No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Revising the null model in language evolution research
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 14 January 2025
Abstract
We comment on the consequences of the target article for language evolution research. We propose that the default assumption should be that of language-readiness in extinct hominins, and the integration of different types of available evidence from multiple disciplines should be used to assess the likely extent of the realization of this readiness. The role of archaeological evidence should be reconsidered.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Arbib, M. A. (2012). How the brain got language: The mirror system hypothesis. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berthet, M., Coye, C., Dezecache, G., & Kuhn, J. (2023). Animal linguistics: A primer. Biological Reviews, 98(1), 81–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botha, R. (2010). On the soundness of inferring modern language from symbolic behaviour. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 20(3), 345–356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botha, R. (2020). Neanderthal language: Demystifying the linguistic powers of our extinct cousins. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botha, R., & Everaert, M. (Eds.). (2013). The evolutionary emergence of language: Evidence and inference. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burkart, J., Martins, E. G., Miss, F., & Zürcher, Y. (2018). From sharing food to sharing information: Cooperative breeding and language evolution. Interaction Studies, 19(1–2), 136–150. https://doi.org/10.1075/is.17026.burCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chater, N., Reali, F., & Christiansen, M. H. (2009). Restrictions on biological adaptation in language evolution. PNAS, 106(4), 1015–1020. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0807191106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christiansen, M. H., & Kirby, S. (Eds.) (2003). Language evolution. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delliponti, A., Raia, R., Sanguedolce, G., Gutowski, A., Pleyer, M., Sibierska, M., … Wacewicz, S. (2023). Experimental semiotics: A systematic categorization of experimental studies on the bootstrapping of communication systems. Biosemiotics, 16(2), 291–310. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12304-023-09534-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W. T. (2010). The evolution of language. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackendoff, R. (2010). Your theory of language evolution depends on your theory of language. In Larson, R. K., Deprez, V., & Yamakido, H. (Eds.), The evolution of human language (pp. 63–72). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511817755.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johansson, S. (2005). Origins of language: Constraints on hypotheses. John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mithen, S. (2024). The language puzzle: How we talked our way out of the stone age. Profile Books.Google Scholar
Müller, T. F., & Raviv, L. (2024). Communication games: Social interaction in the formation of novel communication systems. OSF Preprints. https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/8acdzGoogle Scholar
Nölle, J., & Galantucci, B. (2023). Experimental semiotics: Past, present, and future. In Ibáñez, A. M. G. A. (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of semiosis and the brain (pp. 66–81), Routledge.Google Scholar
Pleyer, M., & Lindner, N. (2014). Constructions, construal and cooperation in the evolution of language. In Cartmill, E. A., Roberts, S., Lyn, H., & Cornish, H. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 10th conference (pp. 244–251). World scientific.Google Scholar
Roberts, G. (2017). The linguist's drosophila: Experiments in language change. Linguistics Vanguard, 3(1), 20160086.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, S. G., Killin, A., Deb, A., Sheard, C., Greenhill, S. J., Sinnemäki, K., … Jordan, F. (2020). CHIELD: The causal hypotheses in evolutionary linguistics database. Journal of Language Evolution, 5(2), 101–120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott-Phillips, T. C., & Kirby, S. (2010). Language evolution in the laboratory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(9), 411–417.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamariz, M. (2017). Experimental studies on the cultural evolution of language. Annual Review of Linguistics, 3, 389–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wacewicz, S., Żywiczyński, P., Hartmann, S., Pleyer, M., & Benitez-Burraco, A. (2020). Language in language evolution research: In defense of a pluralistic view. Biolinguistics, 14(SI), 59–101.Google Scholar
Zhang, E. Q., & Pleyer, M. (2024). Toward interdisciplinary integration in the study of comparative cognition: Insights from studying the evolution of multimodal communication. Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews, 19, 85–90. https://doi.org/10.3819/CCBR.2024.190017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Target article
Reconsidering the link between past material culture and cognition in light of contemporary hunter–gatherer material use
Related commentaries (21)
Advancing paleoanthropology beyond default nulls
All that glitters is not gold: The false-symbol problem in archaeology
Animal artefacts challenge archaeological standards for tracing human symbolic cognition
Archaeology retains a central role for studying the behavioral and cognitive evolution of our species and genus
Are we jingling modern hunter-gatherers and early Homo sapiens?
Behavioural modernity is dead: Long live behavioural modernity
Beyond the binary: Inferential challenges and solutions in cognitive archaeology
Cultural innovation is not only a product of cognition but also of cultural context
Don't ignore cognitive evolution during the three million years that preceded the archaeological record of material culture!
Inferences from absences
Material culture both reflects and causes human cognitive evolution
Negative priors and inferences from absence of evidence in cognitive and linguistic archaeology: Epistemically sound and scientifically strategic
Not just symbolism: Technologies may also have a less than direct connection with cognition
Perishable material choice indicates symbolic and representational capacities
Proposing the DN(C)-model of material evidence for well-calibrated claims about past cultures
Revising the null model in language evolution research
Shared intentionality may have been favored by persistence hunting in Homo erectus
Sports, team games, and physical skill competitions as an important source of symbolic material culture with low preservation probability
The cognitive and evolutionary science of behavioural modernity goes beyond material chronology
The Mbuti people still reproduce a 75,000 years old recursive pattern
What would be pre-modern human cognition?
Author response
Hominin cognition: The null hypothesis