Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T07:33:42.407Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Enactive neuroscience, the direct perception hypothesis, and the socially extended mind

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 June 2015

Tom Froese*
Affiliation:
Departamento de Ciencias de la Computación, Instituto de Investigaciones en Matemáticas Aplicadas y en Sistemas, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, DF 04510, Mexico; Centro de Ciencias de la Complejidad, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, DF 04510, Mexico. [email protected]://froese.wordpress.com

Abstract

Pessoa's The Cognitive-Emotional Brain (2013) is an integrative approach to neuroscience that complements other developments in cognitive science, especially enactivism. Both accept complexity as essential to mind; both tightly integrate perception, cognition, and emotion, which enactivism unifies in its foundational concept of sense-making; and both emphasize that the spatial extension of mental processes is not reducible to specific brain regions and neuroanatomical connectivity. An enactive neuroscience is emerging.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Beaton, M. (2013) Phenomenology and embodied action. Constructivist Foundations 8(3):298313.Google Scholar
Cappuccio, M. & Froese, T. (2014) Introduction. In: Enactive cognition at the edge of sense-making: Making sense of non-sense, ed. Cappuccio, M. & Froese, T., pp. 133. Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2012) Dreaming the whole cat: Generative models, predictive processing, and the enactivist conception of perceptual experience. Mind 121(483):753–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colombetti, G. (2014) The feeling body: Affective science meets the enactive mind. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Jaegher, H., Di Paolo, E. A. & Gallagher, S. (2010) Can social interaction constitute social cognition? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14(10):441–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desmidt, T., Lemoine, M., Belzung, C. & Depraz, N. (2014) The temporal dynamic of emotional emergence. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 13(4):557–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Di Paolo, E. A. (2014) The worldly constituents of perceptual presence. Frontiers in Psychology 5(450). doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Di Paolo, E. A., Rohde, M. & De Jaegher, H. (2010) Horizons for the enactive mind: Values, social interaction, and play. In: Enaction: Toward a new paradigm for cognitive science, ed. Stewart, J., Gapenne, O. & Paolo, E. A. Di, pp. 3387. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Di Paolo, E. A. & Thompson, E. (2014) The enactive approach. In: The Routledge handbook of embodied cognition, ed. Shapiro, L., pp. 6878. Routledge.Google Scholar
Friston, K. J. (2010) The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 11:127–38. doi: 10.1038/nrn2787 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Froese, T. (2014) Steps toward an enactive account of synesthesia. Cognitive Neuroscience 5(2):126–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Froese, T., Iizuka, H. & Ikegami, T. (2014) Embodied social interaction constitutes social cognition in pairs of humans: A minimalist virtual reality experiment. Scientific Reports 4(3672). doi: 10.1038/srep03672.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Froese, T. & Ikegami, T. (2013) The brain is not an isolated “black box,” nor is its goal to become one. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36(3):213–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Froese, T. & Leavens, D. A. (2014) The direct perception hypothesis: Perceiving the intention of another's action hinders its precise imitation. Frontiers in Psychology 5(65). doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00065.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Froese, T., Stanghellini, G. & Bertelli, M. O. (2013) Is it normal to be a principal mindreader? Revising theories of social cognition on the basis of schizophrenia and high functioning autism-spectrum disorders. Research in Developmental Disabilities 34(5):1376–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuchs, T. (2011) The brain – a mediating organ. Journal of Consciousness Studies 18(7–8):196221.Google Scholar
Gallagher, S. (2009) Neurophenomenology. In: The Oxford companion to consciousness, ed. Bayne, T., Cleeremans, A. & Wilken, P., pp. 470–71. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gallagher, S., Hutto, D. D., Slaby, J. & Cole, J. (2013) The brain as part of an enactive system. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36(4):421–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pessoa, L. (2013) The cognitive-emotional brain. From interactions to integration. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roesch, E. B., Nasuto, S. J. & Bishop, J. M. (2012) Emotion and anticipation in an enactive framework for cognition (response to Andy Clark). Frontiers in Psychology 3(398). doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schilbach, L., Timmermans, B., Reddy, V., Costall, A., Bente, G., Schlicht, T. & Vogeley, K. (2013) Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36(4):393462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seth, A. K. (2014) A predictive processing theory of sensorimotor contingencies: Explaining the puzzle of perceptual presence and its absence in synaesthesia. Cognitive Neuroscience 5(2):97118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, E. (2007) Mind in life: Biology, phenomenology, and the sciences of the mind. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Varela, F. J. & Depraz, N. (2005) At the source of time: Valence and the constitutional dynamics of affect. Journal of Consciousness Studies 12(8–10):6181.Google Scholar
Varela, F. J., Lachaux, J.-P., Rodriguez, E. & Martinerie, J. (2001) The brainweb: Phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 2:229–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Varela, F. J., Thompson, E. & Rosch, E. (1991) The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Von Uexküll, J. (1934/1957) A stroll through the worlds of animals and men: A picture book of invisible worlds. In: Instinctive behavior: The development of a modern concept, ed. Schiller, C. H., pp. 580. International Universities Press.Google Scholar