Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T16:01:13.929Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

COVID-19, masks and communication in the operating theatre: the importance of face value

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 September 2020

Shazia P. Sharif*
Affiliation:
The Royal London Hospital, London, UK
Elisabeth Blagrove
Affiliation:
University of Warwick, Coventry, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Shazia P Sharif, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Correspondence
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Dear Editors,

We write in response to the study by Zhang et al. (Reference Zhang, Li, Zhu, Niu, Ruan, Liu and Wang2020) ‘COVID-19 pandemic: study on simple, easy, and practical relaxation techniques while wearing medical protective equipment’. The study team has actively investigated potential anxiety-relieving techniques that healthcare professionals can utilise while working and this is commendable. We want to highlight another area that will impact communication, levels of anxiety and fear – medical protective equipment.

Surgical masks have been worn in operating theatres for over a century as outlined by Zhou, Sivathondan, and Handa (Reference Zhou, Sivathondan and Handa2015). The early impetus to wear masks was to prevent the patient's wound becoming contaminated, leading to subsequent infection. More recently, there has been recognition that masks confer some degree of protection to the surgeon in the event of fluid splash. And now with the potential presence of SARS CoV-2, masks in theatre and other clinical settings are an absolute necessity. An et al. (Reference An, Bellato, Konishi, Pellino, Sensi, Siragusa and Sica2020) report that surgeons' have increased feelings of fear at work associated with surgical mask shortage during the pandemic.

The more constrictive FFP3 masks provide protection for the wearer, as well as the patient, and other staff. However, the FFP3 mask is uncomfortable, very tight fitting and muffles the voice. In addition, the surgeon concurrently wears a visor. Combination of these extra protective barriers can be cumbersome and tiring over long period of operations. The mask, in particular, can cause difficulties in communication. This is especially important when other non-verbal communication channels (e.g. gesture, vocalisation features and posture) are also constrained by the surgical context.

Most obviously, masks obscure the mouth and reduce effective signalling of essential non-verbal cues. Green and Phillips (Reference Green and Phillips2004) demonstrated that both eye and mouth facial regions are typically scanned to ascertain facial expression, however obscuring mouths can also be problematic. First, although muscle activation around the mouth provides more obvious emotional messaging (e.g. smiling, grimacing, etc.), this also provides less reliable and/or authentic signals. Second, although the eye region conveys more nuanced affect, it can also be harder to ‘read’ with certainty, as highlighted by Fox and Damjanovic (Reference Fox and Damjanovic2006) and Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, Raste, and Plumb (Reference Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, Raste and Plumb2001). This may be impacted further by a protective visor.

In the operating theatre, and other clinical contexts, surgeons need to be mindful of the impact that wearing masks and visors have on communication. An increased awareness of the potential for miscommunication will enable mitigation against it. Healthcare professionals must be prepared to check instructions have been understood by repeating back, especially during a stressful procedure. We advocate multi-disciplinary research and collaboration between psychologists and medical professionals to explore barriers and solutions in the clinical setting.

References

An, Y., Bellato, V., Konishi, T., Pellino, G., Sensi, B., Siragusa, L., … Sica, G. S. (2020). Surgeons’ fear of getting infected by COVID19: A global survey. British Journal of Surgery, 11(107). doi:10.1002/bjs.11833.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The ‘reading the mind in the eyes’ test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42(2), 241251. doi:10.1017/S0021963001006643.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, E., & Damjanovic, L. (2006). The eyes are sufficient to produce a threat superiority effect. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 6(3), 534539. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, M. J., & Phillips, M. L. (2004). Social threat perception and the evolution of paranoia. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 28(3), 333342. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.03.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, H., Li, A., Zhu, B., Niu, Y., Ruan, Z., Liu, L., … Wang, H. (2020). COVID-19 pandemic: Study on simple, easy, and practical relaxation techniques while wearing medical protective equipment. Psychological Medicine, 17. doi:10.1017/S0033291720003220.Google Scholar
Zhou, C. D., Sivathondan, P., & Handa, A. (2015). Unmasking the surgeons: The evidence base behind the use of facemasks in surgery. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 108(6), 223228. doi:10.1177/0141076815583167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed