Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-04T19:37:41.277Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Fearful apes, happy apes: Is fearfulness associated with uniquely human cooperation?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 May 2023

Christopher Riddell
Affiliation:
Cognitive Psychology Unit, Leiden University, 2333AK Leiden, the Netherlands [email protected]; https://www.universiteitleiden.nl/en/staffmembers/chris-riddell [email protected]; https://www.universiteitleiden.nl/en/staffmembers/mariska-kret [email protected]; https://www.universiteitleiden.nl/en/staffmembers/tonko-zijlstra
Mariska Kret
Affiliation:
Cognitive Psychology Unit, Leiden University, 2333AK Leiden, the Netherlands [email protected]; https://www.universiteitleiden.nl/en/staffmembers/chris-riddell [email protected]; https://www.universiteitleiden.nl/en/staffmembers/mariska-kret [email protected]; https://www.universiteitleiden.nl/en/staffmembers/tonko-zijlstra
Tonko Zijlstra
Affiliation:
Cognitive Psychology Unit, Leiden University, 2333AK Leiden, the Netherlands [email protected]; https://www.universiteitleiden.nl/en/staffmembers/chris-riddell [email protected]; https://www.universiteitleiden.nl/en/staffmembers/mariska-kret [email protected]; https://www.universiteitleiden.nl/en/staffmembers/tonko-zijlstra
Milica Nikolic
Affiliation:
Research Institute of Child Development and Education, University of Amsterdam, 1018 WS Amsterdam, The Netherlands [email protected]; https://www.uva.nl/en/profile/n/i/m.nikolic/m.nikolic.html

Abstract

In the fearful ape hypothesis, Grossmann argues that heightened fearfulness increases human-unique cooperation. We suggest that this conclusion, however, may be premature. In particular, we question Grossmann's singling out of fear as the affective trait that enhances cooperative care. Additionally, we problematize the extent to which heightened fearfulness in humans, and its association with human-unique cooperation, are supported empirically.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Grossmann's fearful ape hypothesis provides a novel and compelling account for the supposed link between heightened fearfulness and enhanced cooperation in humans. We appreciate the author's re-conceptualization of fear as a potentially adaptive response in some contexts, which in previous clinical works has largely been viewed as maladaptive. Despite its novelty, however, we argue that, at present, there is insufficient evidence to conclude that: (1) it is specifically heightened fearfulness (and not other affective traits or emotions in general) that enhances cooperation and (2) that heightened fearfulness is related to enhanced, “uniquely human,” cooperation.

Grossmann singles out fearfulness as the key affective trait that evolved to support “human-unique” levels of cooperation. He suggests that fearful infants evoke more (allo)parental care and, by receiving more care, become more cooperative children, adults, and parents themselves. We argue, however, that the influence of affective states on cooperative behaviour may not be bound to fearfulness alone. Other affective states may also be associated with – or even evoke more – cooperation.

Similarly to fear, from early in human ontogeny, infants experience and display positive affect (Messinger, Reference Messinger2002; Messinger & Fogel, Reference Messinger, Fogel and Kail2007) and can distinguish positive from other facial expressions (de Haan & Nelson, Reference de Haan, Nelson and A.1998). Furthermore, although results are mixed, there is some evidence that humans also display a perceptual sensitivity bias towards happy faces (Wirth & Wentura, Reference Wirth and Wentura2020; Zsido et al., Reference Zsido, Arato, Ihasz, Basler, Matuz-Budai, Inhof and Coelho2021). Crucially, although Grossmann and others failed to find an association between sensitivity to happy faces and infants' prosociality (Grossmann, Missana, & Krol, Reference Grossmann, Missana and Krol2018; Rajhans, Altvater-Mackensen, Vaish, & Grossmann, Reference Rajhans, Altvater-Mackensen, Vaish and Grossmann2016), there is robust evidence that experienced and displayed positive affect results in more cooperation in child and adult actors (e.g., Aknin, Van de Vondervoort, & Hamlin, Reference Aknin, Van de Vondervoort and Hamlin2018; Centorrino, Djemai, Hopfensitz, Milinski, & Seabright, Reference Centorrino, Djemai, Hopfensitz, Milinski and Seabright2015; Isen & Levin, Reference Isen and Levin1972; Kushlev, Radosic, & Diener, Reference Kushlev, Radosic and Diener2022; Moore, Underwood, & Rosenhan, Reference Moore, Underwood and Rosenhan1973; Rosenhan, Salovey, & Hargis, Reference Rosenhan, Salovey and Hargis1981), as well as observers (including in cooperative parental-care contexts) (e.g., Centorrino et al., Reference Centorrino, Djemai, Hopfensitz, Milinski and Seabright2015; Danvers & Shiota, Reference Danvers and Shiota2018; Lengua & Kovacs, Reference Lengua and Kovacs2005; Scharlemann, Eckel, Kacelnik, & Wilson, Reference Scharlemann, Eckel, Kacelnik and Wilson2001). Therefore, it is not only fearful infants, but also happy infants, who may evoke cooperation in others, and may, by receiving more care, themselves become more cooperative adults. The putative effect of fearfulness on cooperation is therefore not necessarily unique in this regard – and it may also be that happiness, or emotional expressivity more generally, foster cooperation.

