Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T15:04:41.001Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

12 - Measuring Speech Rhythm

from Section III - Measuring Speech

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 November 2021

Rachael-Anne Knight
Affiliation:
City, University of London
Jane Setter
Affiliation:
University of Reading
Get access

Summary

This paper covers the methods for measuring rhythm and the main paradigms used to study rhythm perception. An overview of ideas about speech rhythm is provided, starting with the traditional view of isochrony and rhythm classes. Production and perception methods used to establish rhythm-class differences are presented and critically reviewed, as are a number of research practices associated with them. Recent developments leading to an alternative view of rhythm are discussed, and suggestions for pedagogical practice and future research are provided.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

12.7 References

Abercrombie, D. (1967). Elements of General Phonetics. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Arvaniti, A. (2009). Rhythm, timing and the timing of rhythm. Phonetica, 66, 4663.Google Scholar
Arvaniti, A. (2012a). The usefulness of metrics in the quantification of speech rhythm. Journal of Phonetics, 40(3), 351–73.Google Scholar
Arvaniti, A. (2012b). Rhythm classes and speech perception. In Niebuhr, O. & Pfitzinger, H., eds., Prosodies: Context, Function, and Communication. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter, pp. 7592.Google Scholar
Arvaniti, A. & Rathcke, T. (2015). The role of stress in syllable monitoring. Proceedings of the 18th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Glasgow, UK: The University of Glasgow. www.icphs2015.info/pdfs/Papers/ICPHS0212.pdf.Google Scholar
Arvaniti, A. & Rodriquez, T. (2013). The role of rhythm class, speaking rate, and f0 in language discrimination. Laboratory Phonology, 4(1), 738.Google Scholar
Auer, P., Couper-Kuhlen, E. & Müller, F. (1999). Language in Time: The Rhythm and Tempo of Spoken Interaction. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Balasubramanian, T. (1980). Timing in Tamil. Journal of Phonetics, 8, 449–67.Google Scholar
Baltazani, M. (2007). Prosodic rhythm and the status of vowel reduction in Greek. In Selected Papers on Theoretical and Applied Linguistics from the 17th International Symposium on Theoretical and Applied Linguistics, vol. 1. Thessaloniki: Department of Theoretical and Applied Linguistics, pp. 3143.Google Scholar
Barry, W. & Andreeva, Β. (2001). Cross-language similarities and differences in spontaneous speech patterns. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 31, 5166.Google Scholar
Barry, W. J., Andreeva, B., Russo, M., Dimitrova, S. & Kostadinova, T. (2003). Do rhythm measures tell us anything about language type? In Proceedings of 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Barcelona, pp. 2693–6.Google Scholar
Beckman, M. E. (1986). Stress and Non-Stress Accent. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Bertinetto, P. M. (1989). Reflections on the dichotomy ‘stress’ vs. ‘syllable-timing’. Revue de Phonétique Appliquée, 91-92-93, 99130.Google Scholar
Bertrán, A. P. (1999). Prosodic typology: On the dichotomy between stress-timed and syllable-timed languages. Language Design, 2, 103–30.Google Scholar
Bohannon, J., Koch, D., Homm, P. & Driehaus, A. (2015). Chocolate with high cocoa content as a weight-loss accelerator. International Archives of Medicine, Section: Endocrinology 8(55). https://doi.org/10.3823/1654.Google Scholar
Bolton, T. L. (1894). Rhythm. The American Journal of Psychology, 6(2), 145238.Google Scholar
Borzone de Manrique, A. M. & Signorini, A. (1983). Segmental duration and rhythm in Spanish. Journal of Phonetics, 11, 117–28.Google Scholar
Chung, Y. & Arvaniti, A. (2013). Speech rhythm in Korean: Experiments in speech cycling. Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics (POMA): Proceedings of 21st International Congress of Acoustics, Montréal, 2–7 June 2013. http://scitation.aip.org/content/asa/journal/poma.Google Scholar
Clarke, E. F. (1999). Rhythm and timing in music. In Deutsch, D., ed., The Psychology of Music. New York: Academic Press, pp. 473500.Google Scholar
Classe, A. (1939). The Rhythm of English Prose. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Cummins, F. & Port, R. F. (1998). Rhythmic constraints on stress-timing in English. Journal of Phonetics, 31, 139–48.Google Scholar
Cummins, F. (2009). Rhythm as an affordance for the entrainment of movement. Phonetica, 66(1–2), 1528.Google Scholar
Cutler, A. & Otake, T. (1994). Mora or phoneme? Further evidence for language-specific listening. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 824–44.Google Scholar
Cutler, A., Mehler, J., Norris, D. & Seguí, J. (1986). The syllable’s differing role in the segmentation of French and English. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 385400.Google Scholar
Dankovicová, J. & Dellwo, V. (2007). Czech speech rhythm and the rhythm class hypothesis. In Proceedings of 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Saarbrücken, Germany, pp. 1241–4.Google Scholar
Dauer, R. M. (1983). Stress-timing and syllable-timing reanalyzed. Journal of Phonetics, 11, 5162.Google Scholar
Dauer, R. M. (1987). Phonetic and phonological components of language rhythm. In Proceedings of 11th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Tallinn, pp. 447–9.Google Scholar
Dellwo, V. (2006). Rhythm and speech rate: A variation coefficient for deltaC. In Karnowski, P. & Szigeti, I., eds., Language and Language-Processing: Proceedings of the 38th Linguistic Colloquium. Frankfurt: Peter Lang, pp. 231–41.Google Scholar
Dellwo, V., Aschenberner, B., Dancovicová, J. & Wagner, P. (2004). The BonnTempo Corpus and Tools: A database for the combined study of speech rhythm and rate. In Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Spoken Language Processing, Jeju Island, Korea, pp. 777–80.Google Scholar
Dilley, L. C. & McAuley, J. D. (2008). Distal prosodic context affects word segmentation and lexical processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 294311.Google Scholar
Dowling, W. J. & Harwood, D. L. (1986). Music Cognition. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Farnetani, E. & Kori, S. (1990). Rhythmic structure in Italian noun phrases: A study of vowel durations. Phonetica, 47, 5065.Google Scholar
Fletcher, J. (1991). Rhythm and final lengthening in French. Journal of Phonetics, 19(2), 193212.Google Scholar
Fraisse, P. (1963). The Psychology of Time, New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Fraisse, P. (1982). Rhythm and tempo. In Deutsch, D., ed., The Psychology of Music. New York: Academic Press, pp. 149–80.Google Scholar
Friberg, A. & Sundberg, J. (1995). Time discrimination in a monotonic, isochronous sequence. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 98(5), 2524–31.Google Scholar
Frota, S. & Vigário, M. (2001). On the correlates of rhythmic distinctions: The European/Brazilian Portuguese case. Probus, 13, 247–75.Google Scholar
Goswami, U. (2011). A temporal sampling framework for developmental dyslexia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 310.Google Scholar
Goswami, U. & Leong, V. (2013). Speech rhythm and temporal structure: Converging perspectives? Laboratory Phonology, 4(1), 6792.Google Scholar
Grabe, E. & Low, E. L. (2002). Acoustic correlates of rhythm class. In Gussenhoven, C. & Warner, N., eds., Laboratory Phonology 7. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 515–46.Google Scholar
Hannon, E. E., Lévêque, Y., Nave, K. M. & Trehub, S. E. (2016). Exaggeration of language-specific rhythms in English and French children’s songs. Frontiers of Psychology 2016, 7, 939. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00939.Google Scholar
Harris, M. J. & Gries, S. T. (2011). Measures of speech rhythm and the role of corpus-based word frequency: A multifactorial comparison of Spanish(-English) speakers. International Journal of English Studies, 11(2), 122.Google Scholar
Harris, M. J., Gries, S. T. & Miglio, V. G. (2014). Prosody and its applications to forensic linguistics. Linguistic Evidence in Security, Law and Intelligence, 2(2). https://doi.org/10.5195/lesli.2014.12.Google Scholar
Hawkins, S. (2003). Roles and representations of systematic fine phonetic detail in speech understanding. Journal of Phonetics, 31, 373405.Google Scholar
Hawkins, S. (2014). Situational influences on rhythmicity in speech, music, and their interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B. 369, 20130398. https://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0398.Google Scholar
Hayes, B. (1995). Metrical Stress Theory: Principles and Case Studies. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Horton, R. & Arvaniti, A. (2013). Cluster and classes in the rhythm metrics. San Diego Linguistic Papers, 4, 2852. http://escholarship.org/uc/item/0tt1j553.Google Scholar
James, W. (1890/1950). The Principles of Psychology. New York: Dover Reprint. (Originally published 1890.)Google Scholar
Jeon, H. & Arvaniti, A. (2017). The effects of prosodic context on word segmentation: Rhythmic irregularity and localised lengthening in Korean. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 141, 4251–63.Google Scholar
Jinbo, K. (1927/1980). Kokugo no onseijou no tokushitsu [The top phonetic characteristics of Japanese]. In Shibata, T., Kitamura, H. & Kindaichi, H., eds., Nihon no gengogaku [Linguistics of Japan]. Tokyo:Taishukan, pp. 515. (Originally published 1927.)Google Scholar
Jones, D. (1972). An Outline of English Phonetics, 9th ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. (Originally published 1918.)Google Scholar
Jones, M. R. (1981). Only time can tell: On the topology of mental space and time. Critical Inquiry, 7, 557–76.Google Scholar
Jun, S. (2005). Korean intonational phonology and prosodic transcription. In Jun, S., ed., Prosodic Typology: The Phonology of Intonation and Phrasing. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 201–29.Google Scholar
Kaminskaïa, S., Tennant, J. & Russell, A. (2016). Prosodic rhythm in Ontario French. Journal of French Language Studies, 26(2), 183208.Google Scholar
Keane, E. (2006). Rhythmic characteristics of colloquial and formal Tamil. Language and Speech, 49, 299332.Google Scholar
Klatt, D. H. (1976). Linguistic uses of segmental duration in English: Acoustic and perceptual evidence. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 59, 1208–21.Google Scholar
Knight, R. (2011). Assessing the temporal reliability of rhythm metrics. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 41(3), 271–81.Google Scholar
Kohler, K. (2009). Rhythm in speech and language: A new research paradigm. Phonetica, 66, 2945.Google Scholar
Lee, C. S. & Todd, N. P. M. A. (2004). Towards an auditory account of speech rhythm: Application of a model of the auditory ‘primal-sketch’ to two multi-language corpora. Cognition, 9, 225–54.Google Scholar
Lehiste, I. (1977). Isochrony reconsidered. Journal of Phonetics, 5, 253–63.Google Scholar
Lerdahl, F. & Jackendoff, R. (1981). A Generative Theory of Tonal Music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Li, A. & Post, B. (2014). L2 acquisition of prosodic properties of speech rhythm. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 36(2), 223–55.Google Scholar
Lin, H. & Wang, Q. (2007). Mandarin rhythm: An acoustic study. Journal of Chinese Language and Computing, 17(3), 127–40.Google Scholar
Lloyd James, A. (1940). Speech Signals in Telephony. London: Pitman & Sons.Google Scholar
Loehr, D. (2007). Aspects of rhythm in gesture and speech. Gesture, 72, 179214.Google Scholar
London, J. (2012). Hearing in Time: Psychological Aspects of Musical Meter. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Loukina, A., Kochanski, G., Rosner, B., Keane, E. & Shih, C. (2011). Rhythm measures and dimensions of durational variation in speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 129(5), 3258–70.Google Scholar
Low, E. L., Grabe, E. & Nolan, F. (2000). Quantitative characterisations of speech rhythm: ‘Syllable-timing’ in Singapore English. Language and Speech, 43, 377401.Google Scholar
Lowit, A. (2014). Quantification of rhythm problems in disordered speech: A re-evaluation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369 (1658). https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0404.Google Scholar
Luo, H. & Poeppel, D. (2007). Phase patterns of neuronal responses reliably discriminate speech in human auditory cortex. Neuron, 54, 1001–10.Google Scholar
Mattys, S. L. & Melhorn, J. F. (2005). How do syllables contribute to the perception of spoken English? Insight from the migration paradigm. Language and Speech, 48(2), 223–53.Google Scholar
Miller, M. (1984). On the perception of rhythm. Journal of Phonetics, 12, 7583.Google Scholar
Mok, P. (2009). On the syllable-timing of Cantonese and Beijing Mandarin. Chinese Journal of Phonetics, 2, 148–54.Google Scholar
Molnar, M., Gervain, J. & Carreiras, M. (2014). Within-rhythm class native language discrimination abilities of Basque-Spanish monolingual and bilingual infants at 3.5 months of age. Infancy, 19(3), 326–37.Google Scholar
Moon-Hwan, C. (2004). Rhythm typology of Korean speech. Cognitive Processing, 5, 249–53.Google Scholar
Murty, L., Otake, T. & Cutler, A. (2007). Perceptual tests of rhythmic similarity: I. Mora rhythm. Language and Speech, 50, 7799.Google Scholar
Nakatani, L. H., O’Connor, K. D. & Aston, C. H. (1981). Prosodic aspects of American English speech rhythm. Phonetica, 38, 84106.Google Scholar
Nazzi, T. & Ramus, F. (2003). Perception and acquisition of linguistic rhythm by infants. Speech Communication, 41, 233–43.Google Scholar
Nazzi, T., Jusczyk, P. W. & Johnson, E. K. (2000). Language discrimination by English-learning 5-month-olds: Effects of rhythm and familiarity. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 119.Google Scholar
Nespor, M. & Vogel, I. (1989). On clashes and lapses. Phonology, 6, 69116.Google Scholar
Nolan, F. & Asu, E. L. (2009). The Pairwise Variability Index and coexisting rhythms in language. Phonetica, 66, 6477.Google Scholar
Nolan, F. & Jeon, H. (2014). Speech rhythm: A metaphor? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, p. 369. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0396.Google Scholar
Parker Jones, O. (2006). Durational variability and stress-timing in Hawaiian. In P. Warren and C. I. Watson, eds., Proceedings of the 11th Australian International Conference on Speech & Science Technology, pp. 417–20.Google Scholar
Pellegrino, F., Coupé, C. & Marsico, E. (2011). A cross-language perspective on speech information rate. Language, 87, 539–58.Google Scholar
Pike, K. (1945). The Intonation of American English. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Pointon, G. E. (1980). Is Spanish really syllable-timed? Journal of Phonetics, 8, 293304.Google Scholar
Pointon, G. E. (1995). Rhythm and duration in Spanish. In Lewis, J. W., ed., Studies in General and English Phonetics: Essays in Honour of Professor J. D. O’Connor. New York: Routledge, pp. 266–9.Google Scholar
Post, B. & Payne, E. (2018). Speech rhythm in development: What is the child acquiring? In Prieto, P. & Esteve-Gibert, N., eds., The Development of Prosody in First Language Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 125–44.Google Scholar
Prieto, P., Vanrell, M., Astruc, L., Payne, E. & Post, B. (2012). Phonotactic and phrasal properties of speech rhythm: Evidence from Catalan, English, and Spanish. Speech Communication, 54(6), 681702.Google Scholar
Ramus, F., Nespor, M. & Mehler, J. (1999). Correlates of linguistic rhythm in the speech signal. Cognition, 73, 265–92.Google Scholar
Ramus, F., Dupoux, E. & Mehler, J. (2003). The psychological reality of rhythm class: Perceptual studies. In Proceedings of the 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Barcelona, pp. 337–40.Google Scholar
Renwick, M. E. L. (2013). Quantifying rhythm: Interspeaker variation in %V. Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics (POMA), 14, 060011. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.4854657.Google Scholar
Roach, P. (1982). On the distinction between ‘stress-timed’ and ‘syllable-timed’ languages. In Crystal, D., ed., Linguistic Controversies: Essays in Linguistic Theory and Practice in Honour of F. R. Palmer. London: Edward Arnold, pp. 73–9.Google Scholar
Rouas, J., Farinas, J., Pellegrino, F. & André-Obrecht, R. (2005). Rhythmic unit extraction and modelling for automatic language identification. Speech Communication, 47, 436–56.Google Scholar
Scott, D., Isard, S. D. & de Boysson-Bardies, B. (1985). Perceptual isochrony in English and French. Journal of Phonetics, 13, 155–62.Google Scholar
Sebastian, N. & Costa, A. (1997). Metrical information in speech segmentation in Spanish. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12 (5–6), 883–7.Google Scholar
Skoruppa, K., Pons, F., Christophe, A., Bosch, L., Dupoux, E., Sebastián-Gallés, et al. (2009). Language-specific stress perception by nine-month-old French and Spanish infants. Developmental Science, 12(6), 914–19.Google Scholar
Stockmal, V., Markus, D. & Bond, D. (2005). Measures of native and non-native rhythm in a quantity language. Language and Speech, 48, 5563.Google Scholar
Tajima, K. & Port, R. F. (2003). Speech rhythm in English and Japanese. In Local, J., Ogden, R. & Temple, R., eds., Phonetic Interpretation: Papers in Laboratory Phonology VI. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 322–39.Google Scholar
Tan, R. S. K. & Low, E. L. (2014). Rhythmic patterning in Malaysian and Singapore English. Language and Speech, 57(2), 196214.Google Scholar
Tilsen, S. (2016). Selection and coordination: The articulatory basis for the emergence of phonological structure. Journal of Phonetics, 55, 5377.Google Scholar
Tilsen, S. & Arvaniti, A. (2013). Speech rhythm analysis with decomposition of the amplitude envelope: Characterizing rhythmic patterns within and across languages. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 134(1), 628–39.Google Scholar
Tsiartsioni, E. (2003). The Acquisition of Features of Rhythm and Stop Voicing in Greek and English L2. Unpublished M.Phil. Dissertation, Trinity College Dublin.Google Scholar
Turk, A. E. & Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. (2000). Word-boundary-related duration patterns in English. Journal of Phonetics, 28, 397440.Google Scholar
Tzakosta, M. (2004). Acquiring variable stress in Greek: An Optimality-Theoretic approach. Journal of Greek Linguistics, 5, 97125.Google Scholar
Vaissière, J. (1991). Rhythm, accentuation and final lengthening in French. In Sundberg, J., Nord, L. & Carlson, R., eds., Music, Language, Speech and Brain. London: Palgrave, pp. 108–20.Google Scholar
Wagner, P. S. & Dellwo, V. (2004). Introducing YARD (Yet Another Rhythm Determination) and re-introducing isochrony to rhythm research. Proceedings of Speech Prosody, Nara, Japan, 2004. www.isca-speech.org/iscaweb/index.php/archive/online-archive.Google Scholar
Warner, N. & Arai, T. (2001). Japanese mora-timing: A review. Phonetica, 58, 125.Google Scholar
White, L. & Mattys, S. L. (2007). Calibrating rhythm: First language and second language studies. Journal of Phonetics, 35, 501–22.Google Scholar
White, L., Mattys, S. L. & Wiget, L. (2012). Language categorization by adults is based on sensitivity to durational cues, not rhythm class. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 665–79.Google Scholar
Wiget, L., White, L., Schuppler, B., Grenon, I., Rauch, O. & Mattys, S. L. (2010). How stable are acoustic metrics of contrastive speech rhythm? Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 127, 1559–69.Google Scholar
Woodrow, H. (1951). Time perception. In Stevens, S. S., ed., Handbook of Experimental Psychology. New York: Wiley, pp. 1224–36.Google Scholar
Zawaydeh, B. A., Tajima, K. & Kitahara, M. (2002). Discovering Arabic rhythm through a speech cycling task. In Parkinson, D. B. & Benmamoun, E., eds., Perspectives on Arabic Linguistics XIII-XIV. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 3958.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×