from Part I - Conceptual Models of Social Signals
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 13 July 2017
The pace of research devoted to the study of social and emotional intelligence has escalated exponentially (Bar-On & Parker, 2000; Goleman, 2006; Mayer & Salovey, 1997; Salovey & Mayer, 1990), and the upsurge in interest has intensified the need to understand social signals, whose production and deciphering may be strongly affected by these various intelligences (Gardner, 1983). Social signals have been conceptualized in a variety of ways, but as social psychologists, we define a social signal as any variable associated with a communicative act, excluding its written or spoken content, that carries meaning. The signal may be intentionally or unintentionally encoded by a source, and mindfully or mindlessly decoded by a receiver. This definition acknowledges and allows for the smile or smirk that accompanies the expression, “You look good today,” or the falling intonation of the word “today,” to carry more meaning than the actual content of the declaration. By this definition, research on communication and persuasion from its inception has focused on understanding social signal effects. Research designed to investigate the credibility of a source of a persuasive message, for example, often relied upon extra-communication features (age, sex, dress, apparent success, etc.) to signal the extent to which a receiver should ponder or trust the information provided by the source. The speed of speech, attractiveness of the source, and the animation of the speaker all have strong effects on persuasion. This is true even when the content of the verbal or written communication remains constant across experimental conditions.
In experimental research, the social signal is considered from the vantage point of the encoder, or in other terms, as an independent variable. As such, failures to obtain differences attributable to signal variations often were counted as manipulation breakdowns. Less frequently, researchers have focused on the receivers of social signals, and sometimes the interactive behaviors of both encoders and decoders are examined in tandem. In one such study, college-age men were led to believe that a young woman with whom they were to converse via telephone was either beautiful or plain (Snyder, Tanke, & Berscheid, 1977).
To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.