sternberg, R. S., pp. 197235. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pascual-Leone, J. & Johnson, J. (2005) A dialectical constructivist view of developmental intelligence. In: Handbook of understanding and measuring intelligence, ed. Wilhelm, O. & Engle, R., pp. 177201. Sage.Google Scholar
Pascual-Leone, J. & Johnson, J. (2011) A developmental theory of mental attention: Its applications to measurement and task analysis. In: Cognitive development and working memory: A dialogue between neo-Piagetian and cognitive approaches, ed. Barrouillet, P. & Gaillard, V., pp. 1346. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Pessoa, L. (2013) The cognitive-emotional brain. From interactions to integration. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Sander, D., Grafman, J. & Zala, T. (2003) The human amygdala: An evolved system for relevance detection. Reviews in the Neurosciences 14:303–16.Google Scholar
Small, D. M., Gitelman, D. R., Gregory, M. D., Nobre, A. C., Parrish, T. B. & Mesulam, M.-M. (2003) The posterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex mediate the anticipatory allocation of spatial attention. NeuroImage 18:633–41.Google Scholar
Torta, D. M. E., Costa, T., Duca, S., Fox, P. T. & Cauda, F. (2013) Parcellation of the cingulate cortex at rest and during tasks: A meta-analytic clustering and experimental study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7:114.Google Scholar
Tsao, D. Y. & Livingstone, M. S. (2008) Mechanisms of face perception. Annual Review of Neuroscience 31:411–37. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094238.Google Scholar