Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T19:03:13.983Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Strengthening through adversity: The hormesis model in developmental psychopathology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 March 2024

Assaf Oshri*
Affiliation:
Department of Human Development and Family Science, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA
Cullin J. Howard
Affiliation:
Department of Human Development and Family Science, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA
Linhao Zhang
Affiliation:
Department of Human Development and Family Science, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA
Ava Reck
Affiliation:
Department of Human Development and Family Science, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA
Zehua Cui
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, USA
Sihong Liu
Affiliation:
Graduate School of Education, Stanford University, Palo Alto, CA, USA
Erinn Duprey
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
Avary I. Evans
Affiliation:
Department of Human Development and Family Science, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA
Rabeeh Azarmehr
Affiliation:
Department of Human Development and Family Science, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA
Charles F. Geier
Affiliation:
Department of Human Development and Family Science, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA
*
Corresponding author: A. Oshri; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background:

Employing a developmental psychopathology framework, we tested the utility of the hormesis model in examining the strengthening of children and youth through limited levels of adversity in relation to internalizing and externalizing outcomes within a brain-by-development context.

Methods:

Analyzing data from the Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development study (N = 11,878), we formed latent factors of threat, deprivation, and unpredictability. We examined linear and nonlinear associations between adversity dimensions and youth psychopathology symptoms and how change of resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) in the default mode network (DMN) from Time 1 to Time 5 moderates these associations.

Results:

A cubic association was found between threat and youth internalizing problems; low-to-moderate family conflict levels reduced these problems. Deprivation also displayed a cubic relation with youth externalizing problems, with moderate deprivation levels associated with fewer problems. Unpredictability linearly increased both problem types. Change in DMN rsFC significantly moderated the cubic link between threat levels and internalizing problems, with declining DMN rsFC levels from Time 1 to Time 5 facilitating hormesis. Hormetic effects peaked earlier, emphasizing the importance of sensitive periods and developmental timing of outcomes related to earlier experiences.

Conclusions:

Strengthening through limited environmental adversity is crucial for developing human resilience. Understanding this process requires considering both linear and nonlinear adversity-psychopathology associations. Testing individual differences by brain and developmental context will inform preventive intervention programming.

Type
Special Issue Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Prologue

Developmental psychopathology (DP) is a system perspective on human development, focusing on the origins and course of individual patterns of behavioral adaptation and maladaptation. DP considers these patterns within the context of normal versus abnormal developmental processes while considering typical and atypical developmental processes and outcomes (Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2009). Dr Dante Cicchetti, a pioneer in the field, has made numerous substantial theoretical and empirical contributions to the field of developmental psychopathology. Notably, Dr Cicchetti’s research and theory have addressed heterogeneity in developmental outcomes and advocated multilevel research approaches (Rutter, Reference Rutter2006, Reference Rutter2012). In the words of Sir Rutter (Reference Rutter and Hudziak2008), “Cicchetti (Reference Cicchetti, Rolf, Masten, Cicchetti, Nüchterlein and Weintraub1990) similarly emphasized the need to focus on causal processes but went further in insisting that the scientific enterprise had to incorporate genetics and neuroscience as well as psychological studies. One particular contribution of his was the emphasis on the need to recognize the diverse pathways that could lead to the same endpoint….” The present paper will utilize these advances in developmental psychopathology to delve into heterogeneity in human resilience. We adopt a multilevel approach to study the nonlinear brain and behavioral mechanisms of human resilience to environmental adversity.

In the past 30 years, developmental scientists have reached a consensus that resilience is a dynamic process in which, despite significant adversity, children and youth benefit from protective factors (e.g., assets and resources) over time and follow positive trajectories across multiple functioning domains (Curtis & Cicchetti, Reference Curtis and Cicchetti2003; Luthar & Zelazo, Reference Luthar and Zelazo2003; Masten, Reference Masten2001). The study of resilience is an integral part of the DP paradigm (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti2010; Masten et al., Reference Masten, Best and Garmezy1990; Rutter, Reference Rutter2012). Indeed, DP research has been transformative in advancing resilience science by focusing on continuities and discontinuities in human development over time (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti2010; Masten et al., Reference Masten, Best and Garmezy1990; Rutter, Reference Rutter2012). Increasingly, scholars interested in resilience research have shifted their investigations to a multilevel and multimethod approach, modeling risk and protective factors mediating and moderating resilience processes. Developmental psychopathology tenets, including equifinality and multifinality, have influenced these multimethod and multilevel approaches. The idea that organisms, including humans, who were exposed to similar adverse environments may follow diverse developmental trajectories and outcomes has been referred to as the concept of multifinality (Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Klein, Crowell, Derbidge and Gatzke-Kopp2009; Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch1996; Von Bertalanffy, Reference Von Bertalanffy1972). Conversely, the idea of equifinality suggests that individuals exposed to different environmental conditions might, over time, exhibit similar outcomes (Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch1996). Recent advances in methodological and statistical tools have enabled researchers to conduct rigorous research informed by these concepts of equifinality and multifinality. For example, in longitudinal systems models, risk and protective factors transact to produce homogeneous and heterogeneous outcomes in development after exposure to environmental adversity (Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2009; Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten and Cicchetti2016).

Prevailing limitations in the study of human resilience

Despite scientific progress in studying human resilience using a developmental lens, some significant methodological issues remain to be addressed (Curtis & Cicchetti, Reference Curtis and Cicchetti2003; Oshri et al., Reference Oshri, Kogan, Kwon, Wickrama, Vanderbroek, Palmer and Mackillop2018). These issues hamper progress in answering questions about the nature of resilience in human development. A salient and prevailing question is why psychosocial adversity affects the development of some and not others. In other words, what explains the heterogeneity of developmental outcomes in response to psychosocial adversity? First, stressors or adverse experiences encompass complex environmental conditions that are often difficult to operationalize in the context of a research study (Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2016; Nikolaidis et al., Reference Nikolaidis, Heleniak, Fields, Bloom, VanTieghem, Vannucci, Camacho, Choy, Gibson, Harmon, Hadis, Douglas, Milham and Tottenham2022). Human development researchers focusing on risk and resilience processes have recently proposed a dimensional approach to typify adversity (Berman et al., Reference Berman, McLaughlin, Tottenham, Godfrey, Seeman, Loucks, Suomi, Danese and Sheridan2022; McLaughlin & Sheridan, Reference McLaughlin and Sheridan2016). This growing body of research has shown that ignoring the dimensionality of environmental adversity impedes knowledge of the developmental etiology of human risk and resilience (LoPilato et al., Reference LoPilato, Addington, Bearden, Cadenhead, Cannon, Cornblatt, Mathalon, McGlashan, Perkins, Tsuang, Woods and Walker2020; McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan and Lambert2014; Nikolaidis et al., Reference Nikolaidis, Heleniak, Fields, Bloom, VanTieghem, Vannucci, Camacho, Choy, Gibson, Harmon, Hadis, Douglas, Milham and Tottenham2022).

Second, exposure to adversity may promote adaptation to the environment and even resilience to adverse contexts (Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Figueredo, Brumbach and Schlomer2009; Fenneman & Frankenhuis, Reference Fenneman and Frankenhuis2020). However, not every adverse environment type, severity, or developmental timing may trigger strengthening effects. We suggest that modeling linear models on the impact of adverse life environments on human development outcomes may miss nonlinear salubrious and protective effects (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Thompson, Hentges, Parry and Sturge-Apple2022; Oshri et al., Reference Oshri, Cui, Carvalho and Liu2022; Oshri, Reference Oshri2023). For example, over time the process of resilience may emerge in response and not despite exposure to specific types or certain levels of adversity (i.e., strengthening effects). These strengthening effects are understudied and must be further explored, addressed, and better understood via nonlinear modeling.

Lastly, although strengthening effects are evident in response to some types and severities of adversities, there are substantial interindividual differences in when and who benefits from these promotive effects. Modeling human resilience development necessitates investigating the variability-inducing contexts that promote strengthening effects (Riley & Masten, Reference Riley and Masten2005; Wyman, Reference Wyman and Luthar2003). Though not exhaustive, these issues and limitations highlight significant challenges in resilience science that persist despite using current longitudinal multilevel multimethod and system approaches. The present study aims to address these limitations and contribute to a new generation of research on the developmental mechanisms of human resilience in the field of developmental psychopathology.

Adopting a DP perspective, the present study aims to examine the dual role of adversity in the development of psychopathology, highlighting both risk and resilience effects. This approach seeks to enrich and guide future resilience research anchored in developmental psychopathology principles. To tackle the complexity of measuring adversity, we will employ a dimensional approach to assess early adversity (Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Figueredo, Brumbach and Schlomer2009; McLaughlin & Sheridan, Reference McLaughlin and Sheridan2016; McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan and Lambert2014). This method involves categorizing adversity into a taxonomy that distinguishes environments based on experiences of deprivation, threat, and unpredictability (Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Figueredo, Brumbach and Schlomer2009; McLaughlin & Sheridan, Reference McLaughlin and Sheridan2016; McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan and Lambert2014).

The hormesis model and developmental psychopathology

The theoretical model we will use to examine the nonlinear and dynamic response to adversity elucidates the phases of strengthening effects from circumscribed psychosocial hardship (Fig. 1). The hormesis model has been used in other disciplines for over a century (Calabrese & Baldwin, Reference Calabrese and Baldwin2000). We will ground our investigation on the psychosocial stress model of hormesis, a refined and adapted theoretical version of hormesis (Oshri et al., Reference Oshri, Cui, Carvalho and Liu2022), to study the emergence of the resilience process under varying levels of environmental stressors and developmental contexts (Oshri et al., Reference Oshri, Cui, Carvalho and Liu2022; Oshri, Reference Oshri2023). According to this model, low-to-moderate levels of adversity may lead to reduced risk up to an inflection point, after which higher levels of adversity lead to risk for psychopathology (Oshri et al., Reference Oshri, Cui, Carvalho and Liu2022; Oshri, Reference Oshri2023). Moreover, these nonlinear effects cover different aspects of behavioral outcomes to varying levels or severity of psychosocial adversity. Lastly, as neuroplasticity declines with age, we expect that hormetic effects on risk for psychopathology will be more salient in younger youth. Using linear, quadratic and cubic functions, we will showcase how this can be identified and probed in longitudinal modeling.

Figure 1. The psychosocial hormesis model.

Figure 1 provides a diagrammatic presentation of the theoretical mechanisms proposed in Oshri et al. (Reference Oshri, Cui, Carvalho and Liu2022). The overall curve can be classified into two zones (hormetic and toxic) that are separated by the hormetic inflection point at which adversity no longer confers a degree of benefit but is deleterious. The hormetic zone is further divided into two regions (strengthening and buffering) by the hormetic zone threshold, or vertex (i.e., the highest or lowest point of a quadratic function). The “strengthening effect” (the area on the left of the hormetic zone vertex in Fig. 1) is characterized by an adaptive response system underlain by a neurobiological mechanism (referred to here as hormetic neuroplasticity) that prepares or conditions the individual for future environmental adversity. The “buffering effect” (on the right side of the hormetic zone vertex) starts when the adversity level reaches a juncture (stress threshold) and changes direction in relation to psychopathological outcomes. The hormetic zone vertex is defined by the maximum level of adversity in which strengthening occurs (as measured by reduced risk for psychopathology) and the beginning of decreased adjustment benefits following exposure to higher levels of adversity. The latter region is referred to as buffering (and belonging to the hormetic zone) because it includes individuals who display a lower risk for psychopathology than those who have none to minimal levels of adversity. Beyond the hormetic inflection point is the toxic stress zone, and is distinguished from the hormetic zone. This area of extreme stress has been discussed in various models of stress on development (Shonkoff et al., Reference Shonkoff, Garner, Dobbins, Earls, Garner, McGuinn, Pascoe and Wood2012). Adversity levels in the toxic zone are thought to have an overwhelmingly negative impact on the individual and induce risk for the development of psychopathological outcomes.

This study will investigate individual differences in hormetic responses by analyzing (1) the nonlinear associations between dimensions of adversity and symptoms of psychopathology and (2) considering interindividual differences in hormetic effects across changes within a brain network connectivity context that is dynamic and reflects neuroplasticity potential over time. This approach recognizes that environmental and developmental context matters, and not every individual benefits equally at any given time from any given type of limited exposure to adversity (Oshri et al., Reference Oshri, Cui, Carvalho and Liu2022). Therefore, we aim to delineate the brain processes contributing to this heterogeneity in hormetic responses. In particular, this examination will highlight how individual variability in responding to adversity can be partially attributed to the evolving neuroplastic processes in the brain, especially those related to self-regulation, future orientation, and learning.

Data in the present study are drawn from the Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development (ABCD) project, an existing dataset considered the largest longitudinal neurobiological study of youth development in history. Given the ABCD’s assessment limitation of environmental context, we will leverage structural equation modeling (SEM) to latently measure environmental adversity dimensions optimally. Without experimental data, attention to measurement timing (across assessment time points) enables us to keep fidelity to methods that ensure proximity to causal inference between independent, dependent, and moderating variables. Therefore, we will prioritize the temporal order of causation to examine causal effects in these longitudinal, multilevel, multimethod models.

Dimensional adversity and risk for youth psychopathology

Threat

Threat is defined as experiences involving or risking harm (McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan and Lambert2014). This study will focus on threatful environments involving conflict in the family context. Children’s exposure to family conflict has been conceptualized as an early form of a threatening rearing environment (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Thompson, Martin and Cummings2021; Fosco & Feinberg, Reference Fosco and Feinberg2015). Extant research shows that children raised in families laden with family conflict, such as parent-child, interparental, and sibling conflict, are at risk for a range of problem behaviors (Grych et al., Reference Grych, Fincham, Jouriles and McDonald2000). This has led developmental scientists to formulate the emotional security theory (Cummings & Davies, Reference Cummings and Davies2010; Davies & Cummings, Reference Davies and Cummings1994), whereby children experience unsafe situations due to family conflict. This theory suggests family conflict leads to vulnerability for later psychopathology due to the development of dysregulated response patterns to threatening environments (Cummings & Davies, Reference Cummings and Davies2010; McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan and Lambert2014). Data on the strengthening effect of adversity in response to threats such as family conflict exist. In a prospective study, Davies et al. (Reference Davies, Thompson, Hentges, Parry and Sturge-Apple2022) showed a nonlinear (i.e., quadratic) association between children’s emotional reactivity and behavioral dysregulation. Specifically, the study showed that minimal levels of family conflict were linked to less emotional risk, whereas high levels of family conflict were related to children’s increased risk for psychopathology. Based on this previous research, we expect a hormetic effect in response to low-to-moderate threat levels.

Deprivation

Deprivation experiences include an absence of expected inputs from the environment (McLaughlin & Sheridan, Reference McLaughlin and Sheridan2016). Children reared in families who endure material hardship are at risk of experiencing multiple sources of deprivation, scarce financial support, limited access to neighborhood and community resources, and reduced parental supervision (Heflin et al., Reference Heflin, Sandberg and Rafail2009). Socioeconomic (SES) hardship is a robust predictor of adolescent risk behaviors and a range of adjustment problems throughout adolescence and early adulthood, including psychopathology and addiction (Bolland et al., Reference Bolland, Bolland, Tomek, Devereaux, Mrug and Wimberly2016; Lopez-Vergara et al., Reference Lopez-Vergara, Spillane, Merrill and Jackson2016). Similarly, when it comes to parenting, there is robust evidence that depriving attention and awareness of the child’s whereabouts is linked to problem behaviors, particularly delinquency (Corlis & Damashek, Reference Corlis and Damashek2021).

Despite robust evidence linking deprivation to the risk of psychopathological outcomes, this association may exhibit a nonlinear pattern indicative of hormesis. For instance, during the transition to adolescence, parental monitoring and control levels diminish as children seek individuation and increasingly spend time with peers. Furthermore, during this developmental transition, neglectful parenting practices evolve from a lack of parental supervision to a lack of parental monitoring (Burke et al., Reference Burke, Pardini and Loeber2008; Ryan et al., Reference Ryan, Williams and Courtney2013). At this stage, some reduction in parental monitoring could positively influence the development of autonomy in youth. Because a critical developmental task in adolescence is to develop autonomy, it is possible that in a normative sample, some degree of lack of parental monitoring coupled with financial distress can lead to the development of valuable life skills, such as self-reliance, problem-solving abilities, and coping strategies. These life skills are essential for resilient adolescents (Zimmer-Gembeck & Skinner, Reference Zimmer-Gembeck, Skinner and Cicchetti2016) and may result in the development of strengthening and coping skills. Further, a nonlinear effect is also expected in response to SES hardship, challenging the idea that higher SES necessarily leads to favorable developmental outcomes. In fact, growing research shows that privileged children reared in affluent and resourceful environments are also at risk for psychopathology (Luthar & Latendresse, Reference Luthar and Latendresse2005). It is plausible, therefore, that exposure to some level of economic hardship will trigger youth to develop essential life skills that will help them negotiate stressors in life. Consequently, we hypothesize a hormetic effect in the link between deprivation and risk for the development of psychopathology.

Unpredictability

Unpredictability is a less-studied form of environmental adversity in relation to the development of psychopathology. Defined as conditions at which environmental harshness varies over time and space, developmental science research often refers to inconsistent rates and unreliable rearing environments (Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Figueredo, Brumbach and Schlomer2009). A core feature of child development is children’s ability to learn and be shaped by the environment (Ferguson et al., Reference Ferguson, Cassells, MacAllister and Evans2013). This is evident in social learning and the underlying neural mechanisms linked to neuroplasticity (Davidson & McEwen, Reference Davidson and McEwen2012). Neural organization and proliferation in response to environmental input are often mediated via two types of organism-by-environment interactions: experience-expectant and experience-dependent (Schore, Reference Schore1996). Learning via experience-expectant input involves neural responses after exposure to specific expected input during sensitive and critical periods. For example, there is a window of language acquisition among infants and young children largely due to early-life neuroplasticity and the unique ability of infants and young children to distinguish between all phonemes (distinct units of sound).

In contrast, experience-dependent is more pertinent to hormetic effects as it refers to any neuroplastic learning experience spanning the lifespan, such as learning how to drive a car, or even coping strategies such as reappraisal to promote emotion regulation (Gabard-Durnam & McLaughlin, Reference Gabard-Durnam and McLaughlin2019; Greenough et al., Reference Greenough, Black and Wallace1987). Unpredictability in the environment constitutes a salient stressor for the child because environmental input is stochastic, which hinders the learning processes necessary for hormesis. The life history perspective suggests that stress linked to unpredictability triggers survival strategies that promote earlier reproductive behaviors and the propagation of genes. Indeed, unpredictability-related stress has negatively influenced development and motivation in animal and human research (Chen & Baram, Reference Chen and Baram2016; Xu et al., Reference Xu, Harms, Green, Wilson and Pollak2023). Growing developmental psychopathology research shows that unreliability is a robust predictor of children and youth externalizing and internalizing psychopathology (Glynn & Baram, Reference Glynn and Baram2019; Hunt et al., Reference Hunt, Vinograd, Glynn, Davis, Baram, Stern, Nievergelt, Cuccurazzu, Napan, Delmar, Baker and Risbrough2023). Because an unpredictable environment offers minimal opportunity for learning and is associated with mental health risks, we didn’t hypothesize a strengthening effect from experiences of unpredictability among youth (Glynn & Baram, Reference Glynn and Baram2019; Hunt et al., Reference Hunt, Vinograd, Glynn, Davis, Baram, Stern, Nievergelt, Cuccurazzu, Napan, Delmar, Baker and Risbrough2023).

Development in brain context: the case of resting-state functional connectivity of the DMN

Ecological perspectives DP (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2008; Hyde et al., Reference Hyde, Gard, Tomlinson, Burt, Mitchell and Monk2020) suggest that the degree to which environmental adversity affects psychopathology may vary by neurobiological contexts (Boyce, Reference Boyce2016). The present study focuses on the neural context of functional connectivity (i.e., between-region correlated brain activity) within the default mode network (DMN) derived from resting-state fMRI (rs-fMRI) data acquired as part of the ABCD Study (Casey et al., Reference Casey, Cannonier, Conley, Cohen, Barch, Heitzeg, Soules, Teslovich, Dellarco, Garavan, Orr, Wager, Banich, Speer, Sutherland, Riedel, Dick, Bjork, Thomas, Chaarani and Dale2018). We deliberately focused on brain networks instead of specific regions of interest (Bassett & Sporns, Reference Bassett and Sporns2017; Pessoa, Reference Pessoa2014). A network approach to understanding brain function reflects the interconnected and dynamic nature of the brain, offering a more accurate and comprehensive view than examining isolated regions alone (Bassett & Sporns, Reference Bassett and Sporns2017; Pessoa, Reference Pessoa2014). This perspective is crucial for advancing the discovery and understanding of the neural substrates underlying psychiatric disorders as well as complex (Bassett et al., Reference Bassett, Xia and Satterthwaite2018), emergent brain functions more generally. Growing empirical evidence suggests that neuroregulatory capacities are often linked to interconnectivity and communication between multiple regions of interest forming networks. These neural networks better predict specific reactions above and beyond the effects of any singular neural region (Menon, Reference Menon2011; Pessoa, Reference Pessoa2014).

We selected to examine the DMN because of its functions related to resilience and psychopathology. The DMN is one of several large-scale brain networks identified during “rest” – when the brain is awake but not otherwise engaged in task-related activity (Broyd et al., Reference Broyd, Demanuele, Debener, Helps, James and Sonuga-Barke2009; Tashjian et al., Reference Tashjian, Goldenberg and Galván2017). Reported functions supported by DMN include self-referential thought and recalling personal memories, daydreaming, social cognition, and introspection (Andrews-Hanna et al., Reference Andrews-Hanna, Smallwood and Spreng2014; Wade et al., Reference Wade, Prime, Jenkins, Yeates, Williams and Lee2018). The DMN also regulates attention and switches between internal and external modes of attention (Whitfield-Gabrieli & Ford, Reference Whitfield-Gabrieli and Ford2012). Resting-state networks such as the DMN offer a window into the brain’s intrinsic functional organization and architecture, as these networks are constructed from a history of coactivation between brain regions (Greicius et al., Reference Greicius, Supekar, Menon and Dougherty2009). Research shows that transactions between a person’s unique experiences and context and their maturing neural substrate shape these networks across time (Sripada et al., Reference Sripada, Kessler and Angstadt2014; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Verkes, Roozendaal and Hermans2019). These changing and multilevel interactions (brain-by-development) explain the emergent properties of brain network development, which contextualize human resilience as a dynamic process that emerges from the interaction between the environment and individual neurobiology. From this perspective, changes in resting-state networks are critical context for the neuroplasticity processes expected in adolescence. The network level resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) is a malleable asset the child has developed and continues to develop through their experiences (DeCross et al., Reference DeCross, Sambrook, Sheridan, Tottenham and McLaughlin2022; Whittle et al., Reference Whittle, Vijayakumar, Simmons, Dennison, Schwartz, Pantelis, Sheeber, Byrne and Allen2017).

Neuroimaging studies, primarily those using rs-fMRI, have identified that the DMN consists of a distributed set of brain regions, including the medial prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, precuneus, and lateral parietal cortex, among others (Broyd et al., Reference Broyd, Demanuele, Debener, Helps, James and Sonuga-Barke2009). The level of connectivity between all brain regions comprising the DMN is called intra-network connectivity (hereafter called DMN rsFC), operationalized here as the mean correlation value across all connections between DMN regions defined in the Gordon et al. (Reference Gordon, Laumann, Adeyemo, Huckins, Kelley and Petersen2016) i.e., DMN-to-DMN connections. Notably, within-DMN activation changes during adolescence due to normative maturational processes (Horowitz-Kraus et al., Reference Horowitz-Kraus, Farah, Hajinazarian, Eaton, Rajagopal, Schmithorst, Altaye, Vannest and Holland2017). In line with a developmental psychopathology systems framework (Cicchetti & Curtis, Reference Cicchetti and Curtis2015), large-scale brain networks like the DMN can and should be evaluated across multiple levels of analysis to better understand how different brain regions interact and communicate.

The dynamic interaction between the environment, DMN rsFC, and risk for psychopathology versus resilience is only beginning to be understood (Yaesoubi et al., Reference Yaesoubi, Allen, Miller and Calhoun2015). The DMN has been linked to behavioral outcomes involved in resilience among youth, such as future orientation (Fuentes-Claramonte et al., Reference Fuentes-Claramonte, Martín-Subero, Salgado-Pineda, Alonso-Lana, Moreno-Alcázar, Argila-Plaza, Santo-Angles, Albajes-Eizagirre, Anguera-Camós, Capdevila, Sarró, McKenna, Pomarol-Clotet, Salvador and Chao2019), cognitive inhibition (Fulong et al., Reference Fulong, Spruyt, Chao, Dianjiang, Jun and Fang2020), and coping with psychosocial stress and adversity (Rebello et al., Reference Rebello, Moura, Pinaya, Rohde and Sato2018). Some research suggests that DMN rsFC patterns are associated with better emotion regulation and resilience (Miyagi et al., Reference Miyagi, Oishi, Kobayashi, Ueno, Yoshimura, Murai and Fujiwara2020). However, other results indicate relative hyperconnectivity may also be linked to rumination and risk for psychopathology (Whitfield-Gabrieli & Ford, Reference Whitfield-Gabrieli and Ford2012). Further, studies examining the association between DMN connectivity patterns and impulsivity have yielded competing evidence. For instance, Inuggi et al. (Reference Inuggi, Sanz-Arigita, González-Salinas, Valero-García, García-Santos and Fuentes2014) showed that higher trait-impulsivity is related to lower DMN rsFC among 8 to 12–year–olds. Further, risk-taking behaviors in adolescents have been associated with DMN hyperconnectivity during resting state (DeWitt et al., Reference DeWitt, Aslan and Filbey2014), suggesting that stronger DMN rsFC could contribute to youths’ impulsivity. Lastly, DMN rsFC has been associated with adaptive socioemotional functioning in adolescence, including sleep health (Tashjian et al., Reference Tashjian, Goldenberg, Monti and Galván2018; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Cui, Huffman and Oshri2023), emotional regulation(Afzali et al., Reference Afzali, Dagher, Bourque, Spinney and Conrod2022) and reduced problem behaviors (Fox et al., Reference Fox, Abram, Reilly, Eack, Goldman, Csernansky, Wang and Smith2017). Given these functional implications, we examined the DMN rsFC of this network as a neural context where different dimensions of adversity are differentially hormetic and consequently linked to variability of risk for the development of psychopathology.

Developmental timing of hormetic plasticity

Although a large body of research shows that there are sensitive developmental periods to the negative impact of adversity (Knudsen, Reference Knudsen2004), less is known about sensitive periods for strengthening through adversity effects. Similarly, existing literature indicates that neuroplasticity diminishes with age (Gabard-Durnam & McLaughlin, Reference Gabard-Durnam and McLaughlin2019). Consequently, it is reasonable to hypothesize that stress-induced neuroplasticity will be differentially expressed in relation to different timings of developmental outcomes (Knudsen, Reference Knudsen2004). This concept suggests that the neurobiological processes underlying hormetic effects are more pronounced during sensitive periods. Exposure to low-to-moderate levels of adversity may facilitate experience-expectant and dependent learning, potentially serving as an asset to mitigate the impact of future adversity later in development. This phenomenon of hormetic neuroplasticity timing, or the brain’s adaptive response to stressors, may provide a foundational mechanism for resilience in adverse environments. Therefore, longitudinal studies investigating hormetic effects at varying developmental stages and ages are crucial to elucidate the brain mechanisms driving resilience. We posit that part of individual differences in hormetic effects is correlated with time-varying experience-dependent neuroplasticity.

The present study

The main aim of the present study is to examine to what extent and under what adverse conditions children may show linear risk for psychopathology and strengthening effects in response to adversity using the psychosocial hormesis theoretical model (Oshri, Reference Oshri2023). Environmental adversity will be tested across three dimensions: threat, deprivation, and unpredictability. We first hypothesized that the three dimensions of psychosocial adversity will be linearly associated with increased internalizing and externalizing symptoms. We did not have a preconceived hypothesis on expected hormetic effects in response to unpredictability. Given previous literature, however, we did expect a nonlinear effect–that low-to-moderate levels of deprivation and family threat would be consistent with hormesis. In line with the psychosocial hormesis model, we tested the hypothesis that DMN rsFC would serve as a developmental context contributing to variability in hormetic response. Specifically, we examined whether individual differences in hormetic effects could be explained by varying levels of change in DMN rsFC and the age in which the hormetic outcomes are modeled. We hypothesized that age of change in psychopathological risk symptoms (developmental timing) and alterations in DMN rsFC patterns would differentiate youth who benefit from low-to-moderate adversity levels. The aim was to test the hypothesis that changes in brain networks, such as changes in within-DMN rsFC, can lead to differences in response to low-to-moderate stress exposure and inform prevention intervention programs.

Methods

Sample

All hypotheses were tested using data from the ongoing ABCD study, a longitudinal and multisite study of adolescent brain development and mental health. The study included a total of 11,878 children from 21 sites across the United States, representing diverse socioeconomic, ethnic, and biobehavioral health backgrounds. The study procedures received approval from the human research protection programs and institutional review boards at all universities participating in the ABCD project. Full details on study aims, design, recruitment, and procedures may be found in Casey et al. (Reference Casey, Cannonier, Conley, Cohen, Barch, Heitzeg, Soules, Teslovich, Dellarco, Garavan, Orr, Wager, Banich, Speer, Sutherland, Riedel, Dick, Bjork, Thomas, Chaarani and Dale2018) and Garavan et al. (Reference Garavan, Bartsch, Conway, Decastro, Goldstein, Heeringa, Jernigan, Potter, Thompson and Zahs2018). Data accessed from the NIMH data included baseline (N = 11,878; 47.8% female; M age = 10.1), T2 (6 months from baseline), T5 (24 months from baseline), and T7 (36 months from baseline, M age = 12.9). The racial-ethnic composition of the sample was 52.0% European American, 15.0% African American, 20.3% Latino(a), 2.1% Asian/Pacific Islander, and 10.5% Other.

Resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging data

Neuroimaging data were obtained using Siemens, General Electric, or Philips 3T scanners equipped with a 32-channel head coil. During the collection of rs fMRI data, participants were instructed to focus on a crosshair for 20 minutes. The preprocessing of fMRI data utilized FreeSurfer version 5.3.0 before data extraction (Casey et al., Reference Casey, Cannonier, Conley, Cohen, Barch, Heitzeg, Soules, Teslovich, Dellarco, Garavan, Orr, Wager, Banich, Speer, Sutherland, Riedel, Dick, Bjork, Thomas, Chaarani and Dale2018) and by the ABCD Data Analysis and Informatics Core (Hagler et al., Reference Hagler, Hatton, Cornejo, Makowski, Fair, Dick, Sutherland, Casey, Barch, Harms, Watts, Bjork, Garavan, Hilmer, Pung, Sicat, Kuperman, Bartsch, Xue, Heitzeg and Dale2019). We then followed the recommended quality control protocol (Hagler et al., Reference Hagler, Hatton, Cornejo, Makowski, Fair, Dick, Sutherland, Casey, Barch, Harms, Watts, Bjork, Garavan, Hilmer, Pung, Sicat, Kuperman, Bartsch, Xue, Heitzeg and Dale2019) and excluded participants exhibiting excessive head motion. Complete imaging data remained for 9,130 participants at T1 and 7,194 at T5.

Measures

Predictors

Family threat

A latent factor comprising seven items from the Family Environment Scale family conflict subscale was used to assess family threat (Hoffman et al., Reference Hoffman, Clark, Orendain, Hudziak, Squeglia and Dowling2019; Zucker et al., Reference Zucker, Gonzalez, Ewing, Paulus, Arroyo, Fuligni, Morris, Sanchez and Wills2018). All items were coded in the same direction, and the factor structure was optimized via confirmatory factor analysis (CFA; see Table 1 for items and factor loadings, χ 2(12) = 444.27, RMSEA = .06, CFI/TLI = .96/.93, SRMR = .06.)

Table 1. Items factor loadings for family unpredictability, deprivation, and threat

Note. All factor loadings p < .01.

Deprivation

A latent factor of SES-related deprivation was created using a CFA at T1, including caregivers’ marital status (1 = no, 0 = yes), family material deprivation, parental education (1 = < HS Diploma….5 = Post Graduate Degree), family income-to-poverty ratio (dividing the family’s reported annual income by the federal poverty threshold for the year, accounting for household size), neighborhood area deprivation index, and parental monitoring. Family material deprivation was measured via a total of seven items from the Parent Demographics Survey that assessed whether (1 = yes, 0 = no) a family experienced any financial-related hardships (e.g., could not afford food) during the past 12 months. The area deprivation index is a composite score derived from the American Community Survey, incorporating data on 17 neighborhood factors (e.g., income, education, employment, and housing quality). Parental monitoring is measured by one question regarding the frequency of shared dinners between parents and the participant in an average week (Table 1). The model fit for the factor structure was good, χ 2(9) = 54.60, RMSEA = .02, CFI/TLI = 1.00/.99, SRMR = .01.

Unpredictability

The degree of unpredictability within the family was conceptualized as the combination of unpredictable or chaotic behaviors manifested from primary/secondary caregivers, and the amount of disorder characterizing the family environment. This was operationalized through a higher-order CFA composed of primary caregiver unpredictability, secondary caregiver unpredictability, and family disorder as first-order factors. Items for each of the first-order factors were taken from the Adult Behavior Checklist (Achenbach et al., Reference Achenbach, Dumenci and Rescorla2003) and the Family Environment Scale (Moos & Moos, Reference Moos and Moos1994), respectively (See Table 1 for items and factor loadings). The model fit for the higher-order factor structure was good, χ 2(31) = 355.73, RMSEA = .03, CFI/TLI = .967/.952, SRMR = .03.

Within-DMN rsFC change

Network connectivity within-DMN (Pearson correlation) was calculated based on the Gordon parcellation scheme (Gordon et al., Reference Gordon, Laumann, Adeyemo, Huckins, Kelley and Petersen2016) for 12 predefined resting-state networks, including the DMN. Imaging was completed at baseline (T1) and the two-year assessment (T5). A single-indicator latent change score was computed and saved to represent the degree of change in within-DMN rsFC from T1 to T5 (ΔDMN rsFC). In neurocognitive developmental studies, latent change scores are recommended for characterizing neural change over time (Kievit et al., Reference Kievit, Brandmaier, Ziegler, van Harmelen, de Mooij, Moutoussis, Goodyer, Bullmore, Jones, Fonagy, Lindenberger and Dolan2018). Positive ΔDMN rsFC scores indicate the average correlation across all regions within the DMN network increasing from T1 to T5 (i.e., greater DMN rsFC over time), while negative ΔDMN rsFC scores indicate decreases in DMN rsFC over time. On average, the overall sample was characterized by increases in DMN rsFC from T1 to T5, with significant variability (μΔDMN-rsFC = .25, σ 2ΔDMN-rsFC = .003). The latent change model for ΔDMN rsFC was just identified.

Outcome measures

Internalizing and externalizing problems

Adolescents self-reported their internalizing and externalizing symptoms using 19 items from the Brief Problem Monitor (BPM) (Achenbach et al., Reference Achenbach, McConaughy, Ivanova and Rescorla2011). BPM was administered every 6 months from T2 to T7. Items were assessed on a Likert scale ranging from “0” (not true) to “2” (very true). The internalizing problems subscale (αT2 = .71; αT5 = .78; αT7 = .80) reflects a child’s emotional and internal psychological state and the externalizing problems (αT2 = αT1 = .67; αT5 = .67; αT7 = .67) capture a child’s problem behavior that is directed toward the external environment. Single-item latent change scores were computed using the summary score data from T2 - T7 (Geiser, Reference Geiser2020). Model fit indices were good for both the internalizing, χ 2(9) = 56, RMSEA = .02, CFI/TLI = 1.00/.99, SRMR = .02, and externalizing, χ2(9) = 36.74, RMSEA = .02, CFI/TLI = 1.00/.99, SRMR = .02 models.

Covariates

Youth sex (1 = female, 2 = male), income (<5K = 10, …, > = 200K = 1), and parental education (1 = < HS Diploma….5 = Post Graduate Degree) were included as covariates.

Analytic plan

Hypotheses were tested using SEM in Mplus version 8.1 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2009), and figures were generated using the Rstudio version 4.2.0 (R Core Team, 2022). The factor structure of the adversity latent variables (family threat, deprivation, and unpredictability) was assessed through CFA, factor scores saved and mean-centered at zero. Second, linear and nonlinear longitudinal associations between each dimension of adversity and change in later youth behavioral problems were examined using path analysis. Significant nonlinear effects were probed for hormetic patterns by plotting the slope, calculating inflection points/vertices (i.e., values partitioning the cubic curve in conceptually meaningful sections), and estimating the number of participants characterized by each region. Hormetic and toxic zones were distinguished by the conceptual hormetic inflection (where predicted buffering region values surpassed the predicted strengthening region values), rather than the statistical hormetic inflection (pivot point in curve concavity). Both points have been identified on cubic plots for interested readers. Last, latent change in DMN rsFC (from T1 to T5) was tested as a moderator of cubic hormetic effects. Simple slopes of significant cubic moderations were plotted and further probed using multi-group linear/quadratic interaction analysis parceled by region/zone. Covariates were included in all analyses. Multilevel modeling was used to account for the clustering effects of participants within families and scanner type.

A full-information maximum likelihood algorithm was used to estimate the missing data. The weighted least squares with mean and variance adjusted estimator was used for estimating adversity latent factors because of the presence of nominal indicators within the measurement model. Participant factor scores derived from CFAs (including latent change scores) were saved to facilitate their usage in subsequent analyses and transformation to nonlinear regression terms. All other analyses used maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors as it produces unbiased parameter estimates (Enders & Bandalos, Reference Enders and Bandalos2001). Criteria for evaluating model fit were as follows: a maximum value of .06 for the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) and .08 for the standardized root mean squared (SRMR), and a minimum value of .90 for the comparative fit index (CFI) (Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999).

Results

Linear and nonlinear effects of adversity on youth outcomes

Youth age, sex, and family income significantly predicted change in youth internalizing and externalizing problems (Table 2). Nonsignificant covariates (e.g., primary, and secondary parental education) were excluded from further models.

Table 2. Longitudinal cubic regression parameters on internalizing and externalizing symptoms

Note. Table format β (b). LRT = MLR Scaling Corrected Likelihood Ratio Test comparing current model to the significant former. *** p < .001, ** p < .01, * p < .05, ϯ p < .1.

Family threat

We first tested a linear, quadratic, and cubic SEM model on the predictive associations between the level of family threat and internalizing and externalizing symptoms. The linear results showed that family threat predicts significant increases in externalizing problems at both T5, β threat = .08, CI[ .06, .11], p < .001 and T7, βthreat = .03, CI[ .01, .05], p < .05, but not in internalizing problems at either time point. While the quadratic model was largely nonsignificant, adding the cubic family threat path significantly improved model fit, LRT: Δχ 2(4): 9.68, p < .05, and this model predicted changes in internalizing and externalizing problems. In particular, the linear family threat path persisted in predicting youth externalizing problems at T5 and T7 and the cubic family threat term predicted internalizing problems at T5, β threat 3 = −.07, CI[−.13, −.01], p < .05. Follow-up analyses indicated that the conceptual hormetic inflection point (i.e., the transition from hormetic to toxic adversity zones) of the cubic curve occurred at .33 units above the mean (Z = .53, see Fig. 2). Within the hormetic zone, the hormetic zone vertex (i.e., the transition from strengthening to buffering adversity regions) for family threat is at −.32 units below the mean (Z = −.52). Using these thresholds, the majority of the youth in the total sample are characterized by the hormetic zone, either in the strengthening (33%, n = 3,860) or buffering (40%, n = 4,749) regions. In contrast, 27% of the sample (n = 3,263) experienced family threat characteristic of the toxic zone. Overall, this model explained 2% of the change in externalizing problems and 3% of the change in internalizing problems at T5.

Figure 2. Probing hormesis of family threat effect on internalizing symptoms.

Deprivation

The linear model for deprivation predicted change in externalizing problems at T5, β deprivation = .08, CI[.04, .12], p < .001, but not T7 nor either time point of internalizing problems. The addition of the quadratic deprivation path failed to improve model fit (LRT: Δχ 2 (4): 6.66, p = n.s.), but the cubic deprivation path did, LRT: Δχ 2 (8): 19.35, p < .05, and predicted externalizing problems at T5, β deprivation 3 = −.07, CI[−.11, −.03], p < .01. Follow-up analyses indicated that the conceptual hormetic inflection point (i.e., the transition from hormetic to toxic adversity zones) of the overall cubic curve occurs when deprivation is at .07 units above the mean (Z = .09, see Figure 3). The hormetic zone vertex (i.e., the transition from strengthening to buffering adversity regions) for deprivation occurred at −1.22 units below the mean (Z = −1.71). Using these thresholds, approximately half of the youths in the total sample were characterized by the hormetic zone, either in the strengthening (5%, n = 578) or buffering (48%, n = 5,652) regions. In contrast, 48% of the sample (n = 5,646) experienced deprivation characteristic of the toxic zone. This model explained 2% of the change in externalizing problems and 3% of the change in internalizing problems at T5.

Figure 3. Probing hormesis of deprivation on externalizing symptoms.

Unpredictability

The linear model predicting change in internalizing and externalizing problems from T5-T7 found the unpredictability factor only to be linearly and positively associated with externalizing problems, β unpredict = .05, CI[.02, .07], p < .001. Adding the quadratic and cubic unpredictability terms failed to improve model fit significantly and produced no significant associations. Therefore, the hormetic models were not further probed for the unpredictability dimension.

Hormetic moderation analysis

Latent change from T1 - T5 within the DMN resting-state functional connectivity (ΔDMN rsFC) was examined as a moderator of the observed hormetic relations between family threat and deprivation on child behavioral outcomes. Only the cubic effect of family threat on internalizing symptoms was significantly moderated by ΔDMN rsFC. This moderating effect persisted after including covariates and correcting for multiple comparisons using the Benjamini-Hochberg approach with a false discovery rate of .05, β THREAT3xΔDMN-LC = .10, CI[.03, .17], p corrected = .01. Plotting the simple slopes of the cubic function (See Fig. 4) revealed an overall pattern of effects that lower levels of ΔDMN rsFC intensified the cubic relation between family threat and youth internalizing problems while increases of ΔDMN rsFC flattened this curve.

Figure 4. Simple slopes of the cubic family threat X change DMN rsFC interaction predicting youth internalizing problems in the full sample.

We further probed the specific moderating effects of ΔDMN rsFC through a series of multi-group analyses. Participants were parsed into conceptually meaningful groups (i.e., strengthening/buffering regions, and toxic/hormetic zones) based on their level of family threat in relation to the inflection points and vertices previously identified on the cubic curve. This approach separates the cubic curve observed in the full sample into smaller sections that can be explored with more conventional linear/quadratic interaction analyses. Specifically, linear interactions were examined in four family threat regions including, strengthening (values less than the hormetic zone vertex [x < −32]), buffering (values between the hormetic zone vertex and the conceptual hormetic inflection [x = −.31–.33]), rising toxic (values between the conceptual hormetic inflection and toxic vertex [x = .34–.75]), and falling toxic (values greater than the toxic vertex [x > .75]). Quadratic interactions were examined both in the hormetic (values less than the conceptual hormetic inflection [x < .33]) and toxic zones (values greater than the conceptual hormetic inflection [x > .33]). Significant within-group interaction paths were compared between groups with a Wald chi-square test. Specific moderating effects and regions of significance were identified using a Johnson-Neyman approach for linear × linear and quadratic × linear effects (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Stromeyer and Schwieterman2013), and by plotting simple slopes at + 1/−1 SD for linear and quadratic associations.

The results of the multi-group interaction analysis found ΔDMN rsFC to significantly moderate linear family threat in the strengthening region (β THREATxDMN-LC = .31, CI[.12, .50], p < .01), and quadratic family threat in the toxic zone (βTHREAT2xDMN-LC = .48, CI[.11, .84], p < .05). The strengthening region’s linear interaction path was significantly different from the linear interaction paths observed in both the buffering (W (1) = 4.38, p < .05) and rising toxic regions (W (1) = 5.97, p < .05). Likewise, the toxic zone quadratic interaction path was significantly different from the quadratic interaction path in the hormetic zone, W (1) = 7.83, p < .01. These significant between-group tests suggest changes in neural activation patterns have unique effects across the spectrum of family threat that are particularly salient in the strengthening region and the toxic zone.

Probing the within-group linear and quadratic effects reflected the same exacerbating role of low ΔDMN rsFC observed during the initial cubic moderation analysis (Fig. 4). Within the strengthening region, the negative linear relation between family threat and internalizing symptoms became more negative as ΔDMN rsFC lessened (Fig. 5a). In the toxic zone, the concavity in the quadratic relation between family threat and internalizing symptoms increased as ΔDMN rsFC decreased (Fig. 5b). In other words, lower ΔDMN rsFC changed its effect in different family risk contexts. Specifically, on the one hand, lower ΔDMN rsFC was associated with increases in the strength of the hormetic effect of family threat on the development of internalizing problems in the strengthening region (family threat became more beneficial at low ΔDMN rsFC; See Fig. 5a). On the other hand, lower ΔDMN rsFC was linked with the potentiation of the harmful effects of family threat in the toxic zone (low ΔDMN rsFC increased the harm of family threat; See Fig. 5b).

Figure 5. (a) Probing interaction by DMN LC in the strengthening zone. (b) Simple slopes of the family threat2 x DMN LC predicting internalizing problems in the toxic zone.

Johnson-Neyman analyses indicated that the moderating effect of ΔDMN rsFC influenced 1,561 (14%) of youths in the overall sample (strengthening n = 394, toxic n = 1,167). Moreover, areas at which the influence of the moderator became significant (area of significance; AoS) were identified as ΔDMN rsFC values below −.046 within the strengthening region (linear interaction; See Fig. 6a) and −.005 in the toxic zone (quadratic interaction; See Fig. 6b). The negative ΔDMN rsFC values characterizing each AoS indicate decreases in DMN rsFC coherence from T1 to T5 (i.e., lower correlation among DMN regions). In fact, post-hoc analyses found that baseline and T5 DMN rsFC means, variances, and the latent change scores were statistically equivalent across groups (strengthening, buffering, and rising/falling toxic regions; Δχ 2(12) = 5.95, p = .92). However, youths in an AoS (either strengthening region or toxic zone) experienced higher baseline levels of DMN rsFC (ΔM DMNrsFC-T1 = .06; W (1) = 240.01, p < .001) and lower DMN rsFC at T5 (ΔM DMNrsFC-T5 = −.04; W (1) = 158.94, p < .001) compared to the remainder of the sample. Additionally, the AoS youths were characterized by a negative ΔDMN rsFC mean (ΔDMN rsFCAOS = −.08) compared to the positive ΔDMN rsFC mean found in the overall sample (ΔDMN rsFCNONSIG = .02; W (1) = 1,608.34, p < .001). In summary, the accentuating effect of ΔDMN rsFC on the association between family threat and youth internalizing problems is significant for youths experiencing reductions in DMN rsFC over the two-year period (from Time 1 to Time 5).

Figure 6. (a) Johnson-Neyman plot of cubic interaction by DMN LC strengthening zone. (b) Johnson-Neyman plot of family threat across values of the DMN LC when X is fixed at its mean (0).

Discussion

The developmental psychopathology perspective offers a rich theoretical framework for studying the origin, course, and mechanisms of resilience. A central focus of developmental psychopathology research is to understand how typical versus atypical development emerges under both normal and abnormal conditions (Cicchetti & Curtis, Reference Cicchetti and Curtis2015). A critical yet unanswered question is how human resilience unfolds as a developmental process in response to, rather than despite, adverse experiences (Oshri et al., Reference Oshri, Cui, Carvalho and Liu2022). The hormesis model, widely used in toxicology, explains the salubrious and strengthening effects of environmental conditions on organisms’ development. However, what constitutes the ‘environment’ in toxicological or biomedical fields differs from its application to human development (Oshri, Reference Oshri2023). This paper aims to test the utility of applying the hormesis model to DP perspective for studying the development of the human resilience process. We aimed to explore what environmental conditions and dimensions of psychosocial adversity may result in strengthening effects and/or risk for developing psychopathology among youth, and how brain-by-development context fosters interindividual variability in hormetic responses.

The current study used a longitudinal sample of 11,878 youth to examine the effect of dimensions of psychosocial adversity, including threat, deprivation, and unpredictability, on change in internalizing and externalizing symptoms of psychopathology. Further, we examined the resilience process to these environmental adversity dimensions by testing the psychosocial hormetic model of resilience. Specifically, we aimed to test whether these dimensions of adversity are related to strengthening, buffering, and toxic effects from stress. This allowed us to examine the hypotheses that resilience may also emerge in response to low-to-moderate stress levels and that this effect may be moderated by resting-state neural context. Results showed that threat was positively related to both increases in internalizing and externalizing symptoms, whereas deprivation and unpredictability were positively associated only with externalizing problems. A nonlinear examination of the hormesis model showed that threat was cubically related to change in internalizing symptoms. Deprivation, in turn, was cubically related to externalizing symptoms, and unpredictability only showed linear associations with internalizing and externalizing symptoms. When testing the within-DMN rsFC change as a moderator of the hormetic effect of family threat on youth internalizing problems, we found that a decline in DMN rsFC (from T1 to T5) altered this association in a “for better-or-worse” fashion. That is, reductions in DMN rsFC amplified both the beneficial effects of low-to-moderate levels of adversity and also the deleterious effects of high levels of family threat on internalizing problems.

The results that threat was positively related to increased risk for psychopathology corroborate the emotional security theory (Cummings et al., Reference Cummings, Koss and Davies2015). Furthermore, this theory has been further advanced to include multiple types of conflict in the family beyond interparental conflict (Cummings et al., Reference Cummings, Koss and Davies2015). Because the family environment is critical in shaping youth emotional regulation during childhood and adolescence, the effect of being brought up in a conflictual family climate is salient for adolescent psychopathology outcomes. The emotional security theory argues that in the face of threat, regulatory response patterns in children are forming and inform later coping responses to future threats from the environment (Cummings & Davies, Reference Cummings and Davies2010). Accordingly, the more children and youth are exposed to increased levels of family conflict over time, the more they are likely to solidify dysregulated emotional responses to stressful environments, which increases their vulnerability to affective symptoms such as internalizing problems.

Likewise, our results revealed a cubic effect in which low-to-moderate levels of family conflict led to a reduction in the change of internalizing psychopathology in adolescence. These findings support research in which interparental conflict was found to be congruent with the psychosocial model for hormesis. For example, in a prospective study, Davies and colleagues (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Thompson, Hentges, Parry and Sturge-Apple2022) showed a nonlinear (quadratic) association between children’s emotional reactivity and behavioral dysregulation. In this study, minimal levels of family conflict were linked to less emotional risk, whereas high levels of family conflict were related to children’s risk for psychopathology (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Thompson, Hentges, Parry and Sturge-Apple2022). Our study adds to this literature by characterizing the cubic association between threat and internalizing problems in adolescence and showing to what extent this effect strengthens and buffers against risk.

The present study tested hormetic moderation by change in DMN rsFC to investigate whether variability in hormetic effects partly stems from individual differences in DMN rsFC changes over time. Results showed that a change in DMN rsFC moderated the link between family threat and later internalizing problems. Specifically, we found that a significant reduction in DMN rsFC from Time 1 to Time 5 increased the strength of the hormetic association between family threat and internalizing problems (i.e., low-to-moderate family conflict became more protective). This suggests that among those who started high (Time 1) in DMN rsFC, a decline in this metric was conducive to hormetic neuroplasticity in this age range. In contrast, individuals who started relatively low in DMN rsFC at Time 1 tended to show increases in DMN rsFC at Time 5 and did not show a hormetic effect.

Moreover, individuals who evinced a reduction in DMN rsFC have shown differential risk for psychopathology across varying levels of family threat in a for better-or-worse pattern. While at low-to-moderate levels of threat, these families showed strengthening effects, at high levels of family threat, these individuals experienced the greatest levels of psychopathology and fell into the toxic hormetic zone (See Fig. 5a,b). Such a change from strengthening to vulnerability outcomes at a specific pattern of change of DMN rsFC across varying levels of family threat may be consistent with biological sensitivity to context theory (Ellis & Boyce, Reference Ellis and Boyce2008; Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Essex and Boyce2005), supporting earlier work testing biological sensitivity to context using neurobiological data (Gard et al., Reference Gard, Shaw, Forbes and Hyde2018; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Oshri, Kogan, Wickrama and Sweet2021; Oshri, Reference Oshri2023). Thus, this study may be the first to indicate a biological sensitivity effect to hormesis.

