Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T05:36:17.720Z Has data issue: true hasContentIssue false

Meta-learning modeling and the role of affective-homeostatic states in human cognition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 September 2024

Ignacio Cea*
Affiliation:
Faculty of Religious Sciences and Philosophy, Temuco Catholic University, Temuco, Chile [email protected] https://igneocj.wixsite.com/ignacio-cea Center for Research, Innovation and Creation, Temuco Catholic University, Temuco, Chile Universidad Alberto Hurtado, Faculty of Philosophy and Humanities, Philosophy Department, Santiago, Chile
*
*Corresponding author.

Abstract

The meta-learning framework proposed by Binz et al. would gain significantly from the inclusion of affective and homeostatic elements, currently neglected in their work. These components are crucial as cognition as we know it is profoundly influenced by affective states, which arise as intricate forms of homeostatic regulation in living bodies.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Binz et al. offer a very promising research program based on meta-learning to advance our understanding of cognition. Nonetheless, their proposal could greatly benefit from integrating affective and homeostatic elements, which have been traditionally underrepresented in cognitive science and are totally absent in their article as well. This integration is predicated on the premise that cognitive processes in general are profoundly influenced by affective states (e.g., emotions and moods), which arise as intricate forms of homeostatic regulation in living organisms.

First, there is ample evidence from psychology and affective neuroscience showing the pervasive influence of affective states on cognitive processes (Cea, Reference Cea, Fossa and Cortés-Rivera2023; Clore & Schiller, Reference Clore, Schiller, Barrett, Lewis and Haviland-Jones2018). When someone feels affectively moved by new information, she will more likely attend to it and think about it for an extended duration, compared to neutral information (Manns & Bass, Reference Manns and Bass2016). People in positive moods tend to engage in more creative thinking and learn subjects meaningfully (Ormrod, Anderman, & Anderman, Reference Ormrod, Anderman and Anderman2019), while those feeling sadness or frustration are prone to a shallower learning (Ahmed, Van der Werf, Kuyper, & Minnaert, Reference Ahmed, Van der Werf, Kuyper and Minnaert2013). This is related to negative emotions being accompanied by cortisol release and the fight-or-flight response that can suppress the prefrontal cortex, thereby hindering higher cognition (Brackett, Reference Brackett2019). Hence, it is reasonable that emotion regulation abilities are the strongest predictors of academic achievement in high-school students (Di Fabio & Palazzeschi, Reference Di Fabio and Palazzeschi2009).

Also, our affective states shape what and how we perceive (Barrett, Reference Barrett2017; Cea & Martínez-Pernía, Reference Cea and Martínez-Pernía2023). According to the affective information principle, our feelings signal the value and urgency of any perceived object (Clore & Schiller, Reference Clore, Schiller, Barrett, Lewis and Haviland-Jones2018). Moreover, by altering people's affects, researchers can sway perceptions, for example, making a beverage seem appealing or distasteful (Berridge & Winkielman, Reference Berridge and Winkielman2003), and people friendly or mean (Li, Moallem, Paller, & Gottfried, Reference Li, Moallem, Paller and Gottfried2007). A key brain area involved is the orbitofrontal cortex, which integrates sensory and affective information, ensuring that our perceptions are always imbued with affect (Barrett & Bar, Reference Barrett and Bar2009).

Concerning attention, positive emotions generally broaden it, leading to a global focus (Fredrickson & Branigan, Reference Fredrickson and Branigan2005), whereas negative emotions narrow it, fostering detail-oriented processing (Bar-Haim, Lamy, Pergamin, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & Van Ijzendoorn, Reference Bar-Haim, Lamy, Pergamin, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Van Ijzendoorn2007). Also, emotionally charged stimuli are shown to capture attention more effectively than neutral ones, both in terms of speed and focus (Hajcak, Jackson, Ferri, & Weinberg, Reference Hajcak, Jackson, Ferri, Weinberg, Barrett, Lewis and Haviland-Jones2018). This can cause perceptual interference, making subsequent neutral stimuli less noticeable (Wang, Kennedy, & Most, Reference Wang, Kennedy and Most2012), a process associated with emotional allocation of attentional resources as indicated by the late positive potential in the parietal lobe (Hajcak et al., Reference Hajcak, Jackson, Ferri, Weinberg, Barrett, Lewis and Haviland-Jones2018).

Concerning memory, William James claimed that “an experience may be so exciting emotionally as almost to leave a scar upon the cerebral tissues” (James, Reference James1890, p. 670). People tend to remember information with emotional significance more easily than neutral material (Phelps & Sharot, Reference Phelps and Sharot2008). Memories associated with strong emotional arousal are recalled more vividly (Schaefer & Philippot, Reference Schaefer and Philippot2005) and with greater detail in certain respects compared to neutral memories (Kensinger & Schacter, Reference Kensinger, Schacter, Barrett, Lewis and Haviland-Jones2018). Neurobiologically, the reciprocal interactions between the amygdala and the hippocampus are considered essential for this (McGaugh, Reference McGaugh2013).

Concerning the homeostatic roots of affect, Seth and Barrett have independently suggested that affect arises from the brain's inferences about the causes of internal body signals to regulate physiological states (e.g., sugar levels, heart-beat, etc.), ensuring survival based on past experiences (Barrett, Reference Barrett2017; Barrett & Simmons, Reference Barrett and Simmons2015; Seth, Reference Seth2021; Seth & Tsakiris, Reference Seth and Tsakiris2018). Hence, according to them, our feelings would then be expressions of our current and future degree of success or failure in staying alive. In this way, moods and emotions would be intimately linked to our bodily nature and homeostatic needs.

This core idea of feelings being rooted in homeostatic regulation in vulnerable systems has been applied to robotics and artificial intelligence. Man and Damasio (Reference Man and Damasio2019) propose a novel class of soft robots that incorporate physical vulnerability and self-regulation akin to living organisms. They hypothesize that this would allow them to develop motivations and evaluations reminiscent of feelings in humans, potentially leading to more intelligent interactions with their environments. Similarly, Bronfman, Ginsburg, and Jablonka (Reference Bronfman, Ginsburg and Jablonka2021) propose that feelings may arise in artificial systems constructed with soft materials through the development of homeostatic, self-preservation mechanisms that could allow them to instantiate an open-ended domain-general form of learning, what they call unlimited associative learning. Finally, Yoshida (Reference Yoshida2017) introduces a reinforcement learning model where agents learn to survive by regulating critical variables like energy levels, using a reward system rooted in homeostatic principles, leading to adaptive behavior. Importantly, all proposals emphasize that the possibility of engineering sentient artificial systems depends on having vulnerable bodies that, akin to ourselves, need to be constantly sensed and regulated to remain integral, and that this would enhance the machines’ cognitive capacities.

To conclude, I would like to suggest some potential benefits of incorporating affective and homeostatic elements into the meta-learning research program: (i) Enhanced adaptability: By incorporating these elements into the meta-learning algorithms, the resulting computational models may better simulate the adaptability of human cognition, like the ability to adjust learning strategies to changing environmental and internal states; (ii) richer contextual understanding: Incorporating affective-homeostatic elements in learning-to-learn processes can result in a deeper understanding of how emotionally salient contexts influence cognition; (iii) improved learning efficiency: Affective-homeostatic signals can guide attention and memory processes, leading to more efficient learning. Meta-learning algorithms that incorporate affective-homeostatic signals or mechanisms may achieve higher efficiency in adapting learning algorithms to new tasks or to new information; (iv) more realistic simulations: By incorporating affect and homeostasis, meta-learned models may more accurately simulate human cognition, which is inherently influenced by affective-bodily states; and (v) cross-domain generalization: The integration of affective-homeostatic states may facilitate better generalization across different cognitive domains, as affect and bodily regulation often play a role in a wide range of cognitive tasks, from decision making to social interactions.

In sum, I encourage Binz et al. to consider the beneficial prospects of incorporating these elements into their proposed research program, so as to acknowledge the intertwined nature of affect, bodily homeostasis, and human cognition.

Acknowledgements

I am very grateful to Kingson Man for his constructive comments on an earlier version of this commentary. I am also very grateful to Thomas Wachter for his encouraging appreciation of my previous work on affect, homeostasis, and cognition.

