Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T13:39:59.492Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Trajectories of functioning in a population-based sample of veterans: contributions of moral injury, PTSD, and depression

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 November 2020

Shira Maguen*
Affiliation:
San Francisco VA Health Care System, San Francisco, CA, USA University of California – San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA
Brandon J. Griffin
Affiliation:
Central Arkansas VA Healthcare System, Little Rock, AR, USA University of Arkansas for Medical Sciences, Little Rock, AR, USA
Laurel A. Copeland
Affiliation:
VA Central Western Massachusetts Healthcare System, Leeds, MA, USA University of Massachusetts Medical School, Worcester, MA, USA
Daniel F. Perkins
Affiliation:
Pennsylvania State University, State College, PA, USA
Cameron B. Richardson
Affiliation:
Pennsylvania State University, State College, PA, USA
Erin P. Finley
Affiliation:
South Texas Veterans Health Care System, San Antonio, TX, USA University of Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio, San Antonio, TX, USA
Dawne Vogt
Affiliation:
National Center for PTSD at VA Boston Health Care System, Boston, MA, USA Boston University School of Medicine, Boston, MA, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Shira Maguen, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Although research has shown that exposure to potentially traumatic and morally injurious events is associated with psychological symptoms among veterans, knowledge regarding functioning impacts remains limited.

Methods

A population-based sample of post-9/11 veterans completed measures of intimate relationship, health, and work functioning at approximately 9, 15, 21, and 27 months after leaving service. Moral injury, posttraumatic stress, and depression were assessed at ~9 months post-separation. We used Latent Growth Mixture Models to identify discrete classes characterized by unique trajectories of change in functioning over time and to examine predictors of class membership.

Results

Veterans were assigned to one of four functioning trajectories: high and stable, high and decreasing, moderate and increasing, and moderate and stable. Whereas posttraumatic stress, depression, and moral injury associated with perpetration and betrayal predicted worse outcomes at baseline across multiple functioning domains, moral injury associated with perpetration and depression most reliably predicted assignment to trajectories characterized by relatively poor or declining functioning.

Conclusions

Moral injury contributes to functional problems beyond what is explained by posttraumatic stress and depression, and moral injury due to perpetration and depression most reliably predicted assignment to trajectories characterized by functional impairment over time.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

In recent years, there has been a shift from focusing on psychological symptoms, disease, and disability to considering the full continua of physical, mental, and social well-being (Cooke, Melchert, & Connor, Reference Cooke, Melchert and Connor2016). Although symptoms are important and can serve as indicators of well-being, functioning also contributes to well-being and is essential to assess in its own right, especially in the course of recovery from psychological trauma. To this end, studies that examine trajectories of posttraumatic stress (e.g. Galatzer-Levy, Huang,& Bonanno, Reference Galatzer-Levy, Huang and Bonanno2018) or depression (e.g. Musliner, Munk-Olsen, Eaton, & Zandi, Reference Musliner, Munk-Olsen, Eaton and Zandi2016) following exposure to stressful life events could be extended by studies that examine how functioning changes over time to improve understanding of recovery from psychological trauma.

Historically, scholars have linked potentially traumatic events (PTEs) to biological and behavioral fear responses endemic to posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). PTSD, especially when it is coupled with depressive symptoms, is associated with functional problems among veterans (Lippa et al., Reference Lippa, Fonda, Fortier, Amick, Kenna, Millberg and McGlinchey2015). Yet, differences in psychological symptoms and functional problems may depend, at least in part, on the nature of the stressful event experienced by an individual. Moral injury is an important related construct that is potentially co-occurring but mechanistically distinct from PTSD and depression (Bryan, Bryan, Roberge, Leifker, & Rozek, Reference Bryan, Bryan, Roberge, Leifker and Rozek2018; Griffin et al., Reference Griffin, Purcell, Burkman, Litz, Bryan, Schmitz and Maguen2019). Events are considered morally injurious if they transgress deeply held moral beliefs and expectations, and the transgression shatters moral or ethical expectations that are derived from religious or spiritual beliefs, or cultural, organizational, and group-based rules (Litz et al., Reference Litz, Stein, Delaney, Lebowitz, Nash, Silva and Maguen2009). Exposure to potentially morally injurious events (PMIEs) may elicit lasting moral injury characterized by biological, psychological/behavioral, social, and religious/spiritual sequelae (Griffin et al., Reference Griffin, Purcell, Burkman, Litz, Bryan, Schmitz and Maguen2019).

Despite a growth of studies focusing on the psychological consequences of moral injury in the context of military service, few studies have explored associations between moral injury and functioning. One study that addressed this topic found that killing in war was associated with lower scores on a broad functioning composite that addressed functioning with respect to employment, education, physical health, and family problems with spouse or children (Maguen et al., Reference Maguen, Metzler, Litz, Seal, Knight and Marmar2009). Another empirical gap is the unknown contribution of moral injury to functional impairment, independent of mental health symptoms. For example, Maguen et al. (Reference Maguen, Metzler, Bosch, Marmar, Knight and Neylan2012) found that even after adjusting for PTSD and depression, killing in war was uniquely associated with suicidal ideation, suggesting that moral injury might explain additional variance in negative outcomes, beyond its relationship with psychological problems.

Whereas these studies establish an association between moral injury and functional problems independent of other mental health symptoms, longitudinal investigations are needed to explore the contributions of moral injury, PTSD, and depression to changes in functioning over the course of recovery from psychological trauma. Better understanding these relationships can inform the development of novel treatments that focus on these shared difficulties and lead to more honed outreach efforts ensuring that individuals suffering with these issues get more targeted treatment.

The goal of this paper was thus to better understand the unique contributions of moral injury related to witnessing, perpetrating, and feeling betrayed, as well as posttraumatic stress and depression symptoms, to trajectories of intimate relationship, health, and occupational functioning among recently separated veterans. Based on prior research indicating that most veterans are resilient (e.g. Galatzer-Levy et al., Reference Galatzer-Levy, Huang and Bonanno2018), we hypothesized that there would be a consistently high functioning trajectory which would comprise the majority of the sample, followed by a high and declining trajectory, a low and increasing trajectory, and a low trajectory that remained stable. Although there has been limited research on factors associated with how functioning changes across time, cross-sectional findings indicate that exposure to PMIEs is associated with functional deficits (e.g. Maguen et al., Reference Maguen, Griffin, Copeland, Perkins, Finley and Vogt2020), especially among those exposed to perpetration-based events who report guilt and shame, negative cognitions related to the self, and self-sabotaging behaviors, and those exposed to betrayal-based events that have difficulty trusting others in the future and feeling safety in relationships. For these reasons, after accounting for the contributions to PTSD and depression, we hypothesized that exposure to PMIEs by perpetration, betrayal, and to a lesser extent witnessing would be associated with a greater likelihood of functional problems, either initially or over time.

Method

Participants and procedure

Data were collected as part of a study of post-9/11 veterans' transitions from military to civilian life, The Veterans Metrics Initiative (TVMI) study (Vogt et al., Reference Vogt, Perkins, Copeland, Finley, Jamieson, Booth and Gilman2018). This prospective cohort study drew from a roster of all separating US service members in fall 2016 identified from the VA/Department of Defense Identity Repository (VADIR). To be included in the sample, participants must have resided within the continental USA and separated from active-duty service or deactivated from the National Guard/Reserve following an activation of at least 180 days within 90 days prior to the first assessment occasion.

A modified Dillman outreach methodology was used, involving multiple contacts by mail and an opportunity to opt-out of additional contacts (Dillman, Smyth, & Christian, Reference Dillman, Smyth and Christian2011). Potential participants were provided a link to a website where they could share their contact information and complete the survey. All potential participants received a pre-incentive of $5 cash, and those who completed surveys received an electronic gift valued at $20 at the first timepoint, with incentives increasing by $5 at each subsequent timepoint (Coughlin et al., Reference Coughlin, Aliaga, Barth, Eber, Maillard, Mahan and Williams2011). Compared to the population of veterans who served in these most recent conflicts, with the exception that lower rank enlisted veterans were less represented, those who completed the initial survey were generally representative of the population in terms of demographic and military characteristics, with few meaningful differences in attrition across subgroups (Vogt et al., Reference Vogt, Perkins, Copeland, Finley, Jamieson, Booth and Gilman2018). A consideration of how rates of probable PTSD and depression compared to other studies also suggests that the sample was generally representative of the larger population in terms of mental health. Specifically, average rates across the timepoints that were the focus of the current investigation (17.1% for PTSD; 17.8% for depression) were similar to the range of two other large-scale, representative studies that have examined veteran's mental health, which found rates of probable PTSD ranging from 13.8 to 23.2% and probable depression ranging from 13.7 to 16.4% (National Academies of Sciences, 2018; Schell & Marshall, Reference Schell, Marshall, Tanielian and Jaycox2008). However, probable PTSD was somewhat lower at the second wave, suggesting that wave 2 probable PTSD rates in this study may somewhat underestimate initial rates of probable PTSD.

Overall, 9566 participants responded at wave 1 (~3 months), 7200 at wave 2 (~9 months), 7201 at wave 3 (~15 months), 6480 at wave 4 (~21 months), and 5844 at wave 5 (~27 months). The current study utilized measures of moral injury, posttraumatic stress, and depression symptoms collected at wave 2. Psychosocial functioning was assessed at each assessment occasion; however, only data collected at waves 2, 3, 4, and 5 were analyzed for this study because changes were made to the functioning measures between waves 1 and 2. For these reasons, our analyses included only those who completed the wave 2 assessment occasion and any amount of the subsequent assessments. The number of participants with one (n = 364, 5.1%), two (n = 650, 9.0%), three (n = 1510, 21.0%) and four (n = 4676, 64.9%) valid responses indicated that the majority of participants completed at least three of four assessment occasions. The mean number of days between when participants completed wave 2 and wave 3 (M = 184.17 days, s.d. = 15.26 days), wave 4 (M = 359.29 days, s.d. = 15.03 days), and wave 5 (M = 543.88 days, s.d. = 13.88 days) showed that participants completed surveys without more than approximately 2 weeks deviation from the due date on average. Demographic and military-related characteristics are displayed in Table 1 (see also online Supplementary Table S1 for bivariate associations).

Table 1. Demographic and military-related characteristics (N = 7200)

Measures

The Well-being Inventory (WBI; Vogt et al., Reference Vogt, Taverna, Nillni, Booth, Perkins, Copeland and Gilman2019) is designed to assess veterans' status, functioning, and satisfaction with regard to work, education, finances, health, and intimate relationships. The functioning scales for selected domains were used in this study: work (4 items; e.g. ‘You completed your work when expected’), health (12 items; e.g. ‘Completed recommended medical care’), and intimate relationships (6 items; e.g. ‘Provided your significant other with the emotional support they sought’). Participants rated each item using a response format from 1 = never to 5 = most or all of the time; items were averaged to create scores for each domain with higher scores indicating better functioning. Psychometric studies support the reliability and validity of the WBI among veterans (Vogt et al., Reference Vogt, Taverna, Nillni, Booth, Perkins, Copeland and Gilman2019). Cronbach's α across timepoints for each domain was acceptable to strong: work (α = 0.82–0.85), health (α = 0.75–0.76), and intimate relationships (α = 0.88–0.89).

The Primary Care PTSD Screen (PC-PTSD-5; Prins et al., Reference Prins, Bovin, Smolenski, Mark, Kimerling, Jenkins-Guarnieri and Tiet2016) is a six-item self-report screening instrument on which participants utilized a dichotomous response format (yes/no) to indicate if they had been exposed to a traumatic event and, if so, whether they had experienced each of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) PTSD symptom domains within the past month (e.g. ‘Have you had nightmares about the event or thought about the event when you did not want to?’). Prins et al. suggest that three is the optimally sensitive cut-point, identifying 94.8% of participants who met criteria for PSTD using the MINI (Sheehan et al., Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier, Sheehan, Amorim, Janavs, Weiller and Dunbar1998). We used the cut-point of three when reporting the percentage of the sample who screened positive for PTSD and a sum total score in regression analyses. Psychometric studies support the use of the PC-PTSD-5 among veterans (Prins et al., Reference Prins, Bovin, Smolenski, Mark, Kimerling, Jenkins-Guarnieri and Tiet2016). Cronbach's α in this sample was strong (α = 0.91).

