Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T07:52:54.818Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Policy Blame Game: How Polarization Distorts Democratic Accountability across the Local, State, and Federal Level

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 December 2022

Rongbo Jin
Affiliation:
School of Government & Public Policy, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA
Alexander Cloudt
Affiliation:
School of Government & Public Policy, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA
Seoungin Choi
Affiliation:
School of Government & Public Policy, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA
Zhuofan Jia
Affiliation:
School of Government & Public Policy, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA
Samara Klar*
Affiliation:
School of Government & Public Policy, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA
*
Corresponding author: Samara Klar, email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Democratic accountability relies on voters to punish their representatives for policies they dislike. Yet, a separation-of-powers system can make it hard to know who is to blame, and partisan biases further distort voters’ evaluations. During the COVID-19 pandemic, precautionary policies were put into place sometimes by governors, sometimes by mayors, and sometimes by no one at all, allowing us to identify when voters hold out-party versus in-party politicians responsible for policies. With a survey spanning 48 states, we test our theory that attitudes toward policies and parties intersect to determine when selective attribution takes place. We find that as individuals increasingly oppose a policy, they are more likely to blame whichever level of government is led by the out-party. This is most pronounced among partisans with strong in-party biases. We provide important insight into the mechanisms that drive selective attribution and the conditions under which democratic accountability is at risk.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press and State Politics & Policy Quarterly

Introduction

Democratic accountability relies on voters’ expectations that their representatives will respond to their preferences and, in equal measure, on politicians’ expectations that voters will punish them for failing to follow through on those preferences (Svolik Reference Svolik2013, 685). The entire premise of “retrospective voting”—one of the most studied topics in American political behavior (Healy, Kuo, and Malhotra Reference Healy, Kuo and Malhotra.2014)—is that voters look back on what representatives have done and judge them for it (Fearon Reference Fearon, Przeworski, Stokes and Manin1999; Ferejohn Reference Ferejohn1986). Yet accountability is not so simple. Beyond the cognitive sophistication that is required to assess and allot blame, a separation-of-powers system—like the one that rules the United States—makes blame attribution especially complicated and cognitively demanding (Healy, Kuo, and Malhotra Reference Healy, Kuo and Malhotra.2014, 145). Adding even more complexity is the tendency for voters to unduly reward politicians from their own party and, conversely, to penalize those from the opposition (Bisgaard Reference Bisgaard2015). In this study, we identify a unique circumstance that allows us to observationally assess the degree to which voters attribute blame based on partisanship or policy.

In the midst of the coronavirus disease-2019 (COVID-19) pandemic, we carried out a survey of 813 adults respondents across 48 states in America regarding two precautionary policies that were unequally distributed across the country: the mandated usage of masks, and restrictions on businesses. Across the United States, these policies were mandated by Governors, by Mayors, or sometimes by no one at all. Across each state, we asked respondents for their opinion on these policies and for their view on who specifically was responsible for the policy (or lack thereof). Because the partisanship of a mayor does not always match that of a governor, we are able to determine how the partisanship of an elected representative affects the degree to which they face blame from their constituents. We further examine how the intensity of voters’ opposition to a policy determines their willingness to blame the out-party.

Across COVID-related policies regarding masks and businesses, we find strong evidence that, as individuals become increasingly opposed to a policy, they become more likely to blame the leader whose partisanship does not match their own (known as selective attribution). Moreover, we find that this tendency to engage in selective attribution is most pronounced among partisans who report the strongest preference for their own party over the other (i.e., who are affectively polarized). This study affirms what previous work has shown (e.g., Malhotra and Kuo Reference Malhotra and Kuo.2008), in terms of the strong influence of partisanship over blame attribution, and we also forge new ground in two important ways: first, by theorizing and demonstrating how selective attribution is exacerbated by policy opposition and, second, by affective polarization. We further build on existing work by examining how selective attribution complicates accountability across different levels of the American federal system.

Our study demonstrates a troubling obstacle to democratic accountability: the intersection of a separation-of-powers, which makes accountability cognitively difficult, with a polarized public who selectively blame the out-party. Our results, however, also point to the limits of partisan-motivated reasoning, as we find that selective attribution only occurs among those who are affectively polarized—but not among roughly a quarter of the population whose in-party preferences are less pronounced. Overall, this study provides important insight into the mechanisms that drive selective attribution and the conditions under which democratic accountability is at risk.

Democratic Accountability and Selective Attribution

Democratic accountability is inherently connected to the concept of retrospective voting. Evaluating how well a candidate has done in the past is a heuristic that people can use to help make a cognitively difficult electoral choice (Fiorina Reference Fiorina1981; Woon Reference Woon2012). But the act of evaluating politicians’ past performance can be no less difficult. One key factor is the complexity of government bureaucracy and especially a division of powers across national, state, and municipal leaders, making it hard to know who is responsible when things go wrong and who deserves kudos for policy wins (Thompson Reference Thompson1980). Moreover, sometimes politicians intentionally misrepresent their own role in popular policies; for example, after House Republicans unanimously opposed President Biden’s $1.9 trillion stimulus bill in March of 2021 (which passed and was signed into law), several Republicans leaders nevertheless proceeded to tout the popular bill to their constituents via social media, deliberately attempting to take credit for the legislation that they had, in fact, voted against (Lutz Reference Lutz2021).

Accurately identifying who is responsible for policies is important because it can ultimately guide how citizens vote. For example, Arceneaux and Stein (Reference Arceneaux and Stein2006) find that voters who held their mayor responsible for the aftermath of Tropical Storm Allison became less likely to vote for them in subsequent elections. Indeed, when we consider the meaning of democracy itself—“a system of governance in which rulers are held accountable for their actions in the public realm by citizens” (Schmitter and Karl Reference Schmitter and Karl.1991)—accountability is the sine qua non.

Complicating the difficult task of correctly attributing credit and blame, voters are known for giving their own party members the benefit of the doubt and unduly placing blame on the out-party. When evaluating a policy or politicians is cognitively burdensome, partisan cues are readily available, and the stakes are not especially high, then individuals are more likely to rely on their partisanship to guide their decisions (Lodge and Taber Reference Lodge, Taber., Lupia, McCubbins and Popkin2000). In these cases, it is nearly impossible to evaluate politics evenhandedly. Existing work finds that partisan-motivated reasoning can determine how partisans interpret death statistics in the Iraq War (Gaines et al. Reference Gaines, Kuklinski, Quirk, Peyton and Verkuilen.2007), identify policy positions of the parties (Johnston, Steenbergen, and Lavine Reference Johnston, Steenbergen and Lavine2012), attribute guilt in the case of sexual misconduct (Klar and McCoy Reference Klar and McCoy2021), evaluate the economy (Lewis-Beck, Nadeau, and Elias Reference Lewis-Beck, Nadeau and Elias.2008), and assess the risk and reality of global warming (Joslyn and Haider-Markel Reference Joslyn and Haider-Markel.2014).