Financial support

This research was funded by ANID-Fondecyt Postdoctoral grant #3210707 and the Center for Research, Innovation and Creation, Temuco Catholic University.

Competing interest

None.

References

Ahmed, W., Van der Werf, G., Kuyper, H., & Minnaert, A. (2013). Emotions, self-regulated learning, and achievement in mathematics: A growth curve analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(1), 150.Google Scholar
Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 1.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (2017). How emotions are made: The secret life of the brain. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., & Bar, M. (2009). See it with feeling: Affective predictions during object perception. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1521), 13251334.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., & Simmons, W. K. (2015). Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(7), 419429.Google Scholar
Berridge, K., & Winkielman, P. (2003). What is an unconscious emotion?(The case for unconscious “liking”). Cognition and Emotion, 17(2), 181211. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930302289Google Scholar
Brackett, M. (2019). Permission to feel: Unlocking the power of emotions to help our kids, ourselves, and our society thrive. Celadon Books.Google Scholar
Bronfman, Z., Ginsburg, S., & Jablonka, E. (2021). When will robots be sentient? Journal of Artificial Intelligence and Consciousness, 8(02), 183203.Google Scholar
Cea, I. (2023). The somatic roots of affect: Toward a body-centered education. In Fossa, P., & Cortés-Rivera, C. (Eds.), Affectivity and learning: Bridging the gap between neurosciences, cultural and cognitive psychology (pp. 555583). Springer.Google Scholar
Cea, I., & Martínez-Pernía, D. (2023). Continuous organismic sentience as the integration of core affect and vitality. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 30(3–4), 733. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.53765/20512201.30.3.007Google Scholar
Clore, G., & Schiller, A. (2018). New light on the affect-cognition connection. In Barrett, L. F., Lewis, M., & Haviland-Jones, J. M. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions, (4th ed., pp. 532546). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Di Fabio, A., & Palazzeschi, L. (2009). An in-depth look at scholastic success: Fluid intelligence, personality traits or emotional intelligence? Personality and Individual Differences, 46(5–6), 581585.Google Scholar
Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition & Emotion, 19(3), 313332.Google Scholar
Hajcak, G., Jackson, F., Ferri, J., & Weinberg, A. (2018). Emotion and attention. In Barrett, L. F., Lewis, M., & Haviland-Jones, J. M. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions, (4th ed., pp. 595609). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (Vol. 1, Issue n/a). Dover Publications.Google Scholar
Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2018). Memory and emotion. In Barrett, L. F., Lewis, M., & Haviland-Jones, J. M. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions, (4th ed., pp. 564578). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Li, W., Moallem, I., Paller, K. A., & Gottfried, J. A. (2007). Subliminal smells can guide social preferences. Psychological Science, 18(12), 10441049.Google Scholar
Man, K., & Damasio, A. (2019). Homeostasis and soft robotics in the design of feeling machines. Nature Machine Intelligence, 1(10), 446452.Google Scholar
Manns, J. R., & Bass, D. I. (2016). The amygdala and prioritization of declarative memories. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(4), 261265.Google Scholar
McGaugh, J. L. (2013). Making lasting memories: Remembering the significant. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(supplement_2), 1040210407.Google Scholar
Ormrod, J. E., Anderman, E. M., & Anderman, L. H. (2019). Educational psychology: Developing learners, (10th ed.). Pearson.Google Scholar
Phelps, E. A., & Sharot, T. (2008). How (and why) emotion enhances the subjective sense of recollection. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(2), 147152.Google Scholar
Schaefer, A., & Philippot, P. (2005). Selective effects of emotion on the phenomenal characteristics of autobiographical memories. Memory, 13(2), 148160.Google Scholar
Seth, A. (2021). Being you: A new science of consciousness. Penguin.Google Scholar
Seth, A. K., & Tsakiris, M. (2018). Being a beast machine: The somatic basis of selfhood. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(11), 969981. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.08.008Google Scholar
Wang, L., Kennedy, B. L., & Most, S. B. (2012). When emotion blinds: A spatiotemporal competition account of emotion-induced blindness. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 438.Google Scholar
Yoshida, N. (2017). Homeostatic agent for general environment. Journal of Artificial General Intelligence, 8(1), 1.Google Scholar