Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-02T18:47:32.449Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Food seeking and food sharing under uncertainty

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 March 2019

Efrat Aharonov-Majar
Affiliation:
Dynamic Decision Making Laboratory, Social and Decision Sciences Department, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213. [email protected]://www.linkedin.com/in/efrat-aharonov-majar-phd-50b60240/
Ramzi Suleiman
Affiliation:
Triangle Research and Development Center (TRDC), Kafr Kara, Israel 30075. Department of Psychology, University of Haifa, Mount Carmel, Haifa, Israel3498838. [email protected]://suleiman.haifa.ac.il/ Department of Philosophy, Al Quds University, Palestinian National Authority.

Abstract

We propose an elaboration of Anselme and Güntürkün's research that considers individuals’ foraging behavior as part of group efforts to cope with uncertainty. We discuss different possibilities for the interaction between individual and group mechanisms for risk reduction in uncertain environments, and we raise some open questions for future research.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bird, D. W. (1997) Delayed reciprocity and tolerated theft: The behavioral ecology of food-sharing strategies. Current Anthropology 38(1):4978. https://doi.org/10.1086/204581.Google Scholar
Bleige Bird, R., Smith, E. A., & SBird, D.W. (2001) The hunting handicap: Costly signaling in human foraging strategies. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 50(1):919. https://doi.org/10.1007/s002650100338.Google Scholar
Blurton Jones, N.G. (1987) Tolerated theft, suggestions about the ecology and evolution of sharing, hoarding and scrounging. Social Science Information 26(1):3154.Google Scholar
Bone, J., Hey, J. & Suckling, J. (2004) A simple risk-sharing experiment. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 28(1):2328.Google Scholar
Charness, G. & Genicot, G. (2009) Informal risk sharing in an infinite-horizon experiment. The Economic Journal 119(537):796825. http://www.jstor.org/stable/20485344.Google Scholar
De Dreu, C. K. W., Greer, L. L., Handgraaf, M. J. J., Shalvi, S., Kleef, G. A. Van, Baas, M., Ten Velden, F.S., Van Dijk, E. & Feith, S. W. W. (2010) The neuropeptide oxytocin regulates parochial altruism in intergroup conflict among humans. Science 328(5984):1408–11. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1189047.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. (1989) Food sharing and reciprocal obligations among chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution 18(5):433459. https://doi.org/10.1016/0047-2484(89)90074-2.Google Scholar
Gurven, M. (2004) To give and to give not: The behavioral ecology of human food transfers. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27:543–83. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X04000123.Google Scholar
Gurven, M., Hill, K., Kaplan, H., Hurtado, A. & Lyles, R. (2000) Food transfers among Hiwi foragers of Venezuela: Tests of reciprocity. Human Ecology 28(2):171218. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007067919982.Google Scholar
Hawkes, K., O'Connell, J. F. & Blurton Jones, N. G. (2001) Hadza meat sharing. Evolution and Human Behavior 22(2):113142. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00066-0.Google Scholar
Jimenez, S. & Pietras, C. (2018) An investigation of the probability of reciprocation in a risk-reduction model of sharing. Behavioural Processes 157:583–89. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2018.04.012.Google Scholar
Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Cadeliña, R. V, Hayden, B., Hyndman, D. C., Preston, R. J., Alden Smith, E., Stuart, D.E. & Yesner, D. R. (1985) Food sharing among ache foragers: Tests of explanatory hypotheses [and Comments and Reply]. Current Anthropology 26(2):223–46.Google Scholar
Kaplan, H., Schniter, E., Smith, V. L. & Wilson, B. J. (2012) Risk and the evolution of human exchange. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 279(1740):2930–35. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2011.2614.Google Scholar
Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, U. & Fehr, E. (2005) Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature 435(7042):673–76. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature03701.Google Scholar
Madden, J. R. & Clutton-Brock, T. H. (2011) Experimental peripheral administration of oxytocin elevates a suite of cooperative behaviours in a wild social mammal. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 278(1709):1189–94. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.1675.Google Scholar
Suleiman, R., Aharonov-Majar, E. & Luzon, P. (2015) The sharing dilemma: Joining cooperative groups and sharing resources as a means of coping with environmental risk. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 28(2). https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.1831.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, G. S. (1990) Food sharing in vampire bats. Scientific American 262(2):7683.Google Scholar
Winterhalder, B. (1986) Diet choice, risk, and food sharing in a stochastic environment. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 5(4):369–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/0278-4165(86)90017-6.Google Scholar
Wittig, R. M., Crockford, C., Deschner, T., Langergraber, K. E., Ziegler, T. E. & Zuberbuhler, K. (2014) Food sharing is linked to urinary oxytocin levels and bonding in related and unrelated wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 281(1778):20133096. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.3096.Google Scholar