Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T17:40:26.608Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Context matters: How macroeconomic forces may alter the reception of negative emotions in art

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 November 2017

Hal Ersner Hershfield
Affiliation:
Marketing and Behavioral Decision Making Areas, Anderson School of Management, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095. [email protected]
Adam Lee Alter
Affiliation:
Stern School of Business, Marketing Department, New York University, New York, NY 10012. [email protected]

Abstract

Menninghaus et al. offer a comprehensive model to explain why people pursue darker emotions in art, but we believe they underplay the considerable role of situational factors in driving these preferences. In particular, changing mood states are likely to shape artistic preferences, in large part because positive mood states act as a protective buffer against otherwise aversive experiences.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adler, J. M. & Hershfield, H. E. (2012) Mixed emotional experience is associated with and precedes improvements in psychological well-being. PLoS ONE 7(4):e3563. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0035633.Google Scholar
Alter, A. L. (2013a) Drunk tank pink. Penguin.Google Scholar
Alter, A. L. (2013b) The benefits of cognitive disfluency. Current Directions in Psychological Science 22:437–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alter, A. L. & Forgas, J. P. (2007) On feeling happy but fearing failure: The effects of mood on self-handicapping strategies. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 43:947–54.Google Scholar
Bless, H., Fiedler, K. & Forgas, J. P. (2006) Mood and regulation of information processing and behavior. In: Affect in social thinking and behavior, ed. Forgas, J. P., pp. 6584. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bollen, J., Pepe, A. & Mao, H. (2009) Modeling public mood and emotion: Twitter sentiment and socio-economic phenomena. In: Proceedings of the Fifth International AAAI Conference on Weblogs and Social Media (ICWSM 2011), 17–21 July 2011, Barcelona, Spain. Association for the Advancement of Artificial Intelligence. arXiv:0911.1583.Google Scholar
Fong, C. T. (2006) The effects of emotional ambivalence on creativity. Academy of Management Journal 49(5):10161030.Google Scholar
Forgas, J. (2001) Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fredrickson, B. (2004) The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences 359(1449):1367–78.Google Scholar
Garg, N., Wansink, B. & Inman, J. J. (2007) The influence of incidental affect on consumers' food intake. Journal of Marketing 71(1):194206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hershfield, H. E., Scheibe, S., Sims, T. L. & Carstensen, L. L. (2013) When feeling bad can be good: Mixed emotions benefit physical health across adulthood. Social Psychological and Personality Science 4(1):5461.Google Scholar
Kamstra, M. J., Kramer, L. A. & Levi, M. D. (2003) Winter blues: A SAD stock market cycle. American Economic Review 93(1):324–43.Google Scholar
Lerner, J. S., Li, Y., Valdesolo, P. & Kassam, K. S. (2015) Emotion and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology 66:799823.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lerner, J. S., Li, Y. & Weber, E. U. (2013) The financial costs of sadness. Psychological Science 24(1):7279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quigley, B. M. & Tedeschi, J. T. (1996) Mediating effects of blame attributions on feelings of anger. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 22(12):1280–88.Google Scholar
Raghunathan, R. & Trope, Y. (2002) Walking the tightrope between feeling good and being accurate: Mood as a resource in processing persuasive messages. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 83(3):510–25.Google Scholar
Trope, Y., Ferguson, M. & Raghunathan, R. (2000) Mood as a resource in processing self-relevant information. In: Handbook of affect and cognition, vol. 1, ed. Forgas, J. P., pp. 258–76. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Trope, Y. & Neter, E. (1994) Reconciling competing motives in self-evaluation: The role of self-control in feedback seeking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 66(4):646–57.Google Scholar