The finding that hyperconnectivity reflects higher sensitivity to the environment is consistent with earlier work on the role of the DMN in brain-behavior associations. As noted above, heightened DMN rsFC reflects a high within-network DMN engagement history. Given that coordinated switching between DMN and other task-active networks (e.g., SAL or FPN) supports internal and external modes of processing and attention, heightened DMN rsFC has been linked to some youths’ difficulty over time in redirecting neural resources from internally directed thoughts to external stimuli for adaptive task performance. Hyperconnectivity may, in turn, have placed them at increased risk for psychopathology (Whitfield-Gabrieli & Ford, Reference Whitfield-Gabrieli and Ford2012). This interpretation may partially explain our findings, particularly for individuals in the high family threat context. We extend this model by considering stress as a key contextual moderator. We posit that individuals with higher vs. lower DMN rsFC at Time 1 may reflect relatively more advanced maturation in this network. For individuals who start high (T1) and decrease (T5) and experience low family threat, this advanced maturation may support adaptive neuroplasticity and be more conducive to developing coping strategies, likely via inter-network coordination. However, when family threat is high and, presumptively, stress levels are also high, overall network functioning may be overwhelmed, especially in frontal regions facing competing demands (e.g., attention towards external, salient [threatening] contextual cues in the environment vs. problem-solving, developing coping strategies). Depleting these PFC resources may thus expose vulnerability to task-switching difficulties involving the DMN and, ultimately, a greater risk for the development of psychopathology.

The effect of deprivation on externalizing problems was found in the present study to be positive (linearly), supporting a large body of research on the effects of deprivation in children’s rearing environment on child risk for the development of externalizing problems (Bolland et al., Reference Bolland, Bolland, Tomek, Devereaux, Mrug and Wimberly2016; Lopez-Vergara et al., Reference Lopez-Vergara, Spillane, Merrill and Jackson2016). In contrast to our expectations, no significant associations existed between children’s exposure to deprivation and changes in internalizing problems. The finding corroborates studies showing that youth who experience deprivation in the form of SES hardship and lack of parental supervision are more likely to develop externalizing symptoms such as conduct and delinquency problem behaviors (Borawski et al., Reference Borawski, Ievers-Landis, Lovegreen and Trapl2003; Ryan et al., Reference Ryan, Williams and Courtney2013). These fit in particular to preadolescence, a developmental transition in which neglect at the family level changes from parental supervision to parental monitoring, which is highly associated with conduct problems (Ryan et al., Reference Ryan, Williams and Courtney2013). In contrast to threat, deprivation did show a cubic association with externalizing symptoms, suggesting that 52% evinced hormesis. The results revealed that 5% of the youth fell into the strengthening zone, and 48% to the buffering zone, suggesting that very low levels of family deprivation yielded some strengthening effects that reduced the risk for later psychopathology.

Studies on the impact of unpredictability on youth development have established a robust link to the development of psychopathology (Glynn & Baram, Reference Glynn and Baram2019; Hunt et al., Reference Hunt, Vinograd, Glynn, Davis, Baram, Stern, Nievergelt, Cuccurazzu, Napan, Delmar, Baker and Risbrough2023). Our models showed that unpredictability has no hormetic effects but a linear impact on the risk of internalizing and externalizing problems. These findings corroborate life history theory and research on the developmental implications of unpredictability. Accordingly, life history theory suggests that unpredictability is particularly stressful for youth and may trigger faster life history strategies or energy allocation priority to behaviors that accelerate physical and cognitive growth and reproductive success, including aggressive and delinquent behavior (Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Figueredo, Brumbach and Schlomer2009). Additionally, plasticity tends to be most pronounced in childhood and adolescence (Fu & Zuo, Reference Fu and Zuo2011). However, in an unpredictable environment, there is less opportunity for experience-dependent learning, which is necessary for hormetic effects (Oshri et al., Reference Oshri, Cui, Carvalho and Liu2022) and resilience-building capacity. Such inconsistent environmental input undermines learning and the required neuroplasticity (Kolb & Gibb, Reference Kolb and Gibb2014) to respond, adapt, and prepare for future adverse environments.

As demonstrated in this paper, in a specific dimension of an adverse environment, youth may exhibit variability in developmental outcomes that cannot be thoroughly tested without nonlinear models. This is due to the potential modification of the path from the independent variables representing the experience of adversity that significantly influences human development. Sensitive and critical periods are fundamental mechanisms driving individual variability in response to environmental inputs, including adversity (Hartley & Frankenhuis, Reference Hartley and Frankenhuis2020). This study partially examined the concept of timing in hormetic plasticity. We found that experiences of threat and deprivation had more pronounced hormetic effects in relation to psychopathological outcomes at earlier ages than later. This aligns with the idea that the timing of developmental outcomes can be altered by neural circuitry organization and function (Gabard-Durnam & McLaughlin, Reference Gabard-Durnam and McLaughlin2020). Research in settings like orphanages has documented the harmful impact of depriving environments in adoptive institutions during infancy on emotional and psychological outcomes (Gunnar & Bowen, Reference Gunnar and Bowen2021). Interestingly, there is limited research on the sensitive periods for resilience-building plasticity. It remains unclear at what developmental stages low-to-moderate adversity might foster growth and resilience.

Further study is needed to understand how the concepts of multifinality and equifinality relate to hormesis, resilience, and risk in the development of psychopathology. In Figure 7, we provide a theoretical presentation of the connection between a given adverse environmental condition (denoted as X1) and a behavioral outcome symptomatic of psychopathology (Y1). In the first illustration, we show that a given developmental connection between the environment and a behavioral outcome can manifest in multiple ways. It can be linear, and nonlinear, suggesting that the statistical modeling of the development of psychopathology can be missed, regardless of other moderating and mediating variables in the prediction models. This brings the discussion to multifinality and equifinality in developmental science and resilience.

Figure 7. Theoretical visualization of multifinality and equifinality.

First, consistent multifinality, we can hypothesize an adverse environmental condition (X 1) to the development of multiple behavioral outcomes (Y1–Y3) by nonlinear effects (Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2009). The principle of multifinality suggests that while some children may exhibit resilience or even positive adaptation after exposure to potentially threatening environments, others may not, highlighting the variability in outcomes following similar experiences. This variability in the effects of adverse environments is surely affected by moderating and mediating mechanisms, however it can also stem from the fact that the adverse condition itself promoted differential risk to behavioral outcomes. Such differences underscore the potential hormetic effects of early experiences, where low-level exposure to stressors can enhance resilience, and reduce risk for psychopathology in some, but not all, individuals. This phenomenon contributes to the observed interindividual variability in the risk of developing psychopathology, challenging linear analyses to fully capture these dynamics.

Similarly, the concept of equifinality, where different dimensions of environmental adversity (X1, X2, X3) lead to a similar developmental outcome (Y), underscores the need for considering nonlinear pathways. This approach recognizes that despite varied adverse experiences, similar outcomes may arise, a process potentially moderated by exposure to multilevel influences of risk and protective factors among youth. Identifying the mechanisms that promote the process of resilience is crucial for developing effective prevention and intervention strategies. To clarify, hormesis provides a framework for understanding the nuanced ways that stressors can influence development. It posits that mild stressors can foster adaptive and strengthening responses, thereby contributing to resilience. This concept helps explain the observed interindividual variability in developmental outcomes, aligning with multifinality and equifinality by illustrating how different levels of stressor exposure and individual responses can lead to varied outcomes from similar or different environmental conditions.

Strengths and limitations

This study leverages multilevel prospective data from 11,878 youths aged 10 to 13, including neuroimaging and survey data. We based our models on a brief survey measure to derive the family threat variable, employing an advanced SEM measurement approach for this purpose. Further, we derived the constructs of deprivation and unpredictability from multiple levels and reporters. In this framework, deprivation is a latent factor, while unpredictability is analyzed through second-order factor analysis. Thus, the first limitation to consider is the temporal sequence from independent to dependent variables. Notably, the unpredictability variable, obtained from data at T5, does not align temporally with other stressors. Temporal sequence also impacted the composition of the threat factor that was limited to a selection of variables in the family conflict scale rather than a more holistic collection of variables. However, all psychopathology outcomes were measured prospectively after the independent variables. An additional limitation is that the ABCD sample represents a community cohort that was not explicitly sampled to include