Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T20:24:34.141Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The wolf will live with the lamb

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 March 2017

Richard Ronay
Affiliation:
Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, 1081 BT Amsterdam, The Netherlands. [email protected]@vu.nlhttp://www.richardronay.comhttp://www.joshtybur.com
Joshua M. Tybur
Affiliation:
Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, 1081 BT Amsterdam, The Netherlands. [email protected]@vu.nlhttp://www.richardronay.comhttp://www.joshtybur.com

Abstract

Maestripieri et al. pit evolutionary psychology against social psychological and economic perspectives in a winner-take-all empirical battle. In doing so, they risk positioning evolutionary psychology as an antagonistic subdisciplinary enterprise. We worry that such a framing may exacerbate tensions between “competing” scientific perspectives and limit evolutionary psychology's potential to serve as a unifying core theory.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Buss, D. M. (1995) Evolutionary psychology: A new paradigm for psychological science. Psychological Inquiry 6:130.Google Scholar
Inbar, Y. & Lammers, J. (2012) Political diversity in social and personality psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science 7:496503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jensen-Campbell, L. A., Graziano, W. G. & West, S. G. (1995) Dominance, prosocial orientation, and female preferences: Do nice guys really finish last? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 68:427–40.Google Scholar
Kelley, H. H. (2000) The proper study of social psychology. Social Psychology Quarterly 63:315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurzban, R. & Leary, M. R. (2001) Evolutionary origins of stigmatization: The functions of social exclusion. Psychological Bulletin 127:187208.Google Scholar
Park, J. H. (2007) Persistent misunderstandings of inclusive fitness and kin selection: Their ubiquitous appearance in social psychology textbooks. Evolutionary Psychology 5:860–73.Google Scholar
Sell, A., Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (2009) Formidability and the logic of human anger. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106(35):15073–78.Google Scholar
Shook, N. J., Terrizzi, J. A., Clay, R. & Oosterhoff, B. (2015) In defense of pathogen disgust and disease avoidance: A response to Tybur et al. (2015). Evolution and Human Behavior 36:498502.Google Scholar
Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (1992) The psychological foundations of culture. In: The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture, ed. Barkow, J., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J., pp. 19136. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tybur, J. M., Inbar, Y., Güler, E. & Molho, C. (2015a) Is the relationship between pathogen avoidance and ideological conservatism explained by sexual strategies? Evolution and Human Behavior 36:489–97.Google Scholar
Tybur, J. M., Inbar, Y., Güler, E. & Molho, C. (2015b) Pathogen disgust requires no defense: A response to Shook, Terrizzi, Clay, & Oosterhoff (2015). Evolution and Human Behavior 36:502504.Google Scholar
Tybur, J. M., Miller, G. F. & Gangestad, S. W. (2007) Testing the controversy: An empirical investigation of adaptationists' attitudes toward politics and science. Human Nature 18:313–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar