Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-08T03:32:28.986Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Why do we remember? The communicative function of episodic memory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 January 2017

Johannes B. Mahr
Affiliation:
Department of Cognitive Science, Cognitive Development Center, Central European University, 1051 Budapest, [email protected]://cognitivescience.ceu.edu/people/johannes-mahr
Gergely Csibra
Affiliation:
Department of Cognitive Science, Cognitive Development Center, Central European University, 1051 Budapest, [email protected]://cognitivescience.ceu.edu/people/gergely-csibra

Abstract

Episodic memory has been analyzed in a number of different ways in both philosophy and psychology, and most controversy has centered on its self-referential, autonoetic character. Here, we offer a comprehensive characterization of episodic memory in representational terms and propose a novel functional account on this basis. We argue that episodic memory should be understood as a distinctive epistemic attitude taken toward an event simulation. In this view, episodic memory has a metarepresentational format and should not be equated with beliefs about the past. Instead, empirical findings suggest that the contents of human episodic memory are often constructed in the service of the explicit justification of such beliefs. Existing accounts of episodic memory function that have focused on explaining its constructive character through its role in future-oriented mental time travel do justice neither to its capacity to ground veridical beliefs about the past nor to its representational format. We provide an account of the metarepresentational structure of episodic memory in terms of its role in communicative interaction. The generative nature of recollection allows us to represent and communicate the reasons why we hold certain beliefs about the past. In this process, autonoesis corresponds to the capacity to determine when and how to assert epistemic authority in making claims about the past. A domain where such claims are indispensable are human social engagements. Such engagements commonly require the justification of entitlements and obligations, which is often possible only by explicit reference to specific past events.

Type
Target Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Addis, D. R., Pan, L., Vu, M. A., Laiser, N. & Schacter, D. L. (2009) Constructive episodic simulation of the future and the past: Distinct subsystems of a core brain network mediate imagining and remembering. Neuropsychologia 47(11):2222–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T. & Schacter, D. L. (2007) Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia 45(7):1363–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.016.Google Scholar
Aikhenvald, A. (2004) Evidentiality. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alba, J. W. & Hasher, W. L. (1983) Is memory schematic? Psychological Bulletin 93(2):203–31. Available at: http://psycnet.apa.org/index.cfm?fa=search.displayRecord&uid=1983-20271-001.Google Scholar
Allan, K., Midjord, P., Martin, D. & Gabbert, F. (2012) Memory conformity and the perceived accuracy of self versus other. Memory & Cognition 40:280–86. Available at: http://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/s13421-011-0141-9.Google Scholar
Anderson, M. C. & Hanslmayr, S. (2014) Neural mechanisms of motivated forgetting. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 18(6):279–92. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1364661314000746.Google Scholar
Asefi, S. L. & Garry, M. (2003) Absolut® memory distortions: Alcohol placebos influence the misinformation effect. Psychological Science 14(1):7780. Available at: http://pss.sagepub.com/content/14/1/77.short.Google Scholar
Atance, C. M. & O'Neill, D. K. (2001) Episodic future thinking. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5(12):533–39. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1364661300018040.Google Scholar
Audi, R. (1995) Memorial justification. Philosophical Topics 23(1):3145. Available at: http://www.jstor.org/stable/43154193.Google Scholar
Bartlett, F. C. (1932) Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bauer, P. J. & Leventon, J. S. (2013) Memory for onetime experiences in the second year of life: Implications for the status of episodic memory. Infancy 18(5):755–81. Available at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12005/abstract.Google Scholar
Bell, B. E. & Loftus, E. F. (1988) Degree of detail of eyewitness testimony and mock juror judgments. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 18(14):1171–92. Available at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1988.tb01200.x/full.Google Scholar
Bell, B. E. & Loftus, E. F. (1989) Trivial persuasion in the courtroom: The power of (a few) minor details. Attitudes and Social Cognition 56(5):669–79. Available at: http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/psp/56/5/669/.Google ScholarPubMed
Bell, R., Schain, C. & Echterhoff, G. (2014) How selfish is memory for cheaters? Evidence for moral and egoistic biases. Cognition 132:437–42. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027714000845.Google Scholar
Benney, K. S. & Henkel, L. A. (2006) The role of free choice in memory for past decisions. Memory 14(8):1001–11. Available at: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/09658210601046163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blank, H. (2009) Remembering: A theoretical interface between memory and social psychology. Social Psychology 40(3):164–75. Available at: http://econtent.hogrefe.com/doi/abs/10.1027/1864-9335.40.3.164.Google Scholar
Blank, H. & Launay, C. (2014) How to protect eyewitness memory against the misinformation effect: A meta-analysis of post-warning studies. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 3:7788. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S2211368114000230.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (2008) Evolutionary economics of mental time travel? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12(6):219–24. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.03.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyer, P. (2009) What are memories for? Functions of recall in cognition and culture. In: Memory in mind and culture, ed. Boyer, P. & Wertsch, J. V., pp. 328. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandom, R. (1983) Asserting. Nous 17(4):637–50. Available at: https://www.jstor.org/stable/2215086.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bregman, N. J. & McAllister, H. A. (1982) Eyewitness testimony: The role of commitment in increasing reliability. Social Psychology Quarterly 45(3):181–84. Available at: http://www.jstor.org/stable/3033652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bright-Paul, A., Jarrold, C. & Wright, D. B. (2005) Age-appropriate cues facilitate source-monitoring and reduce suggestibility in 3- to 7-year-olds. Cognitive Development 20:118. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201404000577.Google Scholar
Brosnan, S. F. & de Waal, F. B. (2002) A proximate perspective on reciprocal altruism. Human Nature 13(1):129–52. Available at: http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s12110-002-1017-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckner, R. L. & Carroll, D. C. (2006) Self-projection and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Science 11(2):4957. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1364661306003275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burge, T. (1993) Content preservation. The Philosophical Review 102(4):457–88. Available at: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2185680.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burns, P., Russell, C. & Russell, J. (2015) Pre-school children's proto-episodic memory assessed by deferred imitation. Memory 23(8):1172–92. Available at: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/09658211.2014.963625.Google Scholar
Castelain, T., Bernard, S., Van der Henst, J. & Mercier, H. (2015) The influence of power and reason on young Maya children's endorsement of testimony. Developmental Science 19(6)957–66. Available at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/desc.12336/abstract.Google Scholar
Cheng, S., Werning, M. & Suddendorf, T. (2016) Dissociating memory traces and scenario construction in mental time travel. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 60:8289. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.11.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark-Foos, A., Brewer, G. & Marsh, R. L. (2015) Judging the reality of others' memories. Memory 23(3):427–36. Available at: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/09658211.2014.893364.Google Scholar
Clayton, N. S. & Dickinson, A. (1998) Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays. Nature 395(6699):272–74. Available at: http://www.nature.com/nature/journal/v395/n6699/abs/395272a0.html.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clayton, N. S. & Russell, (2009) Looking for episodic memory in animals and young children: Prospects for a new minimalism. Neuropsychologia 47(11):2330–40. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0028393208004132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clément, F., Koenig, M. & Harris, P. (2004) The ontogenesis of trust. Mind & Language 19(4):360–79. Available at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.0268-1064.2004.00263.x/abstract.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clifasefi, S. L., Garry, M., Harper, D. N., Sharman, S. J. & Sutherland, R. (2007) Psychotropic placebos create resistance to the misinformation effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 14(1):112–17. Available at: http://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/BF03194037.Google Scholar
Cochran, K. J., Greenspan, R. L., Bogart, D. F. & Loftus, E. F. (2016) Memory blindness: Altered memory reports lead to distortion in eyewitness testimony. Memory & Cognition 44(5):717–26. Available at: http://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/s13421-016-0594-y.Google Scholar
Coman, A., Stone, C. B., Castano, E. & Hirst, W. (2014) Justifying atrocities: The effect of moral-disengagement strategies on socially shared retrieval-induced forgetting. Psychological Science 25(6):1281–85. Available at: http://pss.sagepub.com/content/early/2014/04/16/0956797614531024.Google Scholar
Conway, M. A. (2005) Memory and the self. Journal of Memory and Language 53:594628. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0749596X05000987.Google Scholar
Corballis, M. C. (2013) Mental time travel: A case for evolutionary continuity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17(1):56. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1364661312002458.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (2000) Consider the source: The evolution of adaptations for decoupling and metarepresentation. In: Metarepresentations: A multidisciplinary perspective, ed. Sperber, D., pp. 53115. Vancouver Studies in Cognitive Science. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Craver, C. F., Cova, F., Green, L., Myerson, J., Rosenbaum, R. S., Kwan, D. & Bourgeois-Gironde, S. (2014a) An Allais paradox without mental time travel. Hippocampus 24:1375–80. Available at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/hipo.22318/full.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craver, C. F., Kwan, D., Steindam, C. & Rosenbaum, R. S. (2014b) Individuals with episodic amnesia are not stuck in time. Neuropsychologia 57:191–95. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0028393214000797.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crystal, J. D., Alford, W. T., Zhou, W. & Hohmann, A. G. (2013) Source memory in the rat. Current Biology 23:387–91. Available at: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23394830.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. & Krebs, J. R. (1978) Animal signals: Information or manipulation. In: Behavioral ecology: An evolutionary approach, ed. Krebs, J. R. & Davies, N. B., pp. 282309. Basil Blackwell Scientific.Google Scholar
De Brigard, F. (2012) Predictive memory and the surprising gap. Frontiers in Psychology 3:420. Available at: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3498899/.Google Scholar
De Brigard, F. (2014a) Is memory for remembering? Recollection as a form of episodic hypothetical thinking. Synthese 191(2):155–85. Available at: http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s11229-013-0247-7.Google Scholar
De Brigard, F. (2014b) The nature of memory traces. Philosophy Compass 9(6):402–14. Available at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/phc3.12133/abstract.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dessalles, J.-L. (2007a) Storing events to retell them (Commentary on Suddendorf and Corballis: “The evolution of foresight”). Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30(3):321–22. Available at: http://www.dessalles.fr/papers/Dessalles_07051403.pdf.Google Scholar
Dessalles, J.-L. (2007b) Why we talk: The evolutionary origins of language. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dew, I. T. Z. & Cabeza, R. (2011) The porous boundaries between explicit and implicit memory: Behavioral and neural evidence. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1224(1):174–90. Available at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05946.x/full.Google Scholar
Dodd, D. H. & Bradshaw, J. M. (1980) Leading questions and memory: Pragmatic constraints. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 19:695704. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022537180903795.Google Scholar
Dokic, J. (2001) Is memory purely preservative? In: Time and memory: Issues in philosophy and psychology, ed. Hoerl, C. & McCormack, T., pp. 213–32. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Drummey, A. B. & Newcombe, N. S. (2002) Developmental changes in source memory. Developmental Science 5(4):502–13. Available at: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21883163.Google Scholar
Eagly, A. H., Kulesa, P., Chen, S. & Chaiken, S. (2001) Do attitudes affect memory? Tests of the congeniality hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science 10(1):59. Available at: http://cdp.sagepub.com/content/10/1/5.short.Google Scholar
Echterhoff, G., Higgins, T. E., Kopietz, R. & Groll, S. (2008) How communication goals determine when audience tuning biases memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 137(1):321. Available at: http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/xge/137/1/3/.Google Scholar
Echterhoff, G., Hirst, W. & Hussy, W. (2005) How eye-witnesses resist misinformation: Social postwarnings and the monitoring of memory characteristics. Memory & Cognition 33(5):770–82. Available at: http://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/BF03193073.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Echterhoff, G., Lang, S., Krämer, N. & Higgins, E. T. (2009b) Audience-tuning effects on memory: The role of audience status in sharing reality. Social Psychology 40(3):150–63. Available at: http://econtent.hogrefe.com/doi/abs/10.1027/1864-9335.40.3.150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Firestone, C. & Scholl, B. J. (2016) Cognition does not affect perception: Evaluating the evidence for so-called “top-down” effects. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39:e229. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X15000965. Online ahead of publication.Google Scholar
Fitzgerald, J. M. & Broadbridge, C. L. (2013) Latent constructs of the autobiographical memory questionnaire: A recollection-belief model of autobiographical experience. Memory 21(2):230–48. Available at: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/09658211.2012.725736.Google Scholar
Fivush, R. & Bauer, P. J. (2010) The emergence of recollection: How we learn to recall ourselves in the past. In: The act of remembering: Toward an understanding of how we recall the past, ed., Mace, J. H., pp. 259–84. Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
French, L., Garry, M. & Mori, K. (2011) Relative – not absolute – judgments of credibility affect susceptibility to misinformation conveyed during discussion. Acta Psychologica 136:119–28. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0001691810002155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabbert, F., Memon, A. & Wright, D. B. (2007) I saw it for longer than you: The relationship between perceived encoding duration and memory conformity. Acta Psychologica 124:319–31. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0001691806000515.Google Scholar
Gardiner, J. M. (2001) Episodic memory and autonoetic consciousness: A first-person approach. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 356:1351–61. Available at: http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/356/1413/1351.short.Google Scholar
Giles, J. W., Gopnik, A. & Heyman, G. D. (2002) Source monitoring reduces the suggestibility of preschool children. Psychological Science 13(3):288–91. Available at: http://pss.sagepub.com/content/13/3/288.short.Google Scholar
Goethals, G. R. & Reckman, R. F. (1973) The perception of consistency in attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 9:491501. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/0022103173900309.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. & Graf, P. (1988) Knowing how you know: Young children's ability to identify and remember the sources of their beliefs. Child Development 59(5):1366–71. Available at: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1130499.Google Scholar
Greene, E. (1981) Whodunit? Memory for evidence in text. American Journal of Psychology 94(3):479–96. Available at: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1422258.Google Scholar
Gupta, A. S., van der Meer, M. A. A., Touretzky, D. S. & Redish, A. D. (2010) Hippocampal replay is not a simple function of experience. Neuron 65:695705. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0896627310000607.Google Scholar
Haigh, S. N. & Robinson, E. J. (2009) What children know about the source of their knowledge without reporting it as the source. European Journal of Developmental Psychology 6(3):318–36. Available at: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/17405620601183569?src=recsys.Google Scholar
Hall, L., Johansson, P. & Strandberg, T. (2012) Lifting the veil of morality: Choice blindness and attitude reversals on a self-transforming survey. PLoS ONE 7(9):e45457. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0045457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hannula, D. E., Baym, C. L., Warren, D. E. & Cohen, N. J. (2012) The eyes know: Eye movements as a veridical index of memory. Psychological Science 23(3):278–87. Available at: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3917553/.Google Scholar
Hannula, D. E. & Greene, A. J. (2012) The hippocampus reevaluated in unconscious learning and memory: At a tipping point? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6:80. Available at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00080/full.Google Scholar
Hannula, D. E. & Ranganath, C. (2008) The eyes have it: Hippocampal activity predicts expression of memory in eye movements. Neuron 63:592–99. Available at: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19755103.Google Scholar
Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D. & Maguire, E. A. (2007a) Using imagination to understand the neural basis of episodic memory. The Journal of Neuroscience 27(52):14365–74. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4549-07.2007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D., Vann, S. D. & Maguire, E. A. (2007b) Patients with hippocampal amnesia cannot imagine new experiences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 104(5):1726–31. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0610561104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hassabis, D. & Maguire, E. A. (2007) Deconstructing episodic memory with construction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11(7):299306. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1364661307001258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hassabis, D. & Maguire, E. A. (2009) The construction system of the brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 364(1521):1263–271. Available at: http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/364/1521/1263.short.Google Scholar
Hayne, H. & Jack, F. (2011) Childhood amnesia. WIREs Cognitive Science 2:136–45. Available at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/wcs.107/abstract.Google Scholar
Hemmer, P. & Steyvers, M. (2009) A Bayesian account of reconstructive memory. Topics in Cognitive Science 1:189202. Available at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2008.01010.x/full.Google Scholar
Henke, K. (2010) A model for memory systems based on processing modes rather than consciousness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11(7):523–32. Available at: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20531422.Google Scholar
Henkel, L. A. & Mather, M. (2007) Memory attributions for choices: How beliefs shape our memories. Journal of Memory and Language 57(2):163–76. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0749596X06001185.Google Scholar
Higgins, E. T. & Rholes, W. S. (1978) “Saying is believing”: Effects of message modification on memory and liking for the person described. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 14:363–78. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/002210317890032X.Google Scholar
Hills, T. T. & Butterfill, S. (2015) From foraging to autonoetic consciousness: The primal self as a consequence of embodied prospective foraging. Current Zoology 61(2):368–81. Available at: http://cz.oxfordjournals.org/content/61/2/368.abstract.Google Scholar
Irish, M., Addis, D. R., Hodges, J. R. & Piguet, O. (2012a) Considering the role of semantic memory in episodic future thinking: Evidence from semantic dementia. Brain 135(Pt 7):2178–91. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/aws119.Google Scholar
Jaeger, A., Lauris, P., Selmeczy, D. & Dobbins, I. G. (2012) The costs and benefits of memory conformity. Memory & Cognition 40:101–12. Available at: http://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/s13421-011-0130-z.Google Scholar
Johansson, P., Hall, L., Sikström, S. & Olsson, A. (2005) Failure to detect mismatches between intention and outcome in a simple decision task. Science 310:116–19. Available at: http://science.sciencemag.org/content/310/5745/116.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. K. (1991) Reality monitoring: Evidence from confabulation in organic brain disease patients. In: Awareness of deficit after brain injury: Clinical and theoretical issues, ed. Prigatano, G. P. & Schacter, D. L., pp. 176–97. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. K. (2005) The relation between source memory and episodic memory: Comment on Siedlicki et al. (2005). Psychology and Aging 20(3):529–31. Available at: http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/pag/20/3/529/.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. K., Bush, J. G. & Mitchell, K. J. (1998) Interpersonal reality monitoring: Judging the sources of other people's memories. Social Cognition 16(2):199224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S. & Lindsay, D. S. (1993) Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin 114(1):328. Available at: http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/bul/114/1/3/.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. K. & Raye, C. L. (1981) Reality monitoring. Psychological Review 88(1):6785. Available at: http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/rev/88/1/67/.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. K. & Raye, C. L. (1998) False memories and confabulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2(4):137–45. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1364661398011528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, M. K. & Suengas, A. G. (1989) Reality monitoring judgments of other people's memories. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 27(2):107110. Available at: http://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/BF03329910.Google Scholar
Kappes, A. & Crockett, M. J. (2016) The benefits and costs of a rose-colored hindsight. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20(9):644–46. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1364661316300808.Google Scholar
Keven, N. (2016b) Events, narratives and memory. Synthese 193(8):2497–517. Available at: http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s11229-015-0862-6.Google Scholar
Király, I., Oláh, K., Kovács, A. & Csibra, G. (in preparation) The time-course of belief computation: Can 18- and 36-month-olds revise attributed mental states by episodic recomputation? Google Scholar
Klein, S. B. (2013a) The complex act of projecting oneself into the future. WIREs Cognitive Science 4:6379. Available at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/wcs.1210/full.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, S. B. (2013b) Making the case that episodic recollection is attributable to operations occurring at retrieval rather than to content stored in a dedicated subsystem of long-term memory. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 7:114. Available at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00003/full.Google Scholar
Klein, S. B. (2014) Autonoesis and belief in a personal past: An evolutionary theory of memory indices. Review of Philosophy and Psychology 5:417–47. Available at: http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s13164-014-0181-8.Google Scholar
Klein, S. B. (2015b) What memory is. WIRES Cognitive Science 6(1):138. Available at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/wcs.1333/full.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, S. B., Cosmides, L., Gangi, C. E., Jackson, B., Tooby, J. & Costable, K. A. (2009) Evolution and episodic memory: An analysis and demonstration of a social function of episodic recollection. Social Cognition 27(2):283319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, S. B., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J. & Chance, S. (2002a) Decisions and the evolution of memory: Multiple systems, multiple functions. Psychological Review 109(2):306–29. Available at: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11990320.Google Scholar
Klein, S. B. & Gangi, C. E. (2010) The multiplicity of self: Neuropsychological evidence and its implications for the self as a construct in psychological research. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1191:115. Available at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05441.x/full.Google Scholar
Klein, S. B., German, T. B., Cosmides, L. & Gabriel, R. (2004) A theory of autobiographical memory: Necessary components and disorders resulting from their loss. Social Cognition 22(5):460–90.Google Scholar
Klein, S. B., Loftus, E. & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1996) Self-knowledge of an amnesic patient: Toward a neuropsychology of personality and social psychology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 125(3):250–60. Available at: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8751820.Google Scholar
Klein, S. B., Loftus, J. & Kihlstrom, J. F. (2002b) Memory and temporal experience: The effects of episodic memory loss on an amnesic patient's ability to remember the past and imagine the future. Social Cognition 20(5):353–79. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.20.5.353.21125.Google Scholar
Klein, S. B. & Markowitsch, H. J. (2015) The nature of the semantic/episodic distinction: A missing piece of the “working through” process. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 38:e9. Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26050700.Google Scholar
Klein, S. B. & Nichols, S. (2012) Memory and the sense of personal identity. Mind 121(483):677702. Available at: http://mind.oxfordjournals.org/content/early/2012/10/30/mind.fzs080.short.Google Scholar
Kopietz, R., Echterhoff, G., Niemeier, S., Hellmann, J. H. & Memon, A. (2009) Audience-congruent biases in eyewitness memory and judgment: Influences of a co-witness' liking for a suspect. Social Psychology 40(3):138–49. Available at: http://econtent.hogrefe.com/doi/abs/10.1027/1864-9335.40.3.138.Google Scholar
Koriat, A. & Goldsmith, M. (1996a) Memory metaphors and the real-life/laboratory controversy: Correspondence versus storehouse conceptions of memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19:167228. Available at: https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/behavioral-and-brain-sciences/article/memory-metaphors-and-the-real-lifelaboratory-controversy-correspondence-versus-storehouse-conceptions-of-memory/5C2A5F74C9D820A4FEB516DAA1E2E358.Google Scholar
Kornblith, H. (2012) On reflection. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kouchaki, M. & Gino, F. (2016) Memories of unethical actions become obfuscated over time. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 113(22):6166–71. Available at: http://www.pnas.org/content/113/22/6166.short.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kraut, R. E. (1978) Verbal and nonverbal cues in the perception of lying. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 36(4):380–91. Available at: http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/psp/36/4/380/.Google Scholar
Krebs, J. R. & Dawkins, R. (1984) Animal signals: Mind-reading and manipulation. In: Behavioral ecology: An evolutionary approach, 2nd edition, ed. Krebs, J. R. & Davies, N. B., pp. 380402. Blackwell Science.Google Scholar
Kwan, D., Craver, C. F., Green, L., Myerson, J., Boyer, P. & Rosenbaum, R. S. (2012) Future decision-making without episodic mental time travel. Hippocampus 22(6):1215–19. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.20981.Google Scholar
Kwong See, S. T., Wood, T. L. & Hoffman, H. G. (2001) Perceptions of an old female eyewitness: Is the older eyewitness believable? Psychology and Aging 16(2):346–50. Available at: http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/pag/16/2/346/.Google Scholar
Lampinen, J. M., Neuschatz, J. S. & Payne, D. G. (1997) Memory illusions and consciousness: Examining the phenomenology of true and false memories. Current Psychology 16(3):181224. Available at: http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s12144-997-1000-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lampinen, J. M. & Smith, V. L. (1995) The incredible (and sometimes incredulous) child witness: Child eyewitnesses' sensitivity to source credibility cues. Journal of Applied Psychology 80(5):621–27. Available at: http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/apl/80/5/621/.Google Scholar
Leding, J. K. (2012) False memories and persuasion strategies. Review of General Psychology 16(3):256–68. Available at: http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/gpr/16/3/256/.Google Scholar
Levine, L. J. (1997) Reconstructing memory for emotions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 126(2):165–77. Available at: http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/xge/126/2/165/.Google Scholar
Lindsay, D. S. & Johnson, M. K. (1989) The eyewitness suggestibility effect and memory for source. Memory & Cognition 17(3):349–58. Available at: http://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/BF03198473.Google Scholar
Loftus, E. (1977) Shifting human color memory. Memory & Cognition 5(6):696–99. Available at: http://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/BF03197418.Google Scholar
Loftus, E. (2005) Planting misinformation in the human mind: A 30-year investigation of the malleability of memory. Learning & Memory 12(4):361–66. Available at: http://learnmem.cshlp.org/content/12/4/361.short.Google Scholar
Ludmer, R., Edelson, M. G. & Dudai, Y. (2015) The naïve and the distrustful: State dependency of hippocampal computations in manipulative memory distortion. Hippocampus 25:240–52. Available at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/hipo.22369/full.Google Scholar
Maguire, E. A., Intraub, H. & Mullally, S. L. (2015) Scenes, spaces and memory traces: What does the hippocampus do? The Neuroscientist 22(5):432–39. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858415600389..Google Scholar
Maguire, E. A. & Mullally, S. L. (2013) The hippocampus: A manifesto for change. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 142(4):1180–89. Available at: http://psycnet.apa.org/psycinfo/2013-25332-001.Google Scholar
Maguire, E. A., Vargha-Khadem, F. & Hassabis, D. (2010) Imagining fictitious experiences: Evidence from developmental amnesia. Neuropsychologia 48:3187–92. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0028393210002885.Google Scholar
Marsh, E. J. (2007) Retelling is not the same as recalling. Current Directions in Psychological Science 16(1):1620. Available at: http://cdp.sagepub.com/content/16/1/16.short.Google Scholar
Martin-Ordas, G., Berntsen, D. & Call, J. (2013) Memory for distant past events in chimpanzees and orangutans. Current Biology 23:1438–41. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0960982213007082.Google Scholar
Martin-Ordas, G., Haun, D., Colmenares, F. & Call, J. (2010) Keeping track of time: Evidence for episodic-like memory in great apes. Animal Cognition 13:331–40. Available at: http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s10071-009-0282-4.Google Scholar
Mascaro, O. & Sperber, D. (2009) The moral, epistemic, and mindreading components of children's vigilance towards deception. Cognition 112:367–80. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027709001152.Google Scholar
Mather, M. & Johnson, M. K. (2000) Choice-supportive source monitoring: Do our decisions seem better to us as we age? Psychology and Aging 15(4):596606. Available at: http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/pag/15/4/596/.Google Scholar
Mather, M., Shafir, E. & Johnson, M. K. (2000) Misremembrance of options past: Source monitoring and choice. Psychological Science 11(2):132–38. Available at: http://pss.sagepub.com/content/11/2/132.short.Google Scholar
Mather, M., Shafir, E. & Johnson, M. K. (2003) Remembering chosen and assigned options. Memory & Cognition 31(3):422–33. Available at: http://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/BF03194400.Google Scholar
Matthen, M. (2010) Is memory preservation? Philosophical Studies 148:314. Available at: http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s11098-010-9501-8.Google Scholar
Mazzoni, G. & Kirsch, I. (2002) Autobiographical memories and beliefs: A preliminary metacognitive model. In: Applied metacognition, ed. Perfect, T. J. & Schwartz, B. L., pp. 121–45. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McMyler, B. (2007) Knowledge at second hand. Inquiry 50(5):511–40. Available at: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/00201740701612390.Google Scholar
Mercier, H. (2016) The argumentative theory: Predictions and empirical evidence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20(9):689700. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1364661316300973.Google Scholar
Mercier, H. & Sperber, D. (2011) Why do humans reason? Arguments for an argumentative theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34(2):5774. Available at: https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/behavioral-and-brain-sciences/article/why-do-humans-reason-arguments-for-an-argumentative-theory/53E3F3180014E80E8BE9FB7A2DD44049.Google Scholar
Mercier, H. & Sperber, D. (2017) The enigma of reason. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Merckelbach, H., Jelicic, M. & Pieters, M. (2010) The residual effect of feigning: How intentional faking may evolve into a less conscious form of symptom reporting. Journal of Experimental and Clinical Neuropsychology 33(1):131–39. Available at: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/13803395.2010.495055.Google Scholar
Merckelbach, H., Jelicic, M. & Pieters, M. (2011) Misinformation increase symptom reporting: A test-retest study. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine Short Reports 2(75):16. Available at: http://shr.sagepub.com/content/2/10/75.short.Google Scholar
Michaelian, K. (2011a) Generative memory. Philosophical Psychology 24(3):323–42. Available at: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/09515089.2011.559623.Google Scholar
Michaelian, K. (2012a) Metacognition and endorsement. Mind & Language 27(3):284307. Available at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1468-0017.2012.01445.x/full.Google Scholar
Michaelian, K. (2012b) (Social) metacognition and (self-)trust. Review of Philosophy and Psychology 3:481514. Available at: http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s13164-012-0099-y.Google Scholar
Michaelian, K. (2015) Opening the doors of memory: Is declarative memory a natural kind? WIRES Cognitive Science 6:475–82. Available at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/wcs.1364/full.Google Scholar
Michaelian, K. (2016a) Confabulating, misremembering, relearning: The simulation theory of memory and unsuccessful remembering. Frontiers in Psychology 7:1857. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01857.Google Scholar
Michaelian, K. (2016b) Mental time travel: Episodic memory and our knowledge of the personal past. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Millikan, R. G. (1984) Language, thought and other biological categories: New foundations for realism. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Moscovitch, M. (2008) The hippocampus as a “stupid,” domain-specific module: Implications for theories of recent and remote memory, and of imagination. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 62(1):6279. Available at: http://psycnet.apa.org/index.cfm?fa=buy.optionToBuy&uid=2008-05131-008.Google Scholar
Moser, E. I., Kropff, E. & Moser, M. (2008) Place cells, grid cells and the brain's spatial representation system. Annual Review of Neuroscience 31:6989. Available at: http://www.annualreviews.org/eprint/7t2VcSrTYa8V8yACMweG/full/10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.061307.090723?select23=Choose&&.Google Scholar
Mullally, S. L. & Maguire, E. A. (2014) Learning to remember: The early ontogeny of episodic memory. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 9:1229. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1878929314000024.Google Scholar
Nagel, J. (2015) The social value of reasoning in epistemic justification. Episteme 12(2):297308. Available at: https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/episteme/article/the-social-value-of-reasoning-in-epistemic-justification/01BFE98E16528B9EB6E55E7A2670BEE7.Google Scholar
Nagy, D. G. & Orban, G. (2016) Episodic memory as a prerequisite for online updating of model structure. In: Proceedings of the 38th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. Papafragou, A., Grodner, D., Mirman, D. & Trueswell, J. C., pp. 2699–704. Cognitive Science Society. Available at: https://mindmodeling.org/cogsci2016/papers/0465/index.html.Google Scholar
Nash, R. A., Wheeler, R. L. & Hope, L. (2015) On the persuadability of memory: Is changing people's memories no more than changing their minds? British Journal of Psychology 106(2):308–26. Available at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/bjop.12074/full.Google Scholar
Neisser, U. (1981) John Dean's memory: A case study. Cognition 9(1):122. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/0010027781900111.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1993) The psychological and social origins of autobiographical memory. Psychological Science 4(1):714. Available at: http://pss.sagepub.com/content/4/1/7.short.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. & Fivush, R. (2004) The emergence of autobiographical memory: A social cultural developmental theory. Psychological Review 111(2):486511. Available at: http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/rev/111/2/486/.Google Scholar
Nysberg, L., Kim, A. S. N., Habib, R., Levine, B. & Tulving, E. (2010) Consciousness of subjective time in the brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 107(51):22356–59. Available at: http://www.pnas.org/content/107/51/22356.short.Google Scholar
Oeberst, A. & Blank, H. (2012) Undoing suggestive influence of memory: The reversibility of the eyewitness misinformation effect. Cognition 152(2):141–59. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S001002771200159X.Google Scholar
Okuda, J., Fujii, T., Ohtake, H., Tsukiura, T., Tanji, K., Suzuki, K., Kawashima, R., Fukuda, H., Itoh, M. & Yamadori, A. (2003) Thinking of the future and past: The role of the frontal pole and the medial temporal lobes. NeuroImage 19:1369–80. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053811903001794.Google Scholar
Olick, J. K. & Levy, D. (1997) Collective memory and cultural constraint: Holocaust myth and rationality in German politics. Sociological Review 62(6):921–36. Available at: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2657347.Google Scholar
Olsen, R. K., Moses, S. N., Riggs, L. & Ryan, J. D. (2012) The hippocampus supports multiple cognitive processes through relational binding and comparison. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6 (Article 146, 13 pp.). Available at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00146/full.Google Scholar
Pärnamets, P., Hall, L. & Johansson, P. (2015) Memory distortions resulting from a choice blindness task. In: Proceedings of the 37th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. Noelle, D. C., Dale, R., Warlaumont, A. S., Yoshimi, J., Matlock, T., Jennings, C. D. & Maglio, P. P., pp. 1823–28. Cognitive Science Society. Available at: https://mindmodeling.org/cogsci2015/papers/0316/index.html.Google Scholar
Pasupathi, M., Stallworth, L. M. & Murdoch, K. (1998) How what we tell becomes what we know: Listener effects on speakers' long-term memory for events. Discourse Processes 26(1):125. Available at: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/01638539809545035.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payne, D. G., Elie, C. J., Blackwell, J. M. & Neuschatz, J. S. (1996) Memory illusions: Recalling, recognizing, and recollecting events that never occurred. Journal of Memory and Language 35:261–85.Google Scholar
Perner, J. (1991) Understanding the representational mind. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Perner, J. (2001) Episodic memory: Essential distinctions and developmental implications. In: The self in time: Developmental perspectives, ed. Moore, C. & Lemmon, K., pp. 181202. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Perner, J. (2012) Mini-meta: In search for minimal criteria of metacognition. In: Foundations of metacognition, ed. Beran, M. J., Brandl, J. L., Perner, J. & Proust, J., pp. 94116. Oxford University Press. Available at: http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/view/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199646739.001.0001/acprof-9780199646739-chapter-006.Google Scholar
Perner, J., Kloo, D. & Stöttinger, E. (2007) Introspection & remembering. Synthese 159:253–70. Available at: http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s11229-007-9207-4.Google Scholar
Perner, J. & Ruffman, T. (1995) Episodic memory and autonoetic consciousness: Developmental evidence and a theory of childhood amnesia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 59:516–48. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022096585710247.Google Scholar
Picard, L., Mayor-Dubois, C., Maeder, P., Kalenzaga, S., Abram, M., Duval, C., Eustache, F., Roulet-Perez, E. & Piolino, P. (2013) Functional independence within the self-memory system: New insights from two cases of developmental amnesia. Cortex 49(6):1463–81. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S001094521200305X.Google Scholar