Partisans can be motivated to evaluate objective facts more even-handedly, for example with incentives like financial rewards (Bullock et al. Reference Bullock, Gerber, Hill and Huber.2013; Hill Reference Hill2017; Prior, Sood, and Khanna Reference Prior, Sood and Khanna.2015), accuracy scores (Prior, Sood, and Khanna Reference Prior, Sood and Khanna.2015), or even simply an additional request to justify one’s reasoning (Bolsen, Druckman, and Cook Reference Bolsen, Druckman and Cook.2014). But even when partisans do agree on the state of the world—for example, whether the economy is doing well or poorly—they nevertheless can disagree on who is responsible. This phenomenon is referred to as “selective attribution” (Malhotra and Kuo Reference Malhotra and Kuo.2008; Rudolph and Grant Reference Rudolph and Grant.2002). In fact, being forced to accept unflattering facts about one’s political ingroup can actually provide the cognitive dissonance necessary for selective attribution (Bisgaard Reference Bisgaard2019).

Tilley and Hobolt (Reference Tilley and Hobolt.2011) draw an important distinction between policy performance evaluations from responsibility attributions. When confronted with the cognitive dissonance of a poorly performing policy promoted by one’s political ingroup, they suggest that one can resolve that dissonance by either altering one’s evaluation of the facts on the ground (i.e., selective evaluation of the performance of the policy), or one can accept the poor performance of the policy and instead attempt to attribute that poor performance to one’s political outgroup (i.e., selective attribution). Ultimately, they find weak evidence for the former and stronger evidence for the latter. Even non-partisan testimony, as shown in experimental work by Lyons and Jaeger (Reference Lyons and Jaeger.2014), does not mute partisan-motivated reasoning but is rather weaponized by both sides to defend their own party. Tilley and Hobolt (Reference Tilley and Hobolt.2011) argue that different factors (e.g., media exposure, political sophistication) can moderate the degree of responsibility assigned but, ultimately, it is partisanship that tends to determine the direction of attribution (i.e., who gets the blame).

Just as the conditions are specific when it comes to reducing the effect of partisanship on policy performance evaluation, so too are the conditions specific when it comes to reducing the effect of partisanship on selective attribution. Partisans have been shown to attribute less blame to the out-party when partisans are aware that the policy failure was beyond the purview of the out-party office holder (Malhotra and Kuo Reference Malhotra and Kuo.2008). Inversely, partisanship matters more when a disaster is thought to be within the purview of an out-party official (Healy, Kuo, and Malhotra Reference Healy, Kuo and Malhotra.2014). With an experimental study focusing on the government’s inadequate response to Hurricane Katrina, Malhotra and Kuo (Reference Malhotra and Kuo.2008) finds that both Republicans and Democrats are more likely to blame officials of the opposing political party than they are to blame their own, though the effects of a party cue are mitigated among respondents who also received information about the specific offices each official held. However, when partisans are not sure who ought to be responsible, they tend to blame whichever governmental official does not share their party affiliation (Brown Reference Brown2010). We expect that, in the case of COVID-19 policies, we should find support for this earlier work: respondents will be most likely to blame the policies they oppose on officials who do not share their partisanship.

Hypothesis 1: Respondents will blame policies they oppose on officials who do not share their partisanship.

The tendency to follow partisan cues in evaluating decisions is not uniform across the population. Considerable variation can be expected depending on factors including individuals’ preference for their own party over the other. Johnston, Steenbergen, and Lavine (Reference Johnston, Steenbergen and Lavine2012) distinguish “ambivalent voters” from “univalent voters.” The former hold a mix of both positive and negative affect toward both parties and subsequently engage in more even-handed evaluation of a given policy matter. The latter (those who are univalent) hold overwhelmingly positive feelings toward their own party and overwhelmingly negative feelings toward the opposing party. These individuals are most likely to engage in biased processing, even supporting policies they might otherwise oppose simply because their preferred party endorses it.

Affective polarization—the tendency to favor one’s own party relative to the other (often measured with feeling thermometer scales)—produces a similar phenomenon. Individuals who are low in affective polarization are, according to Arceneaux and Vander Wielen (Reference Arceneaux and Vander Wielen2017), most likely to reflect on issues and events. They are—in line with Johnston, Lavine, and Federico (Reference Johnston, Lavine and Federico.2017)’s work—the least biased in how they process political information. Those high in affective polarization, who are least reflective, engaged in more biased processing.

Empirical work demonstrates how affective polarization can indeed predict partisan biases and, conversely, how those lower on the affective polarization scale appear to evaluate politics with greater objectivity. For example, higher affective polarization predicts behaviors that closely follow elite cues even when it comes to public health (Druckman et al. Reference Druckman, Klar, Krupnikov, Levendusky and Ryan.2021); those who are affectively polarized are more likely to bias their judgments regarding sexual misconduct in politics (Klar and McCoy Reference Klar and McCoy2021); affective polarization similarly predicts support for (or opposition to) democratic norms (Kingzette et al. Reference Kingzette, Druckman, Klar, Krupnikov, Levendusky and Ryan2021). Over the past several decades, it appears that Americans overall have grown more affectively polarized (Iyengar et al. Reference Iyengar, Lelkes, Levendusky, Malhotra and Westwood2019). Yet there still remains substantial variation in the degree to which Americans prefer their own party over the opposing one (Krupnikov and Ryan Reference Krupnikov and Ryan.Forthcoming). We thus expect that selective attribution should similarly depend on an individual’s own stated in-party bias.

Hypothesis 2: As in-party bias affective polarization grows, individuals will be more likely to blame the out-party for the policies they oppose.

We next turn to the unique observational setting that allows us to test these hypotheses.

Data and Methods

In response to the outbreak of COVID-19, a variety of preventative measures emerged across some, but not all, 50 states. By late March of 2020, the Center for Disease Control recommended that Americans wear face-masks and maintain distance from one another in order to prevent the spread of COVID. President Trump opted against any type of executive order that might mandate such a policy and instead left it to states to decide whether or not to mandate these precautions. Governors varied in their approaches to these regulations: some mandated masks and restrictions on businesses for their entire state, others left it to local mayors to implement mandates in their cities or towns, and still others signed laws prohibiting such regulations altogether. At the time that we carried out this study (January 2021), 37 states had state-wide mask mandates and the remaining 13 states had left it to local mayors to implement mandates and regulations. Within these 13 states, policies varied widely across towns and cities. This variation across states and municipalities allows us to measure the degree to which respondents attribute policies to their mayors, their governors, or the president himself.

To test our hypotheses regarding the role of partisanship, policy, and affective polarization in selective attribution, we surveyed 833 adults (aged 18 years or older) across the United States. Our sample was recruited by Dynata, an online public opinion research company that recruits large and diverse samples of adults to participate in opt-in online surveys. Our intention is not to infer the distribution of a variable across the broader population, but rather to test the association between multiple variables within a sample in which the covariates of interest are sufficiently distributed (Druckman and Kam Reference Druckman, Kam., Druckman, Green, Kuklinski and Lupia2011). This convenience sample provides excellent variation on these key variables: partisanship, geographic location, and affective polarization. Hill et al. (Reference Hill, Lo, Vavreck and Zaller2007) argue that small bias found in opt-in Internet samples “is not so great as to vitiate the gains of inexpensive, large, and targeted samples that Internet technology make possible” (3) and all survey work, regardless of mode, should acknowledge any bias that might exist in their sample. Most notably, our sample is more white and more educated than the general population. We have no theoretical bases to expect that these differences should distort our findings. Ultimately, using different data sources is what allows us, collectively as researchers, to explore the external validity of a finding—that is, the extent to which the “causal relationship holds over variations in persons, settings, treatments [and timing], and outcomes” (Shadish, Cook, and Campbell Reference Shadish, Cook and Campbell.2002, 83). Our sample is slightly older, more educated, and less racially diverse. An overview of the sample, as well as a comparison with the US census, is provided in our Appendix.

The respondents we reached resided in 724 different towns or cities across the country and in every state, except for Vermont and Wyoming. The geographic distribution of our sample roughly approximates the actual distribution of where the broad American population resides, with more respondents sampled in higher-population states. We display the distribution of where our respondents reside in Figure 1.

Figure 1. Distribution of state residence among respondents.

We asked each respondent for the name of their state and city or town, and for the partisanship of their mayor and their governor. Although we offered a “not sure” option, 94% of respondents were able to tell us if their governor is a Democrat or a Republican. By contrast, only 73% could say whether their mayor is a Democrat or a Republican (a point to which we will return as we contemplate the implications of our results).Footnote 1

We then asked each respondent whether there was a mask mandate in the city or town where they lived. We provided the following response options: (1) Definitely; (2) Probably; (3) I’m not sure; (4) Probably not; (5) Definitely not. Only 5% of our respondents were “not sure” while the remaining 95% could tell us whether there was or was not a mask mandate. Next, we asked respondents whether they believe that there should be a mask mandate, with the response options: (1) Definitely; (2) Probably; (3) I’m not sure; (4) Probably not; (5) Definitely not. Finally, we asked them a series of questions about who is responsible for the fact that there is, or is not, a mask mandate in their city or town. For those who told us they have a mandate, we asked: “To what extent is your mayor responsible for the fact that there is a mask mandate in the city or town where you live?” We then asked this same question about their governor and about then-sitting President Trump. For those who told us that they do not have a mask mandate, we asked: “To what extent is your mayor responsible for the fact that there is not a mask mandate in the city or town where you live?” We then asked this same question about their governor and about President Trump.

We then repeated this battery of questions, but instead of mask mandates, we asked about restrictions on businesses. We learned whether business restrictions existed in each respondent’s city or town, whether they themselves support business restrictions, and the degree to which they hold their mayor, governor, and president responsible for the fact that there are, or are not, business restrictions where they live. Finally, we asked a series of demographic questions, including partisan identification, as well as thermometer ratings for both parties (in order to measure their affective polarization score). Each of these questions, in addition to the demographic questions we asked, is listed in our Appendix.

Collectively, these questions provide us with all of the information necessary to test our hypotheses. For testing Hypothesis 1, we used information from the respondent about whether there is a mask mandate and business restrictions in their town and whether the respondent opposes their local policies. We subtracted their support for the policy from their certainty that the policy exists, creating a 5-point variable ranging from 0 to 4, where 0 indicates the most support for their local policy and a 4 indicates the most opposition to their local policy. For example, if a respondent is certain the policy exists in their town and they strongly oppose such a policy, they would score a 4. If a respondent is certain that the policy does not exist in their town and they strongly support such a policy, they would also score a 4. A score of 0 would mean that they are certain the policy exists and they fully support it, or that they are certain that the policy does not exist and they fully oppose such a policy. Midpoints on the scale (i.e., scores of 1–3) reflect variation in the strength of their opposition to the local policy. For both the mask mandates and business restrictions, a slight majority fully supported their local policy (60% for mask mandates and 58% for business restrictions). The remaining 40% (for the mask mandate) and 42% (for business restrictions) expressed some degree of opposition.

We next constructed variables to indicate whether a respondent shares the same party identification as their mayor, governor, and president (who, for all respondents, was Trump). We classify leaners as partisans and create a binary variable in each case: 0 if the candidate is from the out-party and 1 if they share the same partisanship.

To test Hypothesis 2, we constructed the standard measure of affective polarization by subtracting respondents’ out-party ratings from their in-party ratings. The thermometer rating ranges from 1 to 10, and so a score of 9 would mean that a respondent expressed the greatest warmth for their own party (10) and the lowest for the out-party (1). The distribution of affective polarization scores is shown in Figure 2. The negative values indicate that just over 1% of respondents reported to favor the out-party over the in-party. We keep these outliers in the dataset and include them in our analyses, but all of the results we present in this paper are identical when we exclude these outliers and instead scale the measure from 0 to 1.

Figure 2. Distribution of in-party bias (also known as affective polarization) among respondents.

We test both hypotheses with one single model, which contains the variables and interactions we require for all of our tests. In this manuscript, we plot all of our results. In our Appendix, we include all of the output from the regression tables. We next turn to our analyses and results. We examine two distinct policies in this study: mask mandates and business closures. We present results for both of these policies as we move sequentially through our hypotheses.

Results

Hypothesis 1: Policy Opposition & Selective Attribution

Our first hypothesis states that respondents will blame policies they oppose on officials who do not share their partisanship. Here we use two key variables: the degree to which respondents oppose their local policy and the (mis)match between their own partisanship and that of their elected representative. We also control for several demographics that might account for selection bias in our observational data (including age [coded categorically from youngest to oldest], racial minority status [coded as 1 for white respondents and 0 for non-white respondents], gender [coded as 1 for women and 0 for men], and education [coded categorically from least amount of educational attainment to most]; see the Appendix for complete details).

