Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-30T20:36:41.077Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Using foresight to prioritise the present

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 May 2017

Adam Bulley
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, QLD 4067, [email protected]@psy.uq.edu.auhttps://www.psy.uq.edu.au/directory/index.html?id=2472https://www.psy.uq.edu.au/directory/index.html?id=39
Gillian Pepper
Affiliation:
Centre for Behaviour and Evolution at Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne NE1 7RU, United Kingdom. [email protected]://gillianpepper.com/
Thomas Suddendorf
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, QLD 4067, [email protected]@psy.uq.edu.auhttps://www.psy.uq.edu.au/directory/index.html?id=2472https://www.psy.uq.edu.au/directory/index.html?id=39

Abstract

Planning for the future may encourage apparently “impulsive” behaviour when the future is anticipated to be bleak. Thus, a seeming failure of self-control in reactive violence could be caused not by a disinclination to plan ahead, but by virtue of this ability. Furthermore, we point to empirical and theoretical shortcomings in the authors' case, such as a failure to distinguish proximate and ultimate explanations.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bolland, J. M. (2003) Hopelessness and risk behaviour among adolescents living in high-poverty inner-city neighbourhoods. Journal of Adolescence 26(2):145–58. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-1971(02)00136-7 Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (2008) Evolutionary economics of mental time travel? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12(6):219–24. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.03.003 Google Scholar
Brezina, T., Tekin, E. & Topalli, V. (2009) ‘Might not be a tomorrow’: A multimethods approach to anticipated early death and youth crime. Criminology 47(4):1091–129. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-9125.2009.00170.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, C. & Lall, U. (2006) Water and economic development: The role of variability and a framework for resilience. Natural Resources Forum 30(4):306–17. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1477-8947.2006.00118.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bulley, A., Henry, J. & Suddendorf, T. (2016) Prospection and the present moment: The role of episodic foresight in intertemporal choices between immediate and delayed rewards. Review of General Psychology 20(1):2947. Available at: http://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000061 Google Scholar
Daly, M. & Wilson, M. (2005) Carpe Diem: Adaptation and devaluing the future. The Quarterly Review of Biology 80(1):5560. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1086/521238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, J. (1999) Guns, germs, and steel: The fates of human societies. WW Norton.Google Scholar
Frank, R. H. (1988) Passions within reason: The strategic role of the emotions. WW Norton & Co.Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T. & Wilson, T. D. (2007) Prospection: Experiencing the future. Science 317(5843):1351–54. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1144161.Google Scholar
Harner, M. (1977) The ecological basis for Aztec sacrifice. American Ethnologist 4(1):117–35. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1525/ae.1977.4.1.02a00070.Google Scholar
Kirby, K. N., Petry, N. M. & Bickel, W. K. (1999) Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General 128(1):7887. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.128.1.78.Google Scholar
Mayr, E. (1961) Cause and effect in biology. Science 134(3489):1501–506. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1126/science.135.3507.972.Google Scholar
McBrearty, S. & Brooks, A. S. (2000) The revolution that wasn't: A new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. Journal of Human Evolution 39(5):453563. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1006/jhev.2000.0435.Google Scholar
Oki, T. & Kanae, S. (2006) Global hydrological cycles and world water resources. Science 313(5790):1068–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suddendorf, T. & Corballis, M. C. (2007) The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30(3):299351. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X07001975.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T. & Moore, C. (2011) Introduction to the special issue: The development of episodic foresight. Cognitive Development 26(4):295–98. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2011.09.001.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1963) On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift Für Tierpsychologie 20:410–33. Available at: http://doi.org/citeulike-article-id:9870373 Google Scholar
World Bank (2013) Intentional homicides (per 100,000 people). Available at: http://data.worldbank.org/indicator/VC.IHR.PSRC.P5?year_high_desc=false.Google Scholar
Zimbardo, P. G., Keough, K. A. & Boyd, J. N. (1997) Present time perspective as a predictor of risky driving. Personality and Individual Differences 23(6):1007–23. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00113-X.Google Scholar