The Patient Health Questionnaire-2 (PHQ-2; Kroenke, Spitzer, & Williams, Reference Kroenke, Spitzer and Williams2003) is a two-item self-report screening instrument for depression occurring over a 2-week period (e.g. ‘Feeling down, depressed or hopeless’). Participants rated each item using a four-point response format (0 = not at all to 3 = nearly every day). Psychometric studies suggest that three is the optimally sensitive cut-point, identifying 87% of participants who met diagnostic criteria for depression using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (Löwe, Kroenke, & Gräfe, Reference Löwe, Kroenke and Gräfe2005). We used the cut-point of three when reporting the percentage of the sample who screened positive for depression and a sum total score in regression analyses. Psychometric studies support the reliability and validity of the PHQ-2 among veterans receiving care in a Department of Veterans Affairs primary care setting (Corson, Gerrity, & Dobscha, Reference Corson, Gerrity and Dobscha2004). Pearson's r in this sample indicated acceptable reliability (r = 0.80).

The Moral Injury Events Scale (MIES; Nash et al., Reference Nash, Marino Carper, Mills, Au, Goldsmith and Litz2013) was designed as a self-report assessment of exposure to and subjective distress stemming from potentially morally injurious military events. Nine items comprise the scale (e.g. ‘I am troubled by having acted in ways that violated my own morals or values’). Participants rated each item using a response format from 1 = strongly disagree to 6 = strongly agree. Although the proposed three-factor structure has had mixed support in research samples (Bryan et al., Reference Bryan, Bryan, Anestis, Anestis, Green, Etienne and Ray-Sannerud2016; Richardson et al., Reference Richardson, Chesnut, Morgan, Bleser, Perkins, Vogt and Finley2020), conceptually the three-factor structure is most inclusive, consistent with our clinical experience, and provides a nuanced way to look at morally injurious events and their sequelae; the three factors assess moral injury stemming from witnessing, perpetrating or being betrayed. Estimates of internal consistency were acceptable for the two-item witnessing (r = 0.76), three-item betrayal (α = 0.82), and four-item perpetration (α = 0.93) subscales. We calculated a mean total score for each subscale.

Data analysis

Preliminary analyses included missing data diagnostics as well as inspection of the variable distributions. First, we estimated a multiple regression with the total number of missing responses across all waves as the dependent variable [F (7,7048) = 12.86, p < 0.001, R 2 = 0.013], and gender, age, minority racial status, branch of service, and rank as independent variables. Being older (β = 0.10, p < 0.001) and a member of a racial minority group (β = 0.57, p < 0.001) was associated with more missing responses. Being a member of the Air Force in comparison to the Army was associated with fewer missing responses (β = −0.03, p = 0.045). For this reason, we concluded the data to be missing at random (Enders, Reference Enders2010), and full information maximum likelihood was employed to maximize data available for analysis without biasing parameter estimates (Schafer & Graham, Reference Schafer and Graham2002). Next, inspection of the distribution of the functioning outcomes revealed some skewness (max = |1.67|) and kurtosis (max = |4.37|), which we accounted for by utilizing the Maximum Likelihood with Robust Standard Errors estimator (MLR). Analyses were performed using Mplus 8.2 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén1998–2017; syntax is available by request of the corresponding author).

For the main analyses, we employed Latent Growth Mixture Modeling (LGMM) to (1) identify distinct trajectories characterized by heterogeneous patterns of change over time in each functioning domain and (2) test whether class membership depended upon baseline ratings of posttraumatic stress, depression, and moral injury (van de Schoot, Sijbrandij, Winter, Depaoli, & Vermunt, Reference van de Schoot, Sijbrandij, Winter, Depaoli and Vermunt2017). First, by comparing a series of simple growth curve models using χ2 difference tests, we observed that a model with intercept (1,1,1,1), linear (0,1,2,3), and non-linear (0,1,4,9) growth parameters generally fit the data best. Residual variance in participants' observed functioning scores was constrained to be equal. Next, we regressed a latent categorical variable representing a specified number of latent trajectories (or classes) onto the growth parameters, obtaining particular intercept, slope, and quadratic parameters for each class. To determine the optimal number of classes, we compared a series of models with an iteratively greater number of classes. Model comparisons included the Lo-Mendell-Rubin Adjusted Likelihood Ratio Test (LRT) and Boot-Strapped Likelihood Ratio Test (BLRT). A significant p value on these indicates that a model with k classes fits the data better than a model with k–1 classes (Nylund, Asparouhov, & Muthén, Reference Nylund, Asparouhov and Muthén2007). Lower values for the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC), and sample size adjusted Bayesian Information Criteria (aBIC) were preferred (Jung & Wickrama, Reference Jung and Wickrama2008). Entropy values closer to 1.00 indicated greater accuracy of classification (Lubke & Muthén, Reference Lubke and Muthén2007). Finally, we inspected plots of the mean levels of functioning across time by class for each model, and we evaluated the percentage of the sample assigned to each class (Hipp & Bauer, Reference Hipp and Bauer2006). In sum, optimal models were selected based on a preponderance of fit comparisons, theoretical coherence, and interpretability.

Finally, we added participants' ratings of posttraumatic stress, depressive symptoms, and moral injury stemming from witnessing, perpetrating, and being betrayed to the models as predictors of the latent variables. Consistent with Asparouhov and Muthén's (Reference Asparouhov and Muthén2013) recommendations, we estimated the growth parameters while including direct effects from the auxiliary variables. In order to facilitate model convergence, intercepts were freely estimated and residual variances for the linear and quadratic parameters were fixed (see also, deRoon-Cassini, Mancini, Rusch, & Bonanno, Reference deRoon-Cassini, Mancini, Rusch and Bonanno2010). Next, measurement error for the latent categorical variable representing most likely class membership was determined. Finally, we estimated associations between the predictors and latent variable representing most likely class membership, with measurement error specified to the values computed in step two. Results predicting most likely class membership were obtained in the form of multinomial logistic regressions.

Results

Intimate relationship functioning

As is displayed in Table 2, review of the information criterion indices showed lower values with each additional class for models specifying one to six classes. However, the LRT indicated that the six-class solution fit no better than the five-class solution. The five-class solution was also not ideal because less than 1% of the sample was assigned to one of the classes. Inspection of the plotted estimated mean values of intimate relationship functioning across time revealed that the four-class solution contained trajectories that were different in form from trajectories revealed by models with fewer classes (online Supplementary Fig. S1), while the five-class solution split one of the primary trajectories observed in the four-class solution into two separate classes with minor differences. Entropy values indicated acceptable accuracy of class assignment for the four-class solution. We therefore selected the four-class solution as optimal.

Table 2. Fit indices for one to six class latent growth mixture models of functioning outcomes

AIC, Akaike Information Criterion; BIC, Bayesian Information Criterion; aBIC, Sample Size Adjusted Bayesian Information Criterion; LRT, Lo-Mendel-Rubin Likelihood Ratio Test; BLRT, Bootstrapped Likelihood Ratio Test.

a Best-fitting model.

Class-specific intercept, slope, and quadratic growth parameters are reported in Table 3, and estimated means by known class are plotted in Fig. 1 (for observed individual values, see also online Supplementary Fig. S2). Veterans assigned to the first class (23.8%, n = 1538), which we labeled the moderate-stable class, exhibited a moderate level of intimate relationship functioning initially that was stable across time (Hedge's grm = 0.04; Lakens, Reference Lakens2013). Next, a minority of the sample (2.0%, n = 129) was assigned to the moderate-increasing class, characterized by a moderate level of intimate relationship functioning initially that increased with time (Hedge's grm = 3.02). The majority of the sample (71.6%, n = 4633) was assigned to the high-stable class and reported a high level of intimate relationship functioning initially that was maintained over time (Hedge's grm = 0.02). Finally, veterans (2.7%, n = 172) whose intimate relationship functioning was initially high and declined with time were assigned to the high-decreasing class (Hedge's grm = 2.40).

Fig. 1. Estimated means by known class for relationship, health, and work functioning.

Table 3. Growth parameters by known class

Est., estimate; S.E., standard error.

*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

As shown in Table 4, direct effects of the auxiliary variables on the intercept parameter (class-invariant), revealed that higher levels of posttraumatic stress, depression, and moral injury stemming from perpetration and betrayal (but not witnessing) were associated with poorer intimate relationship functioning initially. We then explored associations between the auxiliary variables and class assignment (see Table 5). Higher depression ratings at baseline were associated with greater odds of membership in the moderate-stable and moderate-increasing classes relative to the high-stable class. Higher levels of moral injury stemming from perpetration were associated with greater odds of membership in the high-decreasing and moderate-increasing class relative to the high-stable class. Veterans who reported higher levels of moral injury stemming from betrayal had higher odds of being assigned to the high-stable class relative to the high-decreasing class (perhaps because veterans assigned to the high-stable class on average reported lower levels of intimate relationship functioning when compared to those assigned to the high-decreasing class at baseline but no other assessment occasion, see Fig. 1). Veterans reporting higher levels of moral injury stemming from betrayal also had greater odds of being assigned to the moderate-stable class than the high-decreasing class (OR 1.57, p = 0.005, 95% CI 1.22–2.03) and marginally greater odds of being assigned to the moderate-stable class than the high-stable class (OR 1.10, p = 0.059, 95% CI 1.00–1.20).

Table 4. Regression of intercept growth parameter on covariates

Est., estimate; S.E., standard error.

*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Table 5. Multinomial logistic regressions predicting class membership

Odds Ratios (OR) with 95% Confidence Intervals (95% CI) represent the change in odds of assignment to each comparison class relative to the reference class (High and Stable Functioning) per unit increase in each predictor.

*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Health functioning

Next, we examined veterans' health functioning scores. Review of the information criterion indices showed lower values for models containing an additional class for solutions specifying one to six classes. The LRT indicated that neither the six-class nor five-class solutions were better than simpler models containing one less class. Guidance from the entropy values indicated that the accuracy of class assignment decreased with the specification of additional classes beyond the three-class solution. Inspection of the plotted mean levels of health functioning across time revealed that the three-class solution contained distinct trajectories, while the four-class solution contained an additional class with a minor deviation from one of the three primary trajectories (online Supplementary Fig. S3). Thus, we selected the three-class solution as optimal.

Class-specific intercept, slope, and quadratic growth parameters are reported in Table 3, and estimated means by known class are displayed in Fig. 1 (for observed individual values, see also online Supplementary Fig. S4). The majority of the sample was assigned to the first class (95.3%, n = 6814) and reported high initial levels of health functioning that were stable over time (Hedge's grm = 0.11), establishing the high-stable class. Next, a considerable minority (3.3%, n = 238) was assigned to the high-decreasing class, characterized by a high level of health functioning initially that declined over time (Hedge's grm = 1.83). The third class (1.4%, n = 99), which we labeled as the moderate-increasing class, exhibited a moderate level of health functioning initially that improved with time (Hedge's grm = 2.17).

Higher levels of posttraumatic stress, depression, and moral injury stemming from perpetration and betrayal were associated with poorer health functioning at baseline, and higher levels of moral injury from witnessing were associated with better health functioning at baseline (see Table 4). As is shown in Table 5, higher levels of moral injury from perpetration were associated with greater odds of membership in the high-decreasing class and moderate-increasing relative to the high-stable class. Higher depression scores at baseline were associated with lower odds of membership in the high-decreasing class relative to the high-stable class (again with the high-stable class on average reporting lower levels of health functioning when compared to those assigned to the high-decreasing class at baseline but no other assessment occasion, see Fig. 1).

Work functioning

Finally, we examined veterans' work functioning. The information criterion indices indicated that fit improved with the specification of an additional class for models specifying one to six classes. Guidance from the LRT suggested that neither the six-class model nor the four-class model fits the data any better than models specifying one less class. Thus, either the three- or five-class solutions appeared best. The three- and five-class solutions demonstrated equivalent classification accuracy according to the entropy values. To adjudicate these results, we inspected plots of the mean values of work functioning across time (online Supplementary Fig. S1), and we examined the percentage of the sample assigned to each class. Whereas the three-class solution revealed trajectories with differing form and larger sample sizes by class, the five-class solution extracted additional classes that were only slightly different in form and contained few veterans (online Supplementary Fig. S5). We therefore selected the three-class solution as optimal.

Class-specific intercept, slope, and quadratic growth parameters are reported in Table 3, and estimated means by known class are displayed in Fig. 1 (for observed individual values, see also online Supplementary Fig. S6). The first class (3.7%, n = 249), which we labeled as the moderate-increasing class, exhibited moderate levels of work functioning initially that improved with time (Hedge's grm = 2.10). Next, a considerable minority of the sample (7.2%, n = 478) was assigned to the high-decreasing class, reporting a high initial level of work functioning that decreased with time (Hedge's grm = 0.98). Finally, the majority of the sample (89.1%, n = 5960) was assigned to the high-stable class, which was characterized by a high level of work functioning initially that was maintained over time (Hedge's grm = 0.17).

Higher levels of posttraumatic stress, depression, and moral injury from perpetration and betrayal were associated with poorer initial levels work functioning (see Table 4). Nevertheless, only higher levels of depression and moral injury from perpetration were associated with greater odds of membership in the high-decreasing and moderate-increasing classes relative to the high-stable class (see Table 5). Higher levels of moral injury from witnessing were associated with greater odds of membership in the moderate-increasing class relative to the high-stable class.

Discussion

Consistent with prior studies that have examined the trajectories of posttraumatic stress among adults exposed to PTEs (e.g. Donoho, Bonanno, Porter, Kearney, & Powell, Reference Donoho, Bonanno, Porter, Kearney and Powell2017; Galatzer-Levy et al., Reference Galatzer-Levy, Huang and Bonanno2018), we found distinct functioning trajectories among veterans following separation from military service. These trajectories include high initial levels of functioning that were maintained across time, moderate initial levels of functioning that were maintained across time, high initial levels of functioning that declined with time, and moderate initial levels of functioning that improved with time. In this population-based sample, most veterans maintained a relatively high level of functioning after separation from military service in intimate relationship, health and work functioning; however, notable proportions of the sample reported difficulties either initially or as time progressed. Functional problems during the first few years post-military depended in part on the types of psychological distress reported when separating from service. Depression in particular, more so than PTSD, was a strong and consistent predictor of initial deficits, with some reporting difficulties that persisted over time and others reporting recovery to initial levels of functioning. Interestingly, prior literature that has explored the impact of both PTSD and depression on functioning has found varying results, depending on the age group and type of functioning assessed. It is rare that PTSD and depression are examined in relationship to multiple functional outcomes, so this is a novel component of this study. More specifically, some studies have found that PTSD and depression equally impact functioning (e.g. disability in older veterans; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Spiro, Frayne, Kimerling, Cypel, Reinhard and Magruder2020), PTSD impacts functioning more than depression (educational functioning in student veterans; Morissette et al., Reference Morissette, Ryan-Gonzalez, Yufik, DeBeer, Kimbrel, Sorrells and Meyer2019), or depression impacts functioning more than PTSD (physical health functioning in service members; Asnaani et al., Reference Asnaani, Kaczkurkin, Benhamou, Yarvis, Peterson and Young-McCaughan2018). Future studies are needed to better understand which types of functioning are most impacted by depression.

Moral injury stemming from perpetration was consistently associated with worse functioning, more so than moral injuries from witnessing or betrayal, even after accounting for the impact of posttraumatic stress and depression. This is consistent with prior studies finding that killing in war, especially involving civilian women and children, is associated with poorer post-military outcomes (e.g. Maguen et al., Reference Maguen, Metzler, Litz, Seal, Knight and Marmar2009). Veterans with moral injury due to killing may self-sabotage their well-being due to beliefs that they do not deserve supportive relationships, healthy lifestyles, or desirable jobs (Litz et al., Reference Litz, Stein, Delaney, Lebowitz, Nash, Silva and Maguen2009; Purcell, Koenig, Bosch, & Maguen, Reference Purcell, Koenig, Bosch and Maguen2016). We extend prior findings by using a longitudinal framework to demonstrate that those with moral injury stemming from perpetration may have had initial high levels of functioning that then declined over time in relationship, health, and work domains. There is evidence that the impact of moral injury may unfold over time and that coming to terms with integrating what happened in war with one's current identity can be a process (Purcell et al., Reference Purcell, Koenig, Bosch and Maguen2016).

Although treatments are emerging that deal with the direct impact of killing in war (e.g. Burkman, Purcell, & Maguen, Reference Burkman, Purcell and Maguen2019; Maguen et al., Reference Maguen, Burkman, Madden, Dinh, Bosch, Keyser and Neylan2017; Purcell, Burkman, Keyser, Fucella, & Maguen, Reference Purcell, Burkman, Keyser, Fucella and Maguen2018a, Reference Purcell, Griffin, Burkman and Maguen2018b) and moral injury more generally (Gray et al., Reference Gray, Schorr, Nash, Lebowitz, Amidon, Lansing and Litz2012; Harris et al., Reference Harris, Usset, Voecks, Thuras, Currier and Erbes2018; Norman, Wilkins, Myers, & Allard, Reference Norman, Wilkins, Myers and Allard2014), assessment of exposure to PMIEs is still not part of routine clinical care (Maguen & Burkman, Reference Maguen and Burkman2013). It is important that clinicians assess for PMIEs and moral injury in a thoughtful and sensitive manner and feel well-trained to do so. Given that some clinicians may feel that moral injury assessment and treatment go beyond their areas of expertise, it may be beneficial to allocate additional resources for training in moral injury and associated areas (e.g. spirituality). Overall, preventive efforts aimed at facilitating positive post-deployment functioning may therefore be improved by clinical assessment and treatment of moral injury. This is especially critical given the association between killing and suicide, even after accounting for a number of mental health outcomes (Maguen et al., Reference Maguen, Metzler, Bosch, Marmar, Knight and Neylan2012).

Veterans may also be distressed by exposure to transgressive acts for which others are responsible, either by witnessing or being betrayed. In the current study, betrayal-based moral injury was associated with worse relationship, health, and occupational functioning initially, although betrayal was not the strongest driver of functional problems over time when accounting for depression and PTSD. An area for continued examination is how moral injuries driven by others can impact interpersonal functioning and seeking care. For example, recent studies found a link between betrayal, and poorer health status (e.g. Klest, Tamaian, & Boughner, Reference Klest, Tamaian and Boughner2019), which appeared to depend in part on non-adherence to medical treatment and distrust toward providers.

Another important implication of this study relates to the conceptualization of moral injury following exposure to transgressive events for which others are responsible. Namely, a variety of factor structures have been proposed for the MIES (Bryan et al., Reference Bryan, Bryan, Anestis, Anestis, Green, Etienne and Ray-Sannerud2016; Nash et al., Reference Nash, Marino Carper, Mills, Au, Goldsmith and Litz2013; Richardson et al., Reference Richardson, Chesnut, Morgan, Bleser, Perkins, Vogt and Finley2020), some that aggregate witnessing and being betrayed and others that separate the two. Findings from this study support disaggregating betrayal and witnessing as evidenced by divergent patterns of association between witnessing and betrayal with functional outcomes. For instance, betrayal was associated with poorer health functioning initially, and witnessing was associated with greater health functioning initially. Those who witnessed were also more likely to report work functioning that improved with time as opposed to stable and high work functioning. Additional psychometric work may be needed to better capture the aspects of moral injury by witnessing, especially among individuals, who despite providing excellent care, are unable to prevent others from suffering the inevitable injuries of war (e.g. combat medics, mortuary affairs, chaplains).

By better understanding how moral injury contributes to changes in psychosocial functioning during the process of post-deployment reintegration, we can hone assessment, and treatment to aid those at highest risk of adverse outcomes over time. More specifically, these trajectories could contribute to better tracking of functional outcomes over time as part of the treatment process, better measurement of unique post-deployment mental health issues (including PMIEs), and subsequently to more targeted treatment. For example, we know that those that have exposures to potentially morally injurious perpetration events have poorer functioning outcomes, yet these exposures are not systematically measured when a veteran engages in treatment, nor are functional outcomes. Veterans with these exposures can be referred to more targeted moral injury treatment that specifically targets functioning rather than referred to a general PTSD treatment that may not hone-in on the most problematic exposures, which can be missed within the PTSD framework.

Finally, prior research has found that morally injurious experiences are associated with profound guilt and shame (Griffin et al., Reference Griffin, Purcell, Burkman, Litz, Bryan, Schmitz and Maguen2019), which are often considered hallmark symptoms of moral injury and may act as mediators between exposures and functional impairment (Norman et al., Reference Norman, Haller, Kim, Allard, Porter and Stein2018). It may also be that perpetration, in particular, compared to witnessing or betrayal, is associated with higher guilt and shame, which may explain some of our findings. While many veterans are exposed to PMIEs, not all go on to develop functional impairment, so it is important to better understand some of the mediators that may contribute to this impact. In addition to guilt and shame, other studies have found that factors such as self-forgiveness, meaning-making, and a sense of entrapment can all be associated with adverse outcomes (Currier, Holland, & Malott, Reference Currier, Holland and Malott2015; Levi-Belz & Zerach, Reference Levi-Belz and Zerach2018; Purcell et al., Reference Purcell, Burkman, Keyser, Fucella and Maguen2018a, Reference Purcell, Griffin, Burkman and Maguen2018b) and these mediators should continue to be explored in the context or moral injury.

Limitations and future directions

Limitations of these findings are important to note. First, the percentages of the sample assigned to each class and the severity of reported distress may differ in other samples. In a large sample where most individuals function well, identifying chronically low functioning trajectories is difficult given the small number of individuals assigned to this group. Studies of treatment-seeking veterans may reveal more nuanced differences between trajectories (e.g. whether decline is early or delayed) and increase proportions of the sample assigned to classes characterized by impairment, thereby enhancing the utility of multinomial logistic regressions predicting class membership using additional variables (e.g. demographic and military-related characteristics). Second, results may differ in veterans of earlier eras. Specifically, health functioning in this sample was generally high, given that participants were primarily young and healthy veterans of more recent conflicts in Afghanistan and Iraq. Moreover, though the trajectories of relationship and work functioning identified using the full sample replicated in split half subsamples comprised of randomly selected cases, trajectories of health functioning replicated in one randomly selected subsample (i.e. high and stable, high and decreasing, moderate and increasing) but not another. In the other subsample, those assigned to the high and decreasing class were split according to whether impairment in health functioning was immediate or delayed. For this reason, replication studies that examine health functioning are especially important, including among veterans from combat eras earlier than the post-9/11 wars, because the downstream effects of stress-related issues may be delayed and perhaps more evident in older individuals. Third, the emerging literature on moral injury is still wrought with psychometric challenges, and further psychometric work is needed to develop a measure of morally injurious outcomes. Ideally, instruments measuring moral injury could clearly distinguish between exposures to PMIEs, including types, frequency and intensity of exposures, as well as emotional, cognitive, and behavioral manifestations of moral injury. Establishing a gold-standard clinical interview as well as self-report measure with specific cutoff scores to distinguish those with and without moral injury can assist with collecting more targeted and precise information about moral injury exposures and outcomes. Fourth, use of the PHQ-2 and other screening measures to capture mental health symptoms, rather than gold-standard clinical interviews or more comprehensive measures, could have introduced bias and future studies should employ the latter to add to the literature. Fifth, although the average rates of probable PTSD and depression fall within the range of other representative studies, rates of probable PTSD were somewhat lower in the second wave. While this could reflect a combination of both recovery over time as well as the fact that other studies were cross-sectional, it remains possible that wave 2 probable PTSD rates may somewhat underestimate initial rates of probable PTSD.

Given the dynamic process of both mental health symptoms as well as moral injury, further research is needed. Future studies should explore how moral injury unfolds over time, given that most studies examining moral injury have been cross-sectional. More specifically, it is important to better understand at what point after the PMIEs moral injury is most likely to emerge, whether there can be delayed moral injury, especially as veterans adjust to their post-deployment lives, and whether moral injury is a stable construct or tends to fluctuate over the life course (as well as any factors associated with these potential fluctuations).

Conclusion

In this population-based, longitudinal sample of post-9/11 veterans, we found four classes of relationship, health, and work functioning, with the majority falling in the high and stable functioning class across each domain. Whereas posttraumatic stress, depression, and moral injury stemming from perpetration and betrayal predicted worse functioning outcomes at baseline, moral injury due to perpetration and depression most reliably predicted assignment to trajectories characterized by functional impairment over time. Moral injury related to witnessing and betrayal were less frequent predictors of poor functioning when accounting for mental health and other moral injuries. Additional research is needed to understand the relationship between moral injury and functioning, especially as it relates to suicide-related behavior, hazardous substance use, and other important military health problems.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720004249

Financial support

This research was managed by the Henry M. Jackson Foundation for the Advancement of Military Medicine, Inc. (HJF); and collaboratively sponsored by the Bob Woodruff Foundation, Health Net Federal Services, The Heinz Endowments, HJF, Lockheed Martin Corporation, May and Stanley Smith Charitable Trust, National Endowment for the Humanities, Northrop Grumman, Philip and Marge Odeen, Prudential, Robert R. McCormick Foundation, Rumsfeld Foundation, Schultz Family Foundation, Walmart Foundation, Wounded Warrior Project, Inc., and the Veterans Health Administration Health Services Research and Development Service FOP-15-464, with additional support from VA RR&D grant I01RX002946 (PI: Maguen). The views expressed are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views or policy of the Department of Veterans Affairs or the United States Government.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Asnaani, A., Kaczkurkin, A. N., Benhamou, K., Yarvis, J. S., Peterson, A. L., Young-McCaughan, S., … STRONG STAR Consortium (2018). The influence of posttraumatic stress disorder on health functioning in active-duty military service members. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 31(2), 307316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2013). Auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: Three-step approaches using M plus. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 21(3), 329341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryan, C. J., Bryan, A. O., Anestis, M. D., Anestis, J. C., Green, B. A., Etienne, N., … Ray-Sannerud, B. (2016). Measuring moral injury: Psychometric properties of the moral injury events scale in two military samples. Assessment, 23, 557570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryan, C. J., Bryan, A. O., Roberge, E., Leifker, F. R., & Rozek, D. C. (2018). Moral injury, posttraumatic stress disorder, and suicidal behavior among National Guard personnel. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice and Policy, 10(1), 3645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burkman, K., Purcell, N., & Maguen, S. (2019). Provider perspectives on a novel moral injury treatment for veterans: Initial assessment of acceptability and feasibility of the Impact of Killing treatment materials. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 75, 7994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooke, P. J., Melchert, T. P., & Connor, K. (2016). Measuring well-being: A review of instruments. The Counseling Psychologist, 44, 730757.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corson, K., Gerrity, M. S., & Dobscha, S. K. (2004). Screening for depression and suicidality in a VA primary care setting: 2 items are better than 1 item. The American Journal of Managed Care, 10(11 Pt 2), 839845.Google Scholar
Coughlin, S. S., Aliaga, P., Barth, S., Eber, S., Maillard, J., Mahan, C., & Williams, M. (2011). The effectiveness of a monetary incentive on response rates in a survey of recent U. S. veterans. Survey Practice, 4, 19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Currier, J. M., Holland, J. M., & Malott, J. (2015). Moral injury, meaning making, and mental health in returning veterans. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 71(3), 229240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
deRoon-Cassini, T. A., Mancini, A. D., Rusch, M. D., & Bonanno, G. A. (2010). Psychopathology and resilience following traumatic injury: A latent growth mixture model analysis. Rehabilitation Psychology, 55, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dillman, D. A., Smyth, J. D., & Christian, L. M. (2011). Internet, mail, and mixed-mode surveys: The tailored design method. New York, NY: Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Donoho, C. J., Bonanno, G. A., Porter, B., Kearney, L., & Powell, T. M. (2017). A decade of war: Prospective trajectories of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms among deployed US military personnel and the influence of combat exposure. American Journal of Epidemiology, 186, 13101318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied missing data analysis. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Galatzer-Levy, I. R., Huang, S. H., & Bonanno, G. A. (2018). Trajectories of resilience and dysfunction following potential trauma: A review and statistical evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 63, 4155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, M. J., Schorr, Y., Nash, W., Lebowitz, L., Amidon, A., Lansing, A., … Litz, B. T. (2012). Adaptive disclosure: An open trial of a novel exposure-based intervention for service members with combat-related psychological stress injuries. Behavior Therapy, 43, 407415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffin, B. J., Purcell, N., Burkman, K., Litz, B. T., Bryan, C. J., Schmitz, M., … Maguen, S. (2019). Moral injury: An integrative review. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 32(3), 350362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, J. I., Usset, T., Voecks, C., Thuras, P., Currier, J., & Erbes, C. (2018). Spiritually integrated care for PTSD: A randomized controlled trial of ‘Building Spiritual Strength’. Psychiatry Research, 267, 420428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hipp, J. R., & Bauer, D. J. (2006). Local solutions in the estimation of growth mixture models. Psychological Methods, 11, 3653.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jung, T., & Wickrama, A. S. (2008). An introduction to latent class growth analysis and growth mixture modeling. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 302317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klest, B., Tamaian, A., & Boughner, E. (2019). A model exploring the relationship between betrayal trauma and health: The roles of mental health, attachment, trust in healthcare systems, and nonadherence to treatment. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 11(6), 656662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroenke, K., Spitzer, R. L., & Williams, J. B. (2003). The Patient Health Questionnaire-2: Validity of a two-item depression screener. Medical Care, 41(11), 12841292. doi: 10.1097/01.MLR.0000093487.78664.3C.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lakens, D. (2013). Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: A practical primer for t tests and ANOVAs. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levi-Belz, Y., & Zerach, G. (2018). Moral injury, suicide ideation, and behavior among combat veterans: The mediating roles of entrapment and depression. Psychiatry Research, 269, 508516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lippa, S. M., Fonda, J. R., Fortier, J. R., Amick, M. A., Kenna, A., Millberg, W. P., & McGlinchey, R. E. (2015). Deployment-related psychiatric and behavioral conditions and their association with functional disability in OEF/OIF/OND Veterans. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 28, 2533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Litz, B. T., Stein, N., Delaney, E., Lebowitz, L., Nash, W. P., Silva, C., & Maguen, S. (2009). Moral injury and moral repair in war veterans: A preliminary model and intervention strategy. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 695706.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Löwe, B., Kroenke, K., & Gräfe, K. (2005). Detecting and monitoring depression with a two-item questionnaire (PHQ-2). Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 58, 163171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubke, G., & Muthén, B. O. (2007). Performance of factor mixture models as a function of model size, covariate effects, and class-specific parameters. Structural Equation Modeling, 14, 2647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maguen, S., & Burkman, K. (2013). Combat-related killing: Expanding evidence-based treatments for PTSD. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 20(4), 476479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maguen, S., Burkman, K., Madden, E., Dinh, J., Bosch, J., Keyser, J., … Neylan, T. C. (2017). Impact of killing in war: A randomized, controlled pilot trial. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 73, 9971012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maguen, S., Griffin, B. J., Copeland, L. A., Perkins, D. F., Finley, E. P., & Vogt, D. (2020). Gender differences in prevalence and outcomes of exposure to potentially morally injurious events among post-9/11 veterans. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 130, 97103. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.06.020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maguen, S., Metzler, T. J., Bosch, J., Marmar, C. R., Knight, S. J., & Neylan, T. C. (2012). Killing in combat may be independently associated with suicidal ideation. Depression and Anxiety, 29, 918923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maguen, S., Metzler, T. J., Litz, B. T., Seal, K. H., Knight, S. J., & Marmar, C. R. (2009). The impact of killing in war on mental health symptoms and related functioning. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 22, 435443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morissette, S. B., Ryan-Gonzalez, C., Yufik, T., DeBeer, B. B., Kimbrel, N. A., Sorrells, A. M., … Meyer, E. C. (2019). The effects of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms on educational functioning in student veterans. Psychological Services. doi: 10.1037/ser0000356. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/ser0000356.Google ScholarPubMed
Musliner, K. L., Munk-Olsen, T., Eaton, W. W., & Zandi, P. P. (2016). Heterogeneity in long-term trajectories of depressive symptoms: Patterns, predictors and outcomes. Journal of Affective Disorders, 192, 199211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2017). Mplus user's guide (8th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.Google Scholar
Nash, W. P., Marino Carper, T. L., Mills, M. A., Au, T., Goldsmith, A., & Litz, B. T. (2013). Psychometric evaluation of the moral injury events scale. Military Medicine, 178, 646652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
National Academies of Sciences. (2018) Engineering, and medicine; health and medicine division; board on health care services; committee to evaluate the Department of Veterans Affairs Mental Health Services. Evaluation of the Department of Veterans Affairs Mental Health Services. Washington, DC: National Academies Press (US). Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK499503/ doi: 10.17226/24915.Google Scholar
Norman, S. B., Haller, M., Kim, H. M., Allard, C. B., Porter, K. E., Stein, M. B., … Progress Team (2018). Trauma related guilt cognitions partially mediate the relationship between PTSD symptom severity and functioning among returning combat veterans. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 100, 5662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norman, S. B., Wilkins, K. C., Myers, U. S., & Allard, C. B. (2014). Trauma informed guilt reduction therapy with combat veterans. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 21, 7888.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nylund, K. L., Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. O. (2007). Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 14(4), 535569.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prins, A., Bovin, M. J., Smolenski, D. J., Mark, B. P., Kimerling, R., Jenkins-Guarnieri, M. A., … Tiet, Q. Q. (2016). The primary care PTSD screen from DSM-5: Development and evaluation within a veteran primary care sample. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 31, 12061211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purcell, N., Burkman, K., Keyser, J., Fucella, P., & Maguen, S. (2018a). Healing from moral injury: A qualitative evaluation of the impact of killing treatment for combat veterans. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 27, 645673.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purcell, N., Griffin, B. J., Burkman, K., & Maguen, S. (2018b). ‘Opening a Door to a New Life’: The role of forgiveness in healing from moral injury. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9, 498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purcell, N., Koenig, C. J., Bosch, J., & Maguen, S. (2016). Veterans’ perspectives on the psychosocial impact of killing in war. The Counseling Psychologist, 44, 10621099.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richardson, C. B., Chesnut, R. P., Morgan, N. R., Bleser, J. A., Perkins, D. F., Vogt, D., … Finley, E. (2020). Examining the factor structure of the moral injury events scale in a veteran sample. Military Medicine, 185(1-2), e75e83. doi: 10.1093/milmed/usz129.Google Scholar
Schafer, J. L., & Graham, J. W. (2002). Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods, 7, 147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schell, T. L., & Marshall, G. N. (2008). Survey of individuals previously deployed for OEF/OIF. In Tanielian, T., & Jaycox, L.H. (Eds.), Invisible wounds of war: Psychological and cognitive injuries, their consequences, and services to assist recovery (pp. 87115). Santa Monica, CA: Rand Corporation.Google Scholar
Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., … Dunbar, G. C. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59, 2233.Google ScholarPubMed
Smith, B. N., Spiro, A., Frayne, S. M., Kimerling, R., Cypel, Y., Reinhard, M. J., … Magruder, K. M. (2020). Impact of wartime stress exposures and mental health on later-life functioning and disability in Vietnam-Era women veterans: Findings from the health of Vietnam-Era women's study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 82(2), 147157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van de Schoot, R., Sijbrandij, M., Winter, S. D., Depaoli, S., & Vermunt, J. K. (2017). The GRoLTS-checklist: Guidelines for reporting on latent trajectory studies. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 24, 451467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vogt, D., Perkins, D. F., Copeland, L. A., Finley, E. Pl, Jamieson, C. S., Booth, B., … Gilman, C. L. (2018). The Veterans Metrics Initiative study of US veterans’ experiences during their transition from military service. British Medical Journal, 8, e020734. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2017-020734.Google ScholarPubMed
Vogt, D., Taverna, E., Nillni, Y. I., Booth, B., Perkins, D. F., Copeland, L. A., … Gilman, C. L. (2019). Development and validation of a tool to assess military veterans’ status, functioning, and satisfaction with key aspects of their lives. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 11, 328349.Google ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic and military-related characteristics (N = 7200)

Figure 1

Table 2. Fit indices for one to six class latent growth mixture models of functioning outcomes

Figure 2

Fig. 1. Estimated means by known class for relationship, health, and work functioning.

Figure 3

Table 3. Growth parameters by known class

Figure 4

Table 4. Regression of intercept growth parameter on covariates

Figure 5

Table 5. Multinomial logistic regressions predicting class membership

Supplementary material: File

Maguen et al. supplementary material

Maguen et al. supplementary material

Download Maguen et al. supplementary material(File)
File 13.2 MB