Grossmann also argues that humans display and perceive more fearfulness in comparison to other great ape species, and that this heightened fearfulness evokes increased levels of “human-unique” cooperation (i.e., alloparental care). However, there is not enough evidence that: (1) fearfulness is heightened in humans compared to other great apes; and that (2) heightened fearfulness is associated with enhanced “human-unique” cooperation.

Although some studies found that great apes do not show attentional bias to fear specifically (Kret, Jaasma, Bionda, & Wijnen, Reference Kret, Jaasma, Bionda and Wijnen2016; Kret, Muramatsu, & Matsuzawa, Reference Kret, Muramatsu and Matsuzawa2018), other work has found heightened attention towards fear (Pritsch, Telkemeyer, Mühlenbeck, & Liebal, Reference Pritsch, Telkemeyer, Mühlenbeck and Liebal2017), and towards emotions in general (Wilson & Tomonaga, Reference Wilson and Tomonaga2018, Reference Wilson and Tomonaga2021; for a review, see Kret, Prochazkova, Sterck, & Clay, Reference Kret, Prochazkova, Sterck and Clay2020). Furthermore, the claim that human infants express more fearfulness than other great apes is based on one empirical study (Herrmann, Hare, Cissewski, & Tomasello, Reference Herrmann, Hare, Cissewski and Tomasello2011). This study, however, used a human-centred paradigm (sensitivity to novel humans and objects) to contrast the reactions of human children to adult great apes. Herrmann et al. (Reference Herrmann, Hare, Cissewski and Tomasello2011) assumed that showing an unfamiliar human is similarly relevant and meaningful for a captive adult ape (who is likely exposed to unfamiliar humans many times on a daily basis) and a human child (for whom this context may be drastically different). As such, this study might have or might have not captured the full extent of the fear response in other primates, calling to question Grossmann's claims about uniquely human heightened fear production.

Even if displaying and perceiving fear is, on average, heightened in humans, this does not mean that it necessarily evolved for “human-unique” cooperative care purposes (i.e., alloparental care). First, whether alloparental care is uniquely human is questionable considering some evidence for such forms of cooperation in other primate species (Boesch, Bole, Eckhardt, & Boesch, Reference Boesch, Bole, Eckhardt and Boesch2010; Fairbanks, Reference Fairbanks1990; Samuni, Wittig, & Crockford, Reference Samuni, Wittig and Crockford2019; Tokuyama et al., Reference Tokuyama, Toda, Poiret, Iyokango, Bakaa and Ishizuka2021). Second, to support the association between fearfulness and “human-unique” cooperation, Grossmann cites research showing associations between enhanced fearfulness sensitivity and cooperation in non-alloparental care contexts, such as parental care of own children (e.g., Kiel & Buss, Reference Kiel and Buss2011), and helping/sharing in infants, children, and adults (e.g., Grossmann et al., Reference Grossmann, Missana and Krol2018; Marsh & Ambady, Reference Marsh and Ambady2007; Rajhans et al., Reference Rajhans, Altvater-Mackensen, Vaish and Grossmann2016) that are, importantly, not unique to humans. Given that such forms of cooperation exist also in other species (for a review, see de Waal & Suchak, Reference de Waal and Suchak2010), evidence is lacking that heightened fearfulness is associated with “human-unique” cooperation.

Grossmann further argues that, next to the fight-or-flight response seen in many animals, heightened displayed and detected fearfulness elicits approach behaviours (tend and befriend) in humans in particular. However, the groundwork for a link between fearfulness and cooperation also exists in nonhuman animals. For example, newborn chimpanzees and bonobos are known to express fearfulness and distress through pout moans and whimpers in the context of maternal care (De Waal, Reference de Waal1988), and adult caregivers comfort offspring who show such signs by embracing and kissing them, similarly to humans (de Waal & Preston, Reference de Waal and Preston2017). Furthermore, offspring displaying distress provoke parental cooperative care in other species, including rodents and birds (de Waal & Preston, Reference de Waal and Preston2017). Even outside of infant–parent relationships, nonhuman animals, such as rodents, react to conspecifics displaying fear by engaging in prosocial behaviours (Keysers, Knapska, Moita, & Gazzola, Reference Keysers, Knapska, Moita and Gazzola2022). Therefore, it seems highly unlikely that heightened fearfulness elicits approach behaviours exclusively in humans.

In sum, singling out fearfulness as the affective trait that evolved specifically for cooperative purposes in humans seems premature. Additionally, there is insufficient evidence to conclude that human fear perception and production is significantly increased compared to other nonhuman animals. Finally, the link between heightened fearfulness and alloparental care awaits first empirical support. To truly examine these facets, it will be critical for future work to take the sensitivities and milieu of both human and nonhuman animals into account.

Financial support

CR, MK, and TZ were supported by the European Research Council (starting grant no. 804582) awarded to MK. MN was supported by a NWO VENI grant (Veni.201G.017).

Competing interest

None.

