Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T08:17:25.108Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Knowledge songs as an evolutionary adaptation to facilitate information transmission through music

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 September 2021

Daniel J. Levitin*
Affiliation:
Minerva Schools at Keck Graduate Institute,San Francisco, CA94103, [email protected]; www.daniellevitin.com Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, QC H3A 1G1, Canada

Abstract

I propose an adjunct to the two models presented in the target articles, a function of music that is ubiquitous and would have solved a clear adaptive problem, that of transmitting important survival information among pre-literate humans. This class of knowledge songs uniquely preserved cultural, botanical, medical, safety, and practical information that increased the adaptive fitness of societies.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Berkes, F., Colding, J., & Folke, C. (2000). Rediscovery of traditional ecological knowledge as adaptive management. Ecological Applications 10, 12511262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coe, C. (2003). The ancestress hypothesis: Visual arts as adaptation. Rutgers University Press.Google Scholar
Cross, I. (2007). Symposium. In Klockars, M. & Peltomaa, M. (Eds.), Music meets medicine(pp. 5–13). Signe and Ane Gyllenberg Foundation.Google Scholar
D'Azevedo, W. L. (1962). Uses of the past in Gola discourse. Journal of African History, 3, 1134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyman, I. E., & Rubin, D. C. (1990). Memorabeatlia: A naturalistic study of long-term memory. Memory & Cognition, 18(2), 205214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jennings, J., Antrobus, K., Atencio, S., Glavich, E., Johnson, R., Loffler, G., … & Jennings, J. (2005). “Drinking beer in a blissful mood” Alcohol production, operational chains, and feasting in the ancient world. Current Anthropology, 46(2), 275303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95(2), 163182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lattimore, R. (1951). Homer: The Iliad. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Levitin, D. J. (2008). The world in six songs: How the musical brain created human nature. Penguin Random House.Google Scholar
Lord, A. B. (1960). The singer of tales. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Mehr, S. A., & Krasnow, M. M. (2017). Parent-offspring conflict and the evolution of infant-directed song. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(5), 674684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mehr, S. A., Singh, M., Knox, D., Ketter, D. M., Pickens-Jones, D., Atwood, S., … Glowacki, L. (2019). Universality and diversity in human song. Science, 366(6468), 957970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, C., & Kelly, M. H. (1992). Linguistic prosody and musical meter in song. Journal of Memory and Language, 31(4), 525542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reyes-García, V., & Fernández-Llamazares, Á. (2019). Sing to learn: The role of songs in the transmission of indigenous knowledge among the Tsimane' of Bolivian Amazonia. Journal of Ethnobiology, 39(3), 460477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, D. C. (1995). Memory in oral traditions: The cognitive psychology of epic, ballads, and counting-out rhymes. Oxford University Press on Demand.Google Scholar
Rubin, D. C., Stoltzfus, E. R., & Wall, K. L. (1991). The abstraction of form in semantic categories. Memory & Cognition, 19(1), 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scalise-Sugiyama, M. (1996). On the origins of narrative. Human nature, 7(4), 403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scalise-Sugiyama, M. (2011). The forager oral tradition and the evolution of prolonged juvenility. Frontiers in Psychology 2, 133. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schniter, E., Wilcox, N. T., Beheim, B. A., Kaplan, H. S., & Gurven, M. (2018). Information transmission and the oral tradition: Evidence of a late-life service niche for Tsimane Amerindians. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(1), 94105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwanenflugel, P. J., & LaCount, K. L. (1988) Semantic relatedness and the scope of facilitation for upcoming words in sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning. Memory, and Cognition, 14, 344354.Google Scholar
Wilshaw, A. (2018). Modern humans, origins of. The International Encyclopedia of Anthropology, 114.Google Scholar