We begin at the local level and examine the degree to which opposition to local COVID policies determines the blame that respondents place on their local mayor. In Figure 3 (left panel), the x-axis illustrates increasing opposition to the respondent’s local mask policy. The dark solid line shows the marginal effect of opposition to the policy on the degree to which respondents blame a mayor who shares their partisanship. The dotted line shows the same relationship, but for those whose mayor is from the opposing party. At the left-most side of the x-axis, where respondents express support for the policy, there is no statistically significant difference in whether out-party versus in-party mayors are deemed responsible for the popular policy. But, moving right-ward along the x-axis, we see that as opposition to the policy grows, so does polarization of blame. As respondents become more opposed to their local mask policy, they become significantly more likely to blame an out-party mayor and significantly less likely to blame an in-party mayor.

Figure 3. Marginal effect of opposition to mask mandates (left panel) and business restrictions (right panel) on blaming in-party (solid line) versus out-party (dashed line) mayors.

Note. Ninety-five percent confidence intervals are highlighted.

We find identical results with respect to the business closures (see Figure 3, right panel). Again, as opposition to the policy grows, respondents become more likely to blame mayors who align with the opposing party and less likely to blame in-party mayors. Among those who support the policy, the partisanship of the mayor has no impact on the blame they receive. Substantively, the greatest shifts occur with respect to in-partisans: when it comes to local business closures, as partisans move from most supportive to most opposed to the policy, they shift by one full point on a 5-point scale toward saying that an in-party mayor is not responsible for the policy. Opposing the policies significantly increases the tendency to blame out-partisans but the substantive effect is largest when it comes to shielding in-party mayors from blame.

We next turn to the blame assigned to one’s governor regarding the mask policy (Figure 4, left panel) and the business restrictions (Figure 4, right panel). Here we find strong support for our first hypothesis. As opposition to the mask policy and to business restrictions increase, the gap between blaming in-partisan and out-partisan governors grows significantly. The movement occurs primarily with respect to in-party governors, who become more immune to criticism as opposition grows, while out-party governors become just slightly more likely to receive blame. Again, the shift is substantively greatest when it comes to attitudes toward in-party governors. While partisans are significantly more likely to blame out-party governors as their opposition to the policy grows, the substantively largest shifts occur in their growing tendency to deny that an in-party mayor could be responsible for a policy they dislike.

Figure 4. Marginal effect of opposition to mask mandates (left panel) and business restrictions (right panel) on blaming in-party (solid line) versus out-party (dashed line) governors.

Note. Ninety-five percent confidence intervals are highlighted.

Finally, we turn to the President himself. At the time that we administered this study, Republican President Trump led the nation. Therefore, in this case, all of our Democratic respondents are out-partisans and all of our Republican respondents are in-partisans. Again, we present the blame attributed to Trump for the mask policy (Figure 5, left panel) and for the business restrictions (Figure 5, right panel).

Figure 5. Marginal effect of opposition to mask mandates (left panel) and business restrictions (right panel) on blaming in-party (solid line) versus out-party (dashed line) president.

Note. Ninety-five percent confidence intervals are highlighted.

As before, selective attribution significantly grows as opposition to the policy increases. We find that as opposition grows, Trump receives dramatically more blame from out-party members (i.e., Democrats). In-party members (i.e., Republicans) remain unlikely to hold the president responsible for their local policy, regardless of their opposition to it. Over both mask mandates and policy closures, and across all three levels of government (mayor, governor, and president), we find that individuals become significantly more likely to attribute a policy they oppose on a level of government that aligns with the opposing party. This is substantively largest among Democrats who become much more likely to blame Trump as their opposition to the policy grows: they shift by three points on the 5-point blame scale when it comes to masks, and by more than one and a half full points when it comes to business closures.

Hypothesis 2: The Exacerbating Role of Affective Polarization

Individuals who are affectively polarized—that is, who hold a strong personal preference for their own party over the other—are uniquely prone to ascribe blame based on partisanship. This leads us to our second hypothesis: selective attribution for disliked policies will be most pronounced among those who are affectively polarized. We test this by including a measure of each respondent’s in-party bias (also known as affective polarization), calculated based on their thermometer ratings. As before, we will begin at the local level (analyzing the blame that is attributed to one’s mayor) and will work our way up toward the presidency.

Figure 6 (left panel) illustrates the marginal effect of opposing one’s local mask policy on blame (y-axis) as in-party bias increases (x-axis). The solid line shows the effect on blame among in-partisans and the dashed line displays the effect on blaming among out-partisans. If our hypothesis were supported, we would see a significant interaction between the two: blame attributed to in-party mayors should decrease and out-party mayors should increase as affective polarization grows. Our hypothesis regarding the role of in-party bias is thus not supported when it comes to mayors. We find no role of affective polarization in determining selective attribution. This runs counter to our expectation and, after we turn to the results for governor and the presidency, we will discuss this particular result further.

Figure 6. Marginal effect of in-party bias on blaming in-party (solid line) versus out-party (dashed line) mayors as opposition to mask policy increases (left panel) and opposition to business restrictions increases (right panel).

Note. Zero line is darkened and 95% confidence intervals are highlighted.

When it comes to the blame that is attributed to the governor, however, the data strongly support our hypothesis. The blame attributed to in-party governors is displayed with the solid line in Figure 7, and out-party governors is displayed with the dashed line. We display the results for local mask policies in the left panel and business closures in the right panel. Here we see that, among those lowest in in-party biases, there is no significant difference in how opposition to a policy drives in-party versus out-party members’ attribution of blame. As in-party bias increases, again, we see stark selective attribution in the case of mask mandates (left panel) and business closures (right panel). With respect to mask mandates, as in-party bias grows, there is a substantial shift both in the tendency for partisans to blame the out-party governor as well as to defend those from the in-party. When it comes to business restrictions, the shift is greatest in partisans’ growing reluctance to blame governors from their own party.

Figure 7. Marginal effect of in-party bias on blaming in-party (solid line) versus out-party (dashed line) governors as opposition to mask policy increases (left panel) and opposition to business restrictions increases (right panel).

Note. Zero line is darkened and 95% confidence intervals are highlighted.

Figure 7 (left panel) illustrates the marginal effect of opposing one’s local mask policy (left panel) and business restrictions (right panel) on blaming Trump (y-axis) as in-party bias increases (x-axis). The dashed line shows the effect on blame among out-partisans (i.e., Democrats evaluating Republican President Donald Trump) and the solid line displays the effect on blaming among in-partisans (i.e., Republicans evaluating Republican President Donald Trump). Beginning with out-partisans (dashed line), we can see that as in-party bias increases, opposing a policy makes individuals more likely to blame the out-party president. Turning to in-partisans (the solid line), we see that, as affective polarization increases, opposing a policy makes people less likely to blame their in-party president. At the lowest levels of in-party bias, Republicans are actually slightly more likely to attribute the policy to their in-party president than are Democrats. But as in-party bias grows, opponents of the policy become resolute in blaming the president only if he is aligned with the opposing party.

We find this exact same pattern with respect to attitudes toward local business closures (Figure 8, right panel). Among those low in in-party bias, opposing a policy leads to no significant difference in how Democrats versus Republicans blame President Trump. But, again, as in-party bias grows, the divergence is stark. At the highest levels of in-party bias, opposition to a policy leads Democrats to blame the Republican party and leads Republicans to shield their in-party president from blame. Substantively, we find very similar patterns with the president as we did for governors: when it comes to mask policies, there are substantive shifts both in the tendency for Democrats to increasingly blame Trump and for Republicans to increasingly deny Trump’s responsibility; but, for business restrictions, the largest substantive shift is out-partisans’ tendency to blame the president for his role as their in-party bias grows.

Figure 8. Marginal effect of in-party bias on blaming in-party (solid line) versus out-party (dashed line) president as opposition to mask policy increases (left panel) and opposition to business restrictions increases (right panel).

Note. Zero line is darkened and 95% confidence intervals are highlighted.

Overall, we find consistent support for Hypothesis 1 across all three levels of government (local, state, and federal). We find support for Hypothesis 2 only at the state level and federal level, but not among mayors. We next devote some more attention to the possible sources for these mixed findings.

What Makes Mayors Different?

We find that, as partisans increasingly oppose a policy, their tendency to blame out-partisan mayors grows. Yet, contrary to our second hypothesis, this is not exacerbated by affective polarization. In other words, personal feelings of dislike toward the out-party exacerbate selective attribution only against governors and the president, but not against the mayor. In this brief section, we consider why this might be. Druckman et al. (Reference Druckman, Klar, Krupnikov, Levendusky and Ryan.2021) show that affective polarization increases partisan-motivated reasoning only when the target is sufficiently politicized. It seems, with the benefit of hindsight, that mayors are not sufficiently politicized. This is evident in our own data, as well as from existing research. Nearly 27% of our respondents could not identify the partisanship of their mayor. This conforms with a dataset of mayoral elections compiled by de Benedictis-Kessner and Warshaw (Reference de Benedictis-Kessner and Warshaw.2016); only 66% of the mayoral elections they examine occurred between two candidates who can be identified as a Democrat or a Republican. Recently, Das et al. (Reference Das, Sinclair, Webster and Yan2022) examined the political rhetoric used on Twitter by different levels of government. They find that political rhetoric from governors tends to align with national politics and parties, whereas mayors are largely focused on local issues and remain relatively distant from national politics. Das and coauthors conclude that, “unlike elected officials at other levels of government, the average American mayor currently appears to be unaffected by the nationalization of American politics” (28). Given the lesson from Druckman et al. (Reference Druckman, Klar, Krupnikov, Levendusky and Ryan.2021) that politicization is a perquisite for affective polarization to impact policy attitudes, we can speculate that mayors are seemingly insufficiently politicized.

Scholars have debated the extent to which mayors’ partisanship impacts even their own policy-making. Gerber and Hopkins (Reference Gerber and Hopkins.2011) found that mayoral partisanship affected the budget they allocate to public safety, but had no impact on tax policy, social policy, or other areas with overlapping authority. Work by de Benedictis-Kessner and Warshaw (Reference de Benedictis-Kessner and Warshaw.2016), though, demonstrates a greater impact of a mayor’s partisanship: Democrats, they show, increase overall city expenditures in a variety of areas mostly through increasing city debt rather than by increasing taxes. This might be a trend that is shifting, though; Hopkins argues that mayors (and other local officials) are becoming more closely tied to national politicians, and mayoral votes are increasingly cast “with an eye toward national politics” (19). If this is indeed the case, then one might expect that affective polarization becomes relevant for mayoral politics in the future. This is certainly a question for future researchers to revisit.

Discussion

Mask mandates and business regulations were evidently a highly salient issue for most Americans: barely more than 5% of our sample did not know whether their city had a mask mandate at the time of our survey, and less than 4% did not know whether regulations were being imposed on businesses. These policies were front-and-center in people’s lives as the country coped with a deadly pandemic. Without any nation-level executive orders, governors and mayors were left to chart their own course in response to the COVID-19 pandemic. This provides a unique opportunity to assess the degree to which voters rely on partisanship as they attribute blame for these highly salient policies.

Our results both affirm preexisting work and also contribute new findings and implications to how we understand democratic accountability, particularly in a polarized era. We provide real-world evidence that partisanship plays a key role in how voters attribute blame or credit. We take this a step further by demonstrating that the degree to which voters oppose a policy exacerbates their likelihood of engaging in selective attribution. Moreover, we introduce the role of in-party bias (or affective polarization) and show that the tendency to selectively blame out-party officials is more pronounced among Americans who are affectively polarized. For those with low in-party biases, there is ostensibly less motivation to blame governmental officials who align with the other party. Selective attribution is this conditional on both attitudes toward the policy and feelings toward the parties.

For this study, we rely on a non-probability-based opt-in panel, which are often used when researchers are interested in studying the relationship between particular variables that are sufficiently distributed across a convenience sample (for e.g., Malhotra and Kuo Reference Malhotra and Kuo.2008). These types of data sources tend not to create specific “panel effects” which would bias survey responses, nor does panel attrition lead to selection bias (Dennis et al. Reference Dennis, Chatt, Li, Motta-Stanko and Pulliam.2005; Dennis Reference Dennis2001). Importantly, the intention of our study is not to infer the distribution of a variable across the broader population, but rather to test the association between multiple variables. We thus simply require a sample in which the covariates of interest are sufficiently distributed (Druckman and Kam Reference Druckman, Kam., Druckman, Green, Kuklinski and Lupia2011). Our convenience sample provides an excellent variation on key variables: partisanship, geographic location, and affective polarization. There are also variables for which our sample does not match that of the population. Most notably, our sample is more white and more educated than a national population. We have no theoretical bases to expect that race or education should distort our findings but, ultimately, using different data sources is what allows us to, collectively as researchers, explore the external validity of a finding (Shadish, Cook, and Campbell Reference Shadish, Cook and Campbell.2002, 83).

