No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Behavioural modernity is dead: Long live behavioural modernity
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 14 January 2025
Abstract
Using Neanderthal symbolism, I extend on Stibbard-Hawkes to show that reconsidering the link between cognitive capacity and material culture extends beyond matters of preservation. A reconceptualization of behavioural modernity inclusive of both extant and extinct populations must begin with an honest theoretical separation of biological and behavioural modernity, which requires to critically engage with how we frame the underlying questions.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Ames, C. J. H., Riel-Salvatore, J., & Collins, B. R. (2013). Why we need an alternative approach to the study of modern human behaviour. Canadian Journal of Archaeology/Journal Canadien d'Archéologie, 37(1), 21–47.Google Scholar
Baquedano, E., Arsuaga, J. L., Pérez-González, A., Laplana, C., Márquez, B., Huguet, R., … Higham, T. (2023). A symbolic Neanderthal accumulation of large herbivore crania. Nature Human Behaviour, 7(3), 342–352. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-022-01503-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blessing, M. A. (2023). Technological complexity as an indicator of behavioural modernity: A case study on Middle Palaeolithic birch tar production and South African Microlithic technology. Doctoral Dissertation, Eberhard Karls University Tübingen. Retrieved from https://publikationen.uni-tuebingen.de/xmlui/handle/10900/136075Google Scholar
Botha, R. (2008). Prehistoric shell beads as a window on language evolution. Language & Communication, 28(3), 197–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botha, R. (2010). On the soundness of inferring modern language from symbolic behaviour. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 20(3), 345–356. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0959774310000454CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conard, N. J. (2008). A critical view of the evidence for a Southern African origin of behavioural modernity. South African Archaeological Society Goodwin Series, 10, 175–179. https://doi.org/10.3316/informit.838113602743050Google Scholar
Conard, N. J. (2010). Cultural modernity: Consensus or conundrum? Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(17), 7621–7622. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1001458107CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conard, N. J. (2011). Comment on Shea 2011: Homo sapiens is as Homo sapiens was: Behavioural variability versus “behavioural modernity” in Paleolithic archaeology. Current Anthropology, 52(1), 17–18.Google Scholar
d'Errico, F., Vanhaeren, M., Barton, N., Bouzouggar, A., Mienis, H., Richter, D., … Lozouet, P. (2009). Additional evidence on the use of personal ornaments in the Middle Paleolithic of North Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(38), 16051–16056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henshilwood, C. S., & Marean, C. W. (2003). The origin of modern human behavior: Critique of the models and their test implications. Current Anthropology, 44(5), 627–651. https://doi.org/10.1086/377665CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keel, T. (2017). Neanderthal-human hybridity and the frontier of critical race studies. Red and Yellow, Black and Brown, 201–217.Google Scholar
Kelly, R. L., Mackie, M. E., & Kandel, A. W. (2023). Rapid increase in production of symbolic artifacts after 45,000 years ago is not a consequence of taphonomic bias. Journal of Archaeological Science, 160, 105885.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Madison, P. (2020). Characterized by darkness: Reconsidering the origins of the Brutish Neanderthal. Journal of the History of Biology, 53(4), 493–519. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10739-020-09623-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madison, P. (2021). Brutish Neanderthals: History of a merciless characterization. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 30(6), 366–374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McBrearty, S., & Brooks, A. S. (2000). The revolution that wasn't: A new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. Journal of Human Evolution, 39(5), 453–563. https://doi.org/10.1006/jhev.2000.0435CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mellars, P. (1989). Major issues in the emergence of modern humans. Current Anthropology, 30(3), 349–385. https://doi.org/10.1086/203755CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meneganzin, A., & Currie, A. (2022). Behavioural modernity, investigative disintegration & Rubicon expectation. Synthese, 200(1), 47. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-022-03491-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nowell, A. (2010). Defining behavioural modernity in the context of Neanderthal and anatomically modern human populations. Annual Review of Anthropology, 39(1), 437–452. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.anthro.012809.105113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nowell, A. (2011). Comment on Shea 2011: Homo sapiens is as Homo sapiens was: Behavioural variability versus “behavioural modernity” in Paleolithic archaeology. Current Anthropology, 52(1), 19–20.Google Scholar