References

Aknin, L. B., Van de Vondervoort, J. W., & Hamlin, J. K. (2018). Positive feelings reward and promote prosocial behavior. Current Opinion in Psychology, 20, 5559. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boesch, C., Bole, C., Eckhardt, N., & Boesch, H. (2010). Altruism in forest chimpanzees: The case of adoption. PLoS ONE, 5(1), e8901. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0008901CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Centorrino, S., Djemai, E., Hopfensitz, A., Milinski, M., & Seabright, P. (2015). Honest signaling in trust interactions: Smiles rated as genuine induce trust and signal higher earning opportunities. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(1), 816. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.08.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danvers, A. F., & Shiota, M. N. (2018). Dynamically engaged smiling predicts cooperation above and beyond average smiling levels. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(1), 112119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.10.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Haan, M., & Nelson, C. A. (1998). Discrimination and categorization of facial expressions of emotion during infancy. In A., Slater (Ed.), Perceptual development: Visual, auditory, and speech perception in infancy (pp. 247–286). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
de Waal, F., & Preston, S. D. (2017). Mammalian empathy: Behavioural manifestations and neural basis. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(8), 498509. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2017.72CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Waal, F. B. (1988). The communicative repertoire of captive bonobos (Pan paniscus), compared to that of chimpanzees. Behaviour, 106(3–4), 183251. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853988X00269CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Waal, F. B., & Suchak, M. (2010). Prosocial primates: selfish and unselfish motivations. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365(1553), 27112722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fairbanks, L. A. (1990). Reciprocal benefits of allomothering for female vervet monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 40(3), 553562. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80536-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossmann, T., Missana, M., & Krol, K. M. (2018). The neurodevelopmental precursors of altruistic behavior in infancy. PLoS Biology, 16(9), e2005281. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.2005281CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrmann, E., Hare, B., Cissewski, J., & Tomasello, M. (2011). A comparison of temperament in nonhuman apes and human infants. Developmental Science, 14(6), 13931405. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01082.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isen, A. M., & Levin, P. F. (1972). Effect of feeling good on helping: Cookies and kindness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21(3), 384388. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0032317CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keysers, C., Knapska, E., Moita, M. A., & Gazzola, V. (2022). Emotional contagion and prosocial behavior in rodents. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 26(8), 688706. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2022.05.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiel, E. J., & Buss, K. A. (2011). Prospective relations among fearful temperament, protective parenting, and social withdrawal: The role of maternal accuracy in a moderated mediation framework. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39(7), 953966. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-011-9516-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kret, M. E., Jaasma, L., Bionda, T., & Wijnen, J. G. (2016). Bonobos (Pan paniscus) show an attentional bias toward conspecifics’ emotions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(14), 37613766. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1522060113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kret, M. E., Muramatsu, A., & Matsuzawa, T. (2018). Emotion processing across and within species: A comparison between humans (Homo sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 132(4), 395. https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000108CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kret, M. E., Prochazkova, E., Sterck, E. H., & Clay, Z. (2020). Emotional expressions in human and non-human great apes. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 115, 378395. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.01.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kushlev, K., Radosic, N., & Diener, E. (2022). Subjective well-being and prosociality around the globe: Happy people give more of their time and money to others. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 13(4), 849861. https://doi.org/10.1177/19485506211043379CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lengua, L. J., & Kovacs, E. A. (2005). Bidirectional associations between temperament and parenting and the prediction of adjustment problems in middle childhood. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 26(1), 2138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2004.10.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, A. A., & Ambady, N. (2007). The influence of the fear facial expression on prosocial responding. Cognition and Emotion, 21(2), 225247. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930600652234CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Messinger, D., & Fogel, A. (2007). The interactive development of social smiling. In Kail, R. V. (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (pp. 327366). Elsevier Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-009735-7.50014-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Messinger, D. S. (2002). Positive and negative: Infant facial expressions and emotions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(1), 16. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, B. S., Underwood, B., & Rosenhan, D. L. (1973). Affect and altruism. Developmental Psychology, 8(1), 99. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0033847CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pritsch, C., Telkemeyer, S., Mühlenbeck, C., & Liebal, K. (2017). Perception of facial expressions reveals selective affect-biased attention in humans and orangutans. Scientific Reports. 7(1), 7782. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-07563-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rajhans, P., Altvater-Mackensen, N., Vaish, A., & Grossmann, T. (2016). Children's altruistic behavior in context: The role of emotional responsiveness and culture. Scientific Reports, 6(1), 16. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep24089CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenhan, D. L., Salovey, P., & Hargis, K. (1981). The joys of helping: Focus of attention mediates the impact of positive affect on altruism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40(5), 899. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.40.5.899CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Samuni, L., Wittig, R. M., & Crockford, C. (2019). Adoption in the Taï chimpanzees: Costs, benefits and strong social relationships. In C. Crockford, L. Vigilant, T. Deschner, & F. Leendertz (Authors) & C. Boesch & R. Wittig (Eds.), The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest: 40 Years of Research (pp. 141158). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108674218.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scharlemann, J. P., Eckel, C. C., Kacelnik, A., & Wilson, R. K. (2001). The value of a smile: Game theory with a human face. Journal of Economic Psychology, 22(5), 617640. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-4870(01)00059-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tokuyama, N., Toda, K., Poiret, M. L., Iyokango, B., Bakaa, B., & Ishizuka, S. (2021). Two wild female bonobos adopted infants from a different social group at Wamba. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 111. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-83667-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, D. A., & Tomonaga, M. (2018). Visual discrimination of primate species based on faces in chimpanzees. Primates, 59(3), 243251. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-018-0649-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, D. A., & Tomonaga, M. (2021). Search asymmetries for threatening faces in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 136(1), 2034. https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000302CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wirth, B. E., & Wentura, D. (2020). It occurs after all: Attentional bias towards happy faces in the dot-probe task. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82(5), 24632481. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-020-02017-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zsido, A. N., Arato, N., Ihasz, V., Basler, J., Matuz-Budai, T., Inhof, O., … Coelho, C. M. (2021). “Finding an emotional face” revisited: Differences in own-age bias and the happiness superiority effect in children and young adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 580565. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.580565CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed