Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-03T07:55:28.505Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - The Evolution of Expertise

from Part I - Introduction and Perspectives

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 May 2018

K. Anders Ericsson
Affiliation:
Florida State University
Robert R. Hoffman
Affiliation:
Florida Institute for Human and Machine Cognition
Aaron Kozbelt
Affiliation:
Brooklyn College, City University of New York
A. Mark Williams
Affiliation:
University of Utah
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alexander, R. D. (1990). How did humans evolve? Reflections on the uniquely unique species. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Ambrose, S. H. (2001). Paleolithic technology and human evolution. Science, 291, 17481753.Google Scholar
Bailey, D. H, & Geary, D. C. (2009). Hominid brain evolution: Testing climatic, ecological, and social competition models. Human Nature, 20, 6779.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (1995). Evolutionary psychology: A new paradigm for psychological science. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 130.Google Scholar
Campitelli, G., & Gobet, F. (2011). Deliberate practice necessary but not sufficient. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 280285.Google Scholar
Chapais, B. (2010). The deep structure of human society: Primate origins and evolution. In Kappeler, P. M. & Silk, J. B. (eds.), Mind the gap: Tracing the origins of human universals (pp. 1951). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Conard, N. J. (2010). Cultural modernity: Consensus or conundrum? Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 107, 76217622.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species by means of natural selection, or preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life. London: Murray.Google Scholar
Davis, K., & Moore, W. E. (1945). Some principles of stratification. American Sociological Review, 10, 242249.Google Scholar
Epstein, D. (2014). The sports gene: Inside the science of extraordinary athletic performance. New York: Penguin.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2008). Deliberate practice and acquisition of expert performance: A general overview. Academic Emergency Medicine, 15, 988994.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2014). Expertise. Current Biology, 24, R508R510.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance: Its structure and acquisition. American Psychologist, 49, 725747.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., Prietula, M. J., & Cokely, E. T. (2007). The making of an expert. Harvard Business Review, 85, 114.Google Scholar
ESPN Hollinger Ratings (December 14, 2015). NBA centers player efficiency ratings. http://insider.espn.go.com/nba/hollinger/statistics/_/position/c.Google Scholar
Ferguson, J. R. (2008). The when, where, and how of novices in craft production. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 15, 5167.Google Scholar
Flinn, M. V., Geary, D. C., & Ward, C. V. (2005). Ecological dominance, social competition, and coalitionary arms races: Why humans evolved extraordinary intelligence. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 1046.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (1995). Reflections of evolution and culture in children’s cognition: Implications for mathematical development and instruction. American Psychologist, 50, 2437.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (2005). The origin of mind: Evolution of brain, cognition, and general intelligence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (2007). Educating the evolved mind: Conceptual foundations for an evolutionary educational psychology. In Carlson, J. S. & Levin, J. R. (eds.), Educating the evolved mind: Conceptual foundations for an evolutionary educational psychology (pp. 199, 177202). Greenwich, CT: Information Age.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (2010). Male, female: The evolution of human sex differences (2nd edn.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C., & Huffman, K. J. (2002). Brain and cognitive evolution: Forms of modularity and functions of mind. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 667698.Google Scholar
Harari, Y. N. (2015). Sapiens: A brief history of humankind. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
Hawkes, K., & Bliege Bird, R. (2002). Showing off, handicap signaling, and the evolution of men’s work. Evolutionary Anthropology, 11, 5867.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 165196.Google Scholar
Lycett, S. J., Collard, M., & McGrew, W. C. (2009). Cladistic analyses of behavioural variation in wild Pan troglodytes: Exploring the chimpanzee culture hypothesis. Journal of Human Evolution, 57, 337349.Google Scholar
McAndrew, F. T. (2002). New evolutionary perspectives on altruism: Multilevel-selection and costly-signaling theories. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 7982.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A. (2008). An experimental simulation of the “copy-successful-individuals” cultural learning strategy: Adaptive landscapes, producer–scrounger dynamics, and informational access costs. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 350363.Google Scholar
Miller, G. F. (2001). The mating mind: How sexual choice shaped the evolution of human nature. New York: Anchor.Google Scholar
Olausson, D. J. (2008). Does practice make perfect? Craft expertise as a factor in aggrandizer strategies. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 15, 2850.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2002). The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature. New York: Penguin.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2010). The cognitive niche: Coevolution of intelligence, sociality, and language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 107, 89938999.Google Scholar
Potts, R. (1998). Variability selection in hominid evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 7, 8196.Google Scholar
Potts, R. (2013). Hominin evolution in settings of strong environmental variability. Quaternary Science Reviews, 73, 113.Google Scholar
Searcy, W. A., & Nowicki, S. (2010). The evolution of animal communication: Reliability and deception in signaling systems. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Shultz, S., & Maslin, M. (2013). Early human speciation, brain expansion and dispersal influenced by African climate pulses. PloS ONE, 8, e76750.Google Scholar
Stout, D. (2011). Stone toolmaking and the evolution of human culture and cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 366, 10501059.Google Scholar
Stout, D., Toth, N., Schick, K., & Chaminade, T. (2008). Neural correlates of Early Stone Age toolmaking: Technology, language and cognition in human evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 363, 19391949.Google Scholar
Tooby, J., and Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In Barkow, J., Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. (eds.), The adapted mind (pp. 19136). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Trigger, B. G. (2007). Understanding early civilizations: A comparative study. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wadley, L. (2001). What is cultural modernity? A general view and a South African perspective from Rose Cottage Cave. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 11, 201221.Google Scholar
Winegard, B., Winegard, B., & Geary, D. C. (2014). Eastwood’s brawn and Einstein’s brain: An evolutionary account of dominance, prestige, and precarious manhood. Review of General Psychology, 18, 3448.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R., & Carmody, R. (2010). Human adaptation to the control of fire. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 19, 187199.Google Scholar
Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection: A selection for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53, 205214.Google Scholar
Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (1997). The handicap principle: A missing piece of Darwin’s puzzle. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zilhao, J. (2007). The emergence of ornaments and art: An archaeological perspective on the origins of “behavioral modernity.” Journal of Archaeological Research, 15, 154.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×