Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T02:15:11.211Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Bridging to the unknown: A transition mechanism in learning and development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Nira Granott
Affiliation:
Assistant Professor School of Human Development, University of Texas at Dallas; Director of the Microdevelopmental Lab UT Dallas
Kurt W. Fischer
Affiliation:
Harvard Graduate School of Education, Harvard University, USA
Jim Parziale
Affiliation:
Part-Time Professor Graduate School of Education, University of Massachusetts, Boston; Classroom Teacher and Science Resource Teacher for Brookline Public School Brookline, Massachusetts
Nira Granott
Affiliation:
University of Texas, Dallas
Jim Parziale
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts, Boston
Get access

Summary

How are new abilities created out of existing, less advanced abilities? This question has puzzled researchers for centuries. Various answers have been suggested, yet the mechanisms underlying development have remained enigmatic. In this chapter, we analyze and demonstrate a process called bridging that provides a specific answer to the fundamental question of how more powerful structures can be achieved on the basis of less powerful ones.

Bridging is a process of leaping into the unknown by inserting marker shells that indicate targets for development and learning (Granott, 1993a, 1994, Granott & Parziale, 1996). The marker shells serve as place-holders that people use to direct their own learning and development toward achieving these targets. A shell is like a formula in mathematics, in which the variables represent an unknown whose values can be later defined. Bridging operates as an attractor in dynamic systems and pulls development toward more advanced, relatively stable levels. People use bridging by creating partially defined shells that mark future skills to be constructed at higher knowledge levels. The shells do not contain the relevant knowledge yet, but they outline it. The shells serve as scaffolds that guide the construction of new knowledge by providing a perspective for processing new experiences. A bridging shell serves as a dynamic attractor after its initial emergence in real-time activities within context.

We suggest that bridging is a transition mechanism that people use spontaneously at a wide range of ages.

Type
Chapter
Information
Microdevelopment
Transition Processes in Development and Learning
, pp. 131 - 156
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2002

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baldwin, J. M. (1894). Mental development in the child and the race: Methods and processes. New York: Macmillan
Bowerman, M. (1989). Learning a semantic system: What role do cognitive predispositions play? In M. Rice & R. Schiefelbusch (Eds.), The teachability of language (chap. 4). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1993). The ecology of cognitive development: Research models and fugitive findings. In R. Wozniak & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Development in context: Acting and thinking in specific environments (pp. 3–44). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Case, R. (1991). A developmental approach to the design of remedial instruction. In A. McKeough & J. Lupert (Eds.), Toward the practice of theory-based instruction (pp. 117–147). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Case, R., & McKeough, A. (1990). Schooling and the development of central conceptual structures: An example from the domain of children's narrative. International Journal of Educational Psychology, 8, 835–855Google Scholar
Case, R., Okamoto, Y., Griffin, S., McKeough, A., Bleiker, C., Henderson, B., & Stephenson, K. M. (1996). The role of central conceptual structures in the development of children's thought. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 61(1–2) (Serial No. 246)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1975). Reflections on language. New York: Pantheon
Chomsky, N. (1980). On cognitive structures and their development: A reply to Piaget. In M. Piatelli-Palmarini (Ed.), Language and learning: The debate between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky (pp. 35–52). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Claparède, E. (1931). L’éducation fonctionnelle. Paris & Neuchâtel: Delachaux & Niestlé
Cole, M. (1988). Cross-cultural research in the sociohistorical tradition. Human Development, 31, 137–157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damasio, A. R., & Damasio, H. (1992). Brain and language. Scientific American, 267(3), 89–95CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dromi, E. (1987/1996). Early lexical development (first published 1987). San Diego: Singular Publishing
Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving. Psychological Monographs, 58 (Whole no. 270)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Fischer, K. W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review, 87, 477–531CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, K. W., & Ayoub, C. (1994). Affective splitting and dissociation in normal and maltreated children: Developmental pathways for self in relationships. In D. Cicchetti & S. L. Toth (Eds.), Rochester Symposium on dysfunctions of the self (pp. 149–222). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press
Fischer, K. W., & Bidell, T. R. (1998). Dynamic development of psychological structures in action and thought. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (pp. 467–561). New York: Wiley
Fischer, K. W., Bullock, D. H., Rotenberg, E. J., & Raya, P. (1993). The dynamics of competence: How context contributes directly to skill. In R. H. Wozniak & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Development in context: Acting and thinking in specific environments. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Fischer, K. W., & Granott, N. (1995). Beyond one-dimensional change: Multiple, concurrent, socially distributed processes in learning and development. Human Development, 38, 302–314CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, K. W., & Rose, S. P. (1994). Dynamic development of coordination of components in brain and behavior: A framework for theory and research. In G. Dawson & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Human behavior and the developing brain (pp. 3–66). New York: Guilford
Fischer, K. W., & Yan, Z. (this volume). Darwin's construction of the theory of evolution: Microdevelopment of explanations of variation and change in species
Fodor, J. A. (1975). The language of thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Gelman, R., Romo, L., & Francis, W. S. (this volume). Notebooks as windows on learning: The case of a science-into-ESL program
Gesell, A. (1940). The first five years of life. New York: Harper & Row
Goldin-Meadow, S., & Alibali, M. W. (this volume). Looking at the hands through time: A microgenetic perspective on learning and instruction
Goldin-Meadow, S., Nusbaum, H., Garber, P., & Church, R. B. (1993). Transitions in learning: Evidence for simultaneously activated rules. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 92–107Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1991). Experiential canalization of behavioral development: Theory. Developmental Psychology, 27(1), 4–13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granott, N. (1992). Microdevelopmental puzzle and the mechanism of cognitive growth: Alternative pathways, parallel access, and co-existing structures. Paper presented at the 22nd Annual Symposium of the Jean Piaget Society, Montreal, Canada
Granott, N. (1993a). Microdevelopment of co-construction of knowledge during problem-solving: Puzzled minds, weird creatures, and wuggles. Doctoral dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA [on line]. Available: http://theses.mit.edu:80/Dienst/UI/2.0/Composite/0018.mit.theses/1993-170/1?nsections-19
Granott, N. (1993b). Patterns of interaction in the co-construction of knowledge: Separate minds, joint effort, and weird creatures. In R. Wozniak & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Development in context: Acting and thinking in specific environments (pp. 183–207). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Granott, N. (1994). On the mechanism of cognitive change: Transition mechanism in microdevelopment and the “Zone of Current Development”. Paper presented at the 24th Annual Symposium of the Jean Piaget Society, Chicago, IL
Granott, N. (1998a). We learn, therefore we develop: Learning versus development – or developing learning? In C. Smith & T. Pourchot (Eds.), Adult learning and development: Perspectives from educational psychology (pp. 15–34). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Granott, N. (1998b). Unit of analysis in transit: From the individual's knowledge to the ensemble process. Mind, Culture, and Activity: An International Journal, 5(1), 42–66CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granott, N. (this volume). How microdevelopment creates macrodevelopment: Reiterated sequences, backward transitions, and the Zone of Current Development
Granott, N., & Parziale, J. (1996). Bridges to and from the unknown: The developmental mechanism of bridging. Paper presented at the 26th Annual Symposium of the Jean Piaget Society. Philadelphia, PA
Grossberg, S. (1987). The adaptive brain (2 vols.). Amsterdam: Elsevier/North-Holland
Inhelder, B., & Piaget, G. (1955/1958). The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence (A. Parsons & S. Seagrim, trans.). New York: Basic Books. (Originally published 1955.)
Klahr, D. (1992). Information-processing approaches to cognitive development. In M. H. Bornstein & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental psychology: An advanced textbook (pp. 273–335). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Klahr, D., Faye, A. L., & Dunbar, K. (1993). Heuristics for scientific experimentation: A developmental study. Cognitive Psychology, 25, 111–146CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krech, D. (1932). The genesis of “hypotheses” in rats. University of California Publications in Psychology, 6, 45–64Google Scholar
Kuhn, D. (this volume). A multi-component system that constructs knowledge: Insights from microgenetic study
Kuhn, D., Garcia-Mila, M., Zohar, A., & Andersen, C. (1995). Strategies of knowledge acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 60(4) (Serial No. 245)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, K., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (this volume). Macro-and microdevelopmental research: Assumptions, research strategies, constraints, and utilities
Leont′ev, A. N. (1981). The problem of activity in psychology. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), The concept of activity in Soviet psychology (pp. 37–71). Armonk, NY: Sharpe
Lerner, R. M. (1991). Changing organism-context relations as the basic process of development: A developmental contextual perspective. Developmental Psychology, 27(1), 27–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M. D. (this volume). Interacting time scales in personality (and cognitive) development: Intentions, emotions, and emergent forms
MacWhinney, B., & Leinbach, J. (1991). Implementations are not conceptualizations: Revising the verb learning model. Cognition, 40, 121–157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcel, A. J. (1983). Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 197–237CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Miller, P. H., & Aloise-Young, P. A. (1995). Preschoolers’ strategic behavior and performance on a same-different task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 60(2), 284–303CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, P. H., & Coyle, T. R. (1999). Developmental change: Lessons from microgenesis. In E. K. Scholnick, K. Nelson, S. A. Gelman, & P. H. Miller (Eds.), Conceptual development: Piaget's legacy (pp. 209–239). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1971). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall
Newman, D., Griffin, P., & Cole, M. (1989). The construction zone: Working for change in school. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
Parziale, J. (1997). Microdevelopment during an activity based science lesson. Doctoral dissertation, Harvard Graduate School of Education, Harvard University
Parziale, J. (this volume). Observing the dynamics of construction: Children building bridges and new ideas
Piaget, J. (1936/1952). The origins of intelligence in children (M. Cook, trans.). New York: International Universities Press. (Originally published 1936.)
Piaget, J. (1970). Piaget's theory. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael's manual of child psychology (pp. 703–732). New York: Wiley
Piaget, J. (1985). The equilibration of cognitive structures: The central problem of intellectual development (T. Brown & K. J. Thampy, trans.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Radziszewska, B., & Rogoff, B. (1988). Influence of adult and peer collaborators on children's planning skills. Developmental Psychology, 24, 840–848CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. New York: Oxford University Press
Rogoff, B., Mistry, J., Goncu, A., & Mosier, C. (1993). Guided participation in cultural activity by toddlers and caregivers. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58(8) (Serial No. 236)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1988). Explorations in parallel distributed processing: A handbook of models, programs, and exercises. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Scarr, S. (1993). Biological and cultural diversity: The legacy of Darwin for development. Child Development, 64, 1333–1353CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schauble, L. (1990). Belief revision in children: The role of prior knowledge and strategies for generating evidence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49, 31–57CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schauble, L. (1996). The development of scientific reasoning in knowledge-rich contexts. Developmental Psychology, 32(1), 102–119CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegler, R. S. (1994). Cognitive variability: A key to understanding cognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3(1), 1–5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegler, R. S. (1996). Emerging minds: The process of change in children's thinking. New York: Oxford University Press
Siegler, R. S. (this volume). Microgenetic studies of self-explanation
Siegler, R. S., & Crowley, K. (1991). The microgenetic method: A direct means for studying cognitive development. American Psychologist, 46, 606–620CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegler, R. S., & Jenkins, E. (1989). How children discover new strategies. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Simon, T. J., & Halford, G. S. (Eds.). (1995). Developing cognitive competence: New approaches to process modeling. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Smith, L. B., & Thelen, E. (Eds.). (1993). A dynamic systems approach to development: Applications. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Thelen, E., & Corbetta, D. (this volume). Microdevelopment and dynamic systems: Applications to infant motor development
Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review, 55, 189–208CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geert, P. (1991). A dynamic systems model of cognitive and language growth. Psychological Review, 98, 3–53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Geert, P. (1994). Dynamic systems of development: Change between complexity and chaos. New York: Harvester Wheatsheaf
Geert, P. (1998a). We almost had a great future behind us: The contribution of non-linear dynamics to developmental-science-in-the-making. Developmental Science, 1(1), 143–159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geert, P. (1998b). A dynamic systems model of basic developmental mechanisms: Piaget, Vygotsky, and beyond. Psychological Review, 105(4), 634–677CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Geert, P. (this volume). Developmental dynamics, intentional action, and fuzzy sets
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Werner, H. (1948). Comparative psychology of mental development. New York: International Universities Press
Werner, H. (1957). The concept of development from a comparative and organismic point of view. In D. B. Harris (Ed.), The concept of development: An issue in the study of human behavior (pp. 125–148). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press
Wertsch, J. V. (1985). Vygotsky and the social formation of mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Bridging to the unknown: A transition mechanism in learning and development
    • By Nira Granott, Assistant Professor School of Human Development, University of Texas at Dallas; Director of the Microdevelopmental Lab UT Dallas, Kurt W. Fischer, Harvard Graduate School of Education, Harvard University, USA, Jim Parziale, Part-Time Professor Graduate School of Education, University of Massachusetts, Boston; Classroom Teacher and Science Resource Teacher for Brookline Public School Brookline, Massachusetts
  • Edited by Nira Granott, University of Texas, Dallas, Jim Parziale, University of Massachusetts, Boston
  • Book: Microdevelopment
  • Online publication: 22 September 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511489709.006
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Bridging to the unknown: A transition mechanism in learning and development
    • By Nira Granott, Assistant Professor School of Human Development, University of Texas at Dallas; Director of the Microdevelopmental Lab UT Dallas, Kurt W. Fischer, Harvard Graduate School of Education, Harvard University, USA, Jim Parziale, Part-Time Professor Graduate School of Education, University of Massachusetts, Boston; Classroom Teacher and Science Resource Teacher for Brookline Public School Brookline, Massachusetts
  • Edited by Nira Granott, University of Texas, Dallas, Jim Parziale, University of Massachusetts, Boston
  • Book: Microdevelopment
  • Online publication: 22 September 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511489709.006
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Bridging to the unknown: A transition mechanism in learning and development
    • By Nira Granott, Assistant Professor School of Human Development, University of Texas at Dallas; Director of the Microdevelopmental Lab UT Dallas, Kurt W. Fischer, Harvard Graduate School of Education, Harvard University, USA, Jim Parziale, Part-Time Professor Graduate School of Education, University of Massachusetts, Boston; Classroom Teacher and Science Resource Teacher for Brookline Public School Brookline, Massachusetts
  • Edited by Nira Granott, University of Texas, Dallas, Jim Parziale, University of Massachusetts, Boston
  • Book: Microdevelopment
  • Online publication: 22 September 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511489709.006
Available formats
×