Pillemer, D. B., Steiner, K. L., Kuwabara, K. J., Kirkegaard Thomsen, D. & Svob, B. (2015) Vicarious memories. Consciousness and Cognition 36:233–45. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053810015001476.Google Scholar
Pool, R. (2008) Memory, history and the claims of the past. Memory Studies 1(2):149–66. Available at: http://mss.sagepub.com/content/1/2/149.short.Google Scholar
Redshaw, J. (2014) Does metarepresentation make human mental time travel unique? WIREs Cognitive Science 5:519–31. Available at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/wcs.1308/full.Google Scholar
Rimé, B., Mesquita, B., Boca, S. & Philippot, P. (1991) Beyond the emotional event: Six studies on the social sharing of emotion. Cognition & Emotion 5(5–6):435–65. Available at: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/02699939108411052.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robins, S. K. (2016a) Misremembering. Philosophical Psychology 29(3):432–47. Available at: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/09515089.2015.1113245.Google Scholar
Robins, S. K. (2016b) Representing the past: Memory traces and the causal theory of memory. Philosophical Studies 173(11):29933013. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11098-016-0647-x. Available at: http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s11098-016-0647-x.Google Scholar
Rodriguez, D. N. & Strange, D. (2015) False memories for dissonance inducing events. Memory 23(2):203–12. Available at: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/09658211.2014.881501.Google Scholar
Roediger, H. L. (1996) Memory illusions. Journal of Memory and Language 35:76100. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0749596X96900054.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roediger, H. L. (2001) Why retrieval is the key process in understanding human memory. In: Memory, consciousness and the brain: The Tallinn conference, ed. Tulving, E., pp. 5275. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Roediger, H. L. & McDermott, K. B. (1995) Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented on lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 21(4):803–14. Available at: http://psycnet.apa.org/psycinfo/1995-42833-001.Google Scholar
Ross, M. (1989) Relation of implicit theories to the construction of personal histories. Psychological Review 96(2):341–57. Available at: http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/rev/96/2/341/.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, M., McFarland, C., Conway, M. & Zanna, M. P. (1983) Reciprocal relation between attitudes and behavior recall. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 45(2):257–67. Available at: http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/psp/45/2/257/.Google Scholar
Ross, M., McFarland, C. & Fletcher, O. J. (1981) The effect of attitude on the recall of personal histories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 40(4):627–34. Available at: http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/psp/40/4/627/.Google Scholar
Rubin, D. C., Schrauf, R. W. & Greenberg, D. L. (2003) Belief and recollection of autobiographical memories. Memory & Cognition 32(6):887901. Available at: http://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/BF03196443.Google Scholar
Rubin, D. C. & Umanath, S. (2015) Event memory: A theory of memory for laboratory, autobiographical and fictional events. Psychological Review 122(1):123. Available at: http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/rev/122/1/1/.Google Scholar
Russell, J. (2014) Episodic memory as re-experiential memory: Kantian, developmental and neuroscientific currents. Review of Philosophy and Psychology 5(3):391411. Available at: http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s13164-014-0194-3.Google Scholar
Russell, J. & Hanna, R. (2012) A minimalist approach to the development of episodic memory. Mind and Language 27(1):2954. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.2011.01434.x.Google Scholar
Santioso, R., Fong, G. T. & Kunda, Z. (1990) Motivated recruitment of autobiographical memories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 59(2): 229–41.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L. (2001) The seven sins of memory: How the mind forgets and remembers. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L. & Addis, D. R. (2007) The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: Remembering the past and imagining the future. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 362:773–86. Available at: http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/362/1481/773.short.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L. & Addis, D. R. (2009) On the nature of medial temporal lobe contributions to the constructive simulation of future events. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 364:1245–53. Available at: http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/364/1521/1245.short.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R. & Buckner, R. L. (2007) Remembering the past to imagine the future: The prospective brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8(9):657–61. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2213.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., Hassabis, D., Martin, V. C., Spreng, R. N. & Szpunar, K. K. (2012) The future of memory: Remembering, imagining, and the brain. Neuron 76:677–94. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0896627312009919.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Guerin, S. A. & St. Jacques, P. (2011) Memory distortion: An adaptive perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15(10):467–74. Available at: http://www.cell.com/trends/cognitive-sciences/abstract/S1364-6613(11)00174-4.Google Scholar
Schelling, T. (1960) The strategy of conflict. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Schino, G. & Aureli, F. (2009) Reciprocal altruism in primates: Partner choice, cognition and emotions. Advances in the Study of Behavior 39:4569. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0065345409390026.Google Scholar
Schino, G. & Aureli, F. (2010) Primate reciprocity and its cognitive requirements. Evolutionary Anthropology 19:130–35. Available at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/evan.20270/full.Google Scholar
Schooler, J. W., Foster, R. A. & Loftus, E. F. (1988) Some deleterious consequences of the act of recollection. Memory & Cognition 16(3):243–51. Available at: http://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/BF03197757.Google Scholar
Scoboria, A., Jackson, D. L., Talarico, J., Hanczakowski, M., Wzsman, L. & Mazzoni, G. (2014) The role of belief in occurrence within autobiographical memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 143(3):1242–58. Available at: http://psycnet.apa.org/psycinfo/2013-29649-001/.Google Scholar
Sheldon, S. & Moscovitch, M. (2010) Recollective performance advantages for implicit memory tasks. Memory 18(7):681–97. Available at: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/09658211.2010.499876.Google Scholar
Speas, P. (2008) On the syntax and semantics of evidentials. Language and Linguistics Compass 2(5):940–65. Available at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2008.00069.x/full.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (2000) Metarepresentations in an evolutionary perspective. In: Metarepresentations: A multidisciplinary perspective, ed. Sperber, D., pp. 117137. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (2001) An evolutionary perspective on testimony and argumentation. Philosophical Topics 29:401–13. Available at: https://www.pdcnet.org//pdc/bvdb.nsf/purchase?openform&fp=philtopics&id=philtopics_2001_0029_0001_0401_0414&onlyautologin=true.Google Scholar
Sperber, D., Clemént, F., Heintz, C., Mascaro, O., Mercier, H., Origgi, G. & Wilson, D. (2010) Epistemic vigilance. Mind & Language 25(4):359–93. Available at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1468-0017.2010.01394.x/full.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. & Hirschfeld, L. A. (2004) The cognitive foundations of cultural stability and diversity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8(1):4046. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1364661303003140.Google Scholar
Spreng, R. N., Mar, R. A. & Kim, A. S. N. (2009) The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind and the default mode: A quantitative meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21(3):489510. Available at: http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/abs/10.1162/jocn.2008.21029.Google Scholar
Squire, L. R. (1992) Declarative and non-declarative memory: Multiple brain systems supporting learning and memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 4(3):232–43. Available at: http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/abs/10.1162/jocn.1992.4.3.232#.V-Fx5JN97m0.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T., Addis, D. R. & Corballis, M. C. (2009) Mental time travel and the shaping of the human mind. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 364(1521):1317–24. Available at: http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/364/1521/1317.short.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T. & Corballis, M. C. (1997) Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind. Genetic, Social & General Psychology Monographs 123(2):133–67. Available at: http://cogprints.org/725/.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T. & Corballis, M. C. (2007) The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30(3):299313.Google Scholar
Szpunar, K. K. (2010) Episodic future thought: An emerging concept. Perspective on Psychological Science 5:142–62. Available at: http://pps.sagepub.com/content/5/2/142.short.Google Scholar
Szpunar, K. K. & McDermott, K. B. (2008a) Episodic future thought and its relation to remembering: Evidence from ratings of subjective experience. Consciousness and Cognition 17:330–34. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053810007000311.Google Scholar
Templer, V. L. & Hampton, R. R. (2013) Episodic memory in nonhuman animals. Current Biology 23(17):R801R806. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0960982213008397.Google Scholar
Teroni, F. (2014) The epistemological disunity of memory. In: Mind, values and metaphysics: Philosophical papers dedicated to Kevin Mulligan, vol. 2, ed. Reboul, A., pp. 183202. Springer.Google Scholar
Terrier, N., Bernard, S., Mercier, H. & Clément, F. (2016) Visual access trumps gender in 3- and 4-year-old children's endorsement testimony. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 146:223–30. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022096516000308.Google Scholar
Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (1992) Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In: The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture, ed. Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J., pp. 163228. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1972) Episodic and semantic memory. In: Organization of memory, ed. Tulving, E. & Donaldson, W., pp. 381402. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1983a) Elements of episodic memory. Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1985) Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology 26:112. Available at: http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/cap/26/1/1/.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (2002a) Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology 53: 125. Available at: http://www.annualreviews.org/doi/abs/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (2005) Episodic memory and autonoesis: Uniquely human? In: The missing link in cognition: Origins of self-selective consciousness, ed. Terrace, H. S. & Metcalfe, J., pp. 356. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Turri, J. (2011) Promises to keep: Speech acts and the value of reflective knowledge. Logos & Episteme 2(4):583–90. Available at: https://www.pdcnet.org/logos-episteme/content/logos-episteme_2011_0002_0004_0583_0590.Google Scholar
Tversky, B. & Marsh, E. J. (2000) Biased retellings of events yield biased memories. Cognitive Psychology 40:138. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S001002859990720X.Google Scholar
von Hippel, W. & Trivers, R. (2011) The evolution and psychology of self-deception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34:156. Available at: https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/behavioral-and-brain-sciences/article/div-classtitlethe-evolution-and-psychology-of-self-deceptiondiv/B87968EC4A6B4DC93A21C217ABC13E13.Google Scholar
Vullioud, C., Clément, F., Scott-Phillips, T. & Mercier, H. (2017) Confidence as an expression of commitment: Why misplaced expressions of confidence backfire. Evolution and Human Behavior 38(1):917. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.06.002.Google Scholar
Waldum, E. R. & Sahakyan, L. (2012) Putting congeniality effects into context: Investigating the role of context in attitude memory using multiple paradigms. Journal of Memory and Language 66: 717–30. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0749596X12000022.Google Scholar
Weiss, M. (1997) Bereavement, commemoration and collective identity in contemporary Israeli society. Anthropological Quarterly 70(2):91101. Available at: http://www.jstor.org/stable/3317509.Google Scholar
Wheeler, M. E., Petersen, S. E. & Buckner, R. L. (2000) Memory's echo: Vivid remembering reactivates sensory-specific cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 97(20):11125–29. Available at: http://www.pnas.org/content/97/20/11125.full.Google Scholar
Wheeler, M. E., Stuss, D. T. & Tulving, E. (1997) Toward a theory of episodic memory: The frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness. Psychological Bulletin, 121(3): 331–54. Available at: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9136640.Google Scholar
Whitcombe, E. L. & Robinson, E. J. (2000) Children's decisions about what to believe and their ability to report the source of their belief. Cognitive Development 15:329–46. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201400000332.Google Scholar
Wimmer, E. L., Hogrefe, G. J. & Perner, J. (1988) Children's understanding of informational access as a source of knowledge. Child Development 59(2):386–96. Available at: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1130318.Google Scholar
Winter, J. (2001) The memory boom in contemporary historical studies. Raritan 21(1):52. Available at: http://raritanquarterly.rutgers.edu/node/6310.Google Scholar