This study also leads to several other important questions for future research. The political context in which our study takes place is arguably unique: a pandemic gripped the nation and the parties stood notably divided regarding their approaches to it. While existing work tends to argue that highly-salient issues are least susceptible to partisan-motivated reasoning (e.g., Mummolo Reference Mummolo2016), our work shows that this very highly-salient issue is subject to evident partisan-driven evaluations—but only among those who are most polarized. Furthermore, COVID policies were particularly subject to competing elite cues, making them especially ripe for polarization (Druckman et al. Reference Druckman, Klar, Krupnikov, Levendusky and Ryan.2021). Given the enormous implications of selective attribution on maintaining a representative democracy, the specific conditions under which individuals are likely to engage in selective attribution are worth continued inquiry. Overall, our study suggests that when blame for policies is not easily identifiable—thanks, in part, to the separation of powers that allows for various levels of government to pass the buck—partisanship plays an especially important role in determining who gets blamed, rightly or wrongly. As the aftermath of these COVID-specific policies unfolds, the retrospective evaluations that guide electoral decision-making will no doubt be distorted by the prism of partisanship.

Data Availability Statement

Replication materials are available on SPPQ Dataverse at https://doi.org/10.15139/S3/5YQIWP (Choi et al. Reference Choi, Cloudt, Jia, Jin and Klar2022).

Funding Statement

The authors received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Appendix

A.1 Section 1. Demographics of study sample relative to US Census

A.2 Section 2. Full survey

In which state do you currently reside?

[Drop-down list with all states, including an option for “I do not reside in the USA”; non-residents excluded from sample].

In which city or town do you live?

[Open-ended].

Do you happen to know whether your Mayor is a Democrat or a Republican?

Republican.

Democrat.

Not sure.

Do you happen to know whether your Governor is a Democrat or a Republican?

Republican.

Democrat.

Not sure.

Is there a mask mandate in the city or town where you live?

Definitely yes.

Probably yes.

Not sure.

Probably not.

Definitely not.

Do you believe there should be a mask mandate in the city or town where you live?

Definitely yes.

Probably yes.

Not sure.

Probably not.

Definitely not.

[IF THE RESPONDENT SAYS THERE IS NOT A MASK MANDATE]:

As far as you know, whose decision was it to not have a mask mandate where you live?

Is your Mayor responsible for the fact that there is not a mask mandate where you live?

Mayor is definitely responsible for not having a mask mandate.

Mayor is probably responsible for not having a mask mandate.

Not sure.

Mayor is probably not responsible for not having a mask mandate.

Mayor is definitely not responsible for not having a mask mandate.

Is your Governor responsible for the fact that there is not a mask mandate where you live?

Governor is definitely responsible for not having a mask mandate.

Governor is probably responsible for not having a mask mandate.

Not sure.

Governor is probably not responsible for not having a mask mandate.

Governor is definitely not responsible for not having a mask mandate.

Is President Trump responsible for the fact that there is not a mask mandate where you live?

President Trump is definitely responsible for not having a mask mandate.

President Trump is probably responsible for not having a mask mandate.

Not sure.

President Trump is probably not responsible for not having a mask mandate.

President Trump is definitely not responsible for not having a mask mandate.

[IF THE RESPONDENT SAYS THERE IS A MASK MANDATE]:

As far as you know, whose decision was it to have a mask mandate where you live?

Is your Mayor responsible for the fact that there is a mask mandate where you live?

Mayor is definitely responsible for the mask mandate.

Mayor is probably responsible for the mask mandate.

Not sure.

Mayor is probably not responsible for the mask mandate.

Mayor is definitely not responsible for the mask mandate.

Is your Governor responsible for the fact that there is a mask mandate where you live?

Governor is definitely responsible for the mask mandate.

Governor is probably responsible for the mask mandate.

Not sure.

Governor is probably not responsible for the mask mandate.

Governor is definitely not responsible for the mask mandate.

Is President Trump responsible for the fact that there is a mask mandate where you live?

President Trump is definitely responsible for the mask mandate.

President Trump is probably responsible for the mask mandate.

Not sure.

President Trump is probably not responsible for the mask mandate.

President Trump is definitely not responsible for the mask mandate.

[ALL RESPONDENTS].

Because of the pandemic, are there restrictions on restaurants or businesses in the city or town where you live?

Definitely yes.

Probably yes.

Not sure.

Probably not.

Definitely not.

Do you believe that restaurants or businesses should be restricted in the city or town where you live?

Definitely yes.

Probably yes.

Not sure.

Probably not.

Definitely not.

[IF THE RESPONDENT SAYS THERE ARE NOT BUSINESS RESTRICTIONS]:

As far as you know, whose decision was it to not have business restrictions where you live?

Is your Mayor responsible for the fact that there are no restrictions on businesses where you live?

Mayor is definitely responsible for having no business restrictions.

Mayor is probably responsible for having no business restrictions.

Not sure.

Mayor is probably not responsible for having no business restrictions.

Mayor is definitely not responsible for having no business restrictions.

Is your Governor responsible for the fact that there are no restrictions on businesses where you live?

Governor is definitely responsible for having no business restrictions.

Governor is probably responsible for having no business restrictions.

Not sure.

Governor is probably not responsible for having no business restrictions.

Governor is definitely not responsible for having no business restrictions.

Is President Trump responsible for the fact that there are no restrictions on businesses where you live?

President Trump is definitely responsible for having no business restrictions.

President Trump is probably responsible for having no business restrictions.

Not sure.

President Trump is probably not responsible for having no business restrictions.

President Trump is definitely not responsible for having no business restrictions.

[IF THE RESPONDENT SAYS THERE ARE BUSINESS RESTRICTIONS]:

As far as you know, whose decision is it to decide whether or not there are business restrictions where you live?

Is your Mayor responsible for implementing restrictions on businesses where you live?

Mayor is definitely responsible for business restrictions.

Mayor is probably responsible for business restrictions.

Not sure.

Mayor is probably not responsible for business restrictions.

Mayor is definitely not responsible for business restrictions.

Is your Governor responsible for implementing restrictions on businesses where you live?

Governor is definitely responsible for business restrictions.

Governor is probably responsible for business restrictions.

Not sure.

Governor is probably not responsible for business restrictions.

Governor is definitely not responsible for business restrictions.

Is President Trump responsible for implementing restrictions on businesses where you live?

President Trump is definitely responsible for business restrictions.

President Trump is probably responsible for business restrictions.

Not sure.

President Trump is probably not responsible for business restrictions.

President Trump is definitely not responsible for business restrictions.

[ALL RESPONDENTS].

In this last section of the survey, we have a few questions about you. Please remember that all of your answers are completely anonymous.

Generally speaking, do you usually think of yourself as a Republican, a Democrat, an Independent, or what?

Strong Democrat.

Democrat.

Independent leaning to Democrat.

Independent.

Independent leaning to Republican.

Republican.

Strong Republican.

When it comes to your political views, would you say you are generally liberal, moderate, or conservative?

Very liberal.

Liberal.

Moderate.

Conservative.

Very conservative.

How warm or cold do you feel toward the Republican party, on a scale from 1 to 10, where 1 is coldest and 10 is warmest?

[Drop down menu from 1 to 10].

How warm or cold do you feel toward the Democratic party, on a scale from 1 to 10, where 1 is coldest and 10 is warmest?

[Drop down menu from 1 to 10].

Are you of Hispanic, Latino, or Spanish origin?

Yes

No

With which racial group do you primarily identify?

White/Caucasian.

African American.

Asian.

Native American.

Other.

What is your gender?

Male.

Female.

Other.

What is the highest degree that you have earned?

Less than high school.

High school graduate.

Some college.

two year degree.

four year degree.

Professional degree.

Doctorate.

What is your age?

18–30 years old.

31–45 years old.

46–55 years old.

56–65 years old.

Over the age of 65 years old.

A.3 Section 3. Details for how each control variable was coded

Gender: 1, female; 0, male.

Race: 1, white; 0, non-white.

Age: 1, 18–30; 2, 31–45; 3, 46–55; 4, 56–65; 7, Over 65.

Education: 1, less than high school; 2, high school graduate; 3, some college; 4, 2-year degree; 5, 4-year degree; 6, professional degree; 7, doctorate.

Democrat: 1, Democrat; 0, Republican.

A.4 Section 4. Regression tables

Table A.4.1. Model for testing determinants of blame attributed to President Trump

Note. All tests of significance are two-tailed.

Table A.4.2. Model for testing determinants of blame attributed to governor

Note. All tests of significance are two-tailed.

Table A.4.3. Model for testing determinants of blame attributed to mayor

Author Biography

Rongbo Jin, Alexander Cloudt, and Seoungin Choi are doctoral candidates in the School of Government and Public Policy at the University of Arizona. Zhuofan Jia is a doctoral student in the School of Government and Public Policy at the University of Arizona. Samara Klar is an Associate Professor in the School of Government and Public Policy at the University of Arizona.

Footnotes

1 A team of two coders independently checked the accuracy of the respondents’ responses regarding partisanship of their leaders. 86.45% provided the accurate response. Our results do not change when we restrict our analyses only those who guessed accurately.

* p < 0.1;

** p < 0.05;

*** p < 0.01.

* p < 0.1;

** p < 0.05;

*** p < 0.01.

* p < 0.1;

** p < 0.05;

*** p < 0.01.

References

Arceneaux, Kevin, and Stein, Robert M.. 2006. “Who is Held Responsible When Disaster Strikes? The Attribution of Responsibility for a Natural Disaster in an Urban Election.” Journal of Urban Affairs 28 (1): 4353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arceneaux, Kevin, and Vander Wielen, Ryan J. 2017. Taming Intuition: How Reflection Minimizes Partisan Reasoning and Promotes Democratic Accountability. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Benedictis-Kessner, Justin, and Warshaw., Christopher 2016.“Mayoral Partisanship and Municipal Fiscal Policy.” Journal of Politics 78 (4): 1124–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bisgaard, Martin 2015. “Bias Will Find a Way: Economic Perceptions, Attributions of Blame, and Partisan-Motivated Reasoning During Crisis.” Journal of Politics 77 (3): 849–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bisgaard, Martin 2019. “How Getting the Facts Right Can Fuel Partisan-Motivated Reasoning.” American Journal of Political Science 63 (4): 824–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolsen, Toby, Druckman, James N., and Cook., Fay Lomax 2014. “The Influence of Partisan Motivated Reasoning on Public Opinion.” Political Behavior 36 (2): 235–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, Adam R. 2010. “Are Governors Responsible for the State Economy? Partisanship, Blame, and Divided Federalism.” The Journal of Politics 72 (3): 605–15.10.1017/S0022381610000046CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bullock, John G., Gerber, Alan S., Hill, Seth J., and Huber., Gregory A. 2013. “Partisan Bias in Factual Beliefs about Politics (No. w19080).” National Bureau of Economic Research.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, Seoungin, Cloudt, Alexander, Jia, Zhuofan, Jin, Rongbo, and Klar, Samara. 2022, “Replication Data for: The Policy Blame Game: How Polarization Distorts Democratic Accountability Across the Local, State, and Federal Level.” UNC Dataverse. V1. https://doi.org/10.15139/S3/5YQIWP.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Das, Sanmay, Sinclair, Betsy, Webster, Steven W., and Yan, Hao 2022. “All (Mayoral) Politics is Local?Journal of Politics. 84(2): 10211034.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennis, J. Michael, Chatt, Cindy, Li, Rick, Motta-Stanko, Alicia, and Pulliam., Paul 2005. Data Collection Mode Effects Controlling for Sample Origins in a Panel Survey: Telephone versus Internet. https://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.550.3088&rep=rep1&type=pdfGoogle Scholar
Dennis, J. Michael 2001. “Are Internet Panels Creating Professional Respondents? The Benefits of Online Panels Far Outweigh the Potential for Panel Effects.” Marketing Research Summer 13: 34–8.Google Scholar
Druckman, James N., and Kam., Cindy D. 2011. “Students as Experimental Participants: A Defense of the ‘Narrow Data Base.” In Cambridge Handbook of Experimental Political Science, eds. Druckman, James N., Green, Donald P., Kuklinski, James H. and Lupia, Arthur. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Druckman, James N., Klar, Samara, Krupnikov, Yanna, Levendusky, Mathew, and Ryan., John B. 2021. “Affective Polarization, Local Contexts and Public Opinion in America.” Nature Human Behaviour 5 (1): 2838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fearon, John. 1999. “Electoral Accountability and the Control of Politicians: Selecting Good Types versus Sanctioning Poor Performance.” In Democracy, Accountability, and Representation (Cambridge Studies in the Theory of Democracy), eds. Przeworski, Adam, Stokes, Susan C. and Manin, Bernard, 5597. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferejohn, John 1986. “Incumbent Performance and Electoral Control.” Public Choice 50 (1): 525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiorina, Morris P. 1981. Retrospective Voting in American National Elections. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Gaines, Brian J., Kuklinski, James H., Quirk, Paul J., Peyton, Buddy, and Verkuilen., Jay 2007. “Same Facts, Different Interpretations: Partisan Motivation and Opinion on Iraq.” The Journal of Politics 69 (4): 957–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerber, Elisabeth R., and Hopkins., Daniel J. 2011. “When Mayors Matter: Estimating the Impact of Mayoral Partisanship City Policy.” American Journal of Political Science 55 (2): 326–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Healy, Andrew, Kuo, Alexander, and Malhotra., Neil 2014. “Partisan Bias in Blame Attribution: When Does it Occur?Journal of Experimental Political Science 1(2): 144–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, Seth J. 2017. “Learning Together Slowly: Bayesian Learning about Political Facts.” The Journal of Politics 79 (4): 1403–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, Seth, Lo, James, Vavreck, Lynn, and Zaller, John. 2007. “The Opt-In Internet Panel: Survey Mode, Sampling Methodology and the Implications for Political Research.” http://jwolf-ra.wdfiles.com/local--files/mode-effects/Hill2007_Opt-InInternetPanels.pdf.Google Scholar
Iyengar, Shanto, Lelkes, Yphtach, Levendusky, Matthew, Malhotra, Neil, and Westwood, Sean J. 2019. “The Origins and Consequences of Affective Polarization in the United States.” Annual Review of Political Science 22: 129–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, Christopher D., Lavine, Howard G., and Federico., Christopher M. 2017. Open Versus Closed: Personality, Identity, and the Politics of Redistribution. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, Christopher D., Steenbergen, Marco R., and Lavine, Howard G. 2012. The Ambivalent Partisan: How Critical Loyalty Promotes Democracy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Joslyn, Mark R., and Haider-Markel., Donald P. 2014. Who Knows Best? Education, Partisanship, and Contested Facts. Politics & Policy 42 (6): 919–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kingzette, Jon, Druckman, James N., Klar, Samara, Krupnikov, Yanna, Levendusky, Matthew, and Ryan, John Barry. (2021). How Affective Polarization Undermines Support for Democratic Norms. PublicOpinion Quarterly, 85(2), 663677.Google Scholar
Klar, Samara, and McCoy, Alexandra (2021). Partisan-Motivated Evaluations of Sexual Misconduct and the Mitigating Role of the #MeToo Movement. American Journal of Political Science, 65(4), 777789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krupnikov, Yanna, and Ryan., John Barry Forthcoming. The Other Divide: Polarization and Disengagement among the American Public. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lewis-Beck, Michael S., Nadeau, Richard, and Elias., Angelo 2008. “Economics, Party, and the Vote: Causality Issues and Panel Data.” American Journal of Political Science 52 (1): 8495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lodge, Milton, and Taber., Charles 2000. “Three Steps Towards a Theory of Motivated Political Reasoning.” In Elements of Reason: Cognition, Choice, and the Bounds of Rationality, eds. Lupia, Arthur, McCubbins, Mathew D. and Popkin, Samuel L.. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lutz, Eric. 2021. “Republicans: You’re Welcome for the COVID Stimulus Money We Voted Against.” VanityFair.com (May 6, 2021). https://www.vanityfair.com/news/2021/05/republicans-youre-welcome-for-covid-stimulus-money-we-voted-againstGoogle Scholar
Lyons, Jeffrey, and Jaeger., William. P. 2014. “Who Do Voters Blame for Policy Failure? Information and the Partisan Assignment of Blame.” State Politics & Policy Quarterly 14 (3): 321–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malhotra, Neil, and Kuo., Alexander G. 2008. “Attributing Blame: The Public’s Response to Hurricane Katrina.” The Journal of Politics 70 (1): 120–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mummolo, Jonathan. 2016. “News From the Other Side: How Topic Relevance Limits the Prevalence of Partisan Selective Exposure.” The Journal of Politics 78 (3): 763–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prior, Markus, Sood, Gaurav, and Khanna., Kabir 2015. “You Cannot Be Serious: The Impact of Accuracy Incentives on Partisan Bias in Reports of Economic Perceptions.” Quarterly Journal of Political Science 10 (4): 489518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudolph, Thomas J., and Grant., J. Tobin 2002. “An Attributional Model of Economic Voting: Evidence From the 2000 Presidential Election.” Political Research Quarterly 55 (4): 805–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmitter, Philippe C., and Karl., Terry Lynn 1991. “What Democracy Is and Is Not.” Journal of Democracy 2 (3): 7588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shadish, William R., Cook, Thomas D., and Campbell., Donald T. 2002. Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Designs for Generalized Causal Inference. Houghton: Mifflin and Company.Google Scholar
Svolik, Milan W. 2013. “Learning to Love Democracy: Electoral Accountability and the Success of Democracy.” American Journal of Political Science 57 (3): 685702.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, Dennis F. 1980. “Moral Responsibility of Public Officials: The Problem of Many Hands.” American Political Science Review 74 (4): 905–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tilley, James, and Hobolt., Sara B. 2011. “Is the Government to Blame? An Experimental Test of How Partisanship Shapes Perceptions of Performance and Responsibility.” The Journal of Politics 73 (2): 316–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woon, Jonathan. 2012. “Democratic Accountability and Retrospective Voting: A Laboratory Experiment.” American Journal of Political Science 56 (4): 913–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Distribution of state residence among respondents.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Distribution of in-party bias (also known as affective polarization) among respondents.

Figure 2

Figure 3. Marginal effect of opposition to mask mandates (left panel) and business restrictions (right panel) on blaming in-party (solid line) versus out-party (dashed line) mayors.Note. Ninety-five percent confidence intervals are highlighted.

Figure 3

Figure 4. Marginal effect of opposition to mask mandates (left panel) and business restrictions (right panel) on blaming in-party (solid line) versus out-party (dashed line) governors.Note. Ninety-five percent confidence intervals are highlighted.

Figure 4

Figure 5. Marginal effect of opposition to mask mandates (left panel) and business restrictions (right panel) on blaming in-party (solid line) versus out-party (dashed line) president.Note. Ninety-five percent confidence intervals are highlighted.

Figure 5

Figure 6. Marginal effect of in-party bias on blaming in-party (solid line) versus out-party (dashed line) mayors as opposition to mask policy increases (left panel) and opposition to business restrictions increases (right panel).Note. Zero line is darkened and 95% confidence intervals are highlighted.

Figure 6

Figure 7. Marginal effect of in-party bias on blaming in-party (solid line) versus out-party (dashed line) governors as opposition to mask policy increases (left panel) and opposition to business restrictions increases (right panel).Note. Zero line is darkened and 95% confidence intervals are highlighted.

Figure 7

Figure 8. Marginal effect of in-party bias on blaming in-party (solid line) versus out-party (dashed line) president as opposition to mask policy increases (left panel) and opposition to business restrictions increases (right panel).Note. Zero line is darkened and 95% confidence intervals are highlighted.

Figure 8

Table A.4.1. Model for testing determinants of blame attributed to President Trump

Figure 9

Table A.4.2. Model for testing determinants of blame attributed to governor

Figure 10

Table A.4.3. Model for testing determinants of blame attributed to mayor

Supplementary material: Link

Jin et al. Dataset

Link