Peeters, S., & Zwart, H. (2020). Neanderthals as familiar strangers and the human spark: How the “golden years” of Neanderthal research reopen the question of human uniqueness. History and Philosophy of the Life Sciences, 42(3), 33. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40656-020-00327-wCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scerri, E. M. L., & Will, M. (2023). The revolution that still isn't: The origins of behavioural complexity in Homo sapiens. Journal of Human Evolution, 179, 103358. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2023.103358CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shea, J. J. (1998). Neanderthal and early modern human behavioural variability A regional-scale approach to lithic evidence for hunting in the Levantine Mousterian. Current Anthropology, 39(S1), S45–S78. https://doi.org/10.1086/204690CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shea, J. J. (2011). Homo sapiens is as Homo sapiens was. Current Anthropology, 52(1), 1–35. https://doi.org/10.1086/658067CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stiner, M. C. (2017). Love and death in the Stone Age: What constitutes first evidence of mortuary treatment of the human body? Biological Theory, 12(4), 248–261. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13752-017-0275-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Villa, P., & Roebroeks, W. (2014). Neanderthal demise: An archaeological analysis of the modern human superiority complex. PLoS ONE, 9(4), e96424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wadley, L. (2013). Recognizing complex cognition through innovative technology in Stone Age and Palaeolithic sites. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 23(2), 163–183. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0959774313000309CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wynn, T., Overmann, K. A., & Coolidge, F. L. (2016). The false dichotomy: A refutation of the Neanderthal indistinguishability claim. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 94, 1–22.Google ScholarPubMed
Zilhão, J. (2007). The emergence of ornaments and art: An archaeological perspective on the origins of “behavioural modernity.” Journal of Archaeological Research, 15(1), 1–54. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10814-006-9008-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zilhão, J., Angelucci, D. E., Badal-García, E., d'Errico, F., Daniel, F., Dayet, L., … Montes-Bernárdez, R. (2010). Symbolic use of marine shells and mineral pigments by Iberian Neanderthals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(3), 1023–1028.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Target article
Reconsidering the link between past material culture and cognition in light of contemporary hunter–gatherer material use
Related commentaries (21)
Advancing paleoanthropology beyond default nulls
All that glitters is not gold: The false-symbol problem in archaeology
Animal artefacts challenge archaeological standards for tracing human symbolic cognition
Archaeology retains a central role for studying the behavioral and cognitive evolution of our species and genus
Are we jingling modern hunter-gatherers and early Homo sapiens?
Behavioural modernity is dead: Long live behavioural modernity
Beyond the binary: Inferential challenges and solutions in cognitive archaeology
Cultural innovation is not only a product of cognition but also of cultural context
Don't ignore cognitive evolution during the three million years that preceded the archaeological record of material culture!
Inferences from absences
Material culture both reflects and causes human cognitive evolution
Negative priors and inferences from absence of evidence in cognitive and linguistic archaeology: Epistemically sound and scientifically strategic
Not just symbolism: Technologies may also have a less than direct connection with cognition
Perishable material choice indicates symbolic and representational capacities
Proposing the DN(C)-model of material evidence for well-calibrated claims about past cultures
Revising the null model in language evolution research
Shared intentionality may have been favored by persistence hunting in Homo erectus
Sports, team games, and physical skill competitions as an important source of symbolic material culture with low preservation probability
The cognitive and evolutionary science of behavioural modernity goes beyond material chronology
The Mbuti people still reproduce a 75,000 years old recursive pattern
What would be pre-modern human cognition?
Author response
Hominin cognition: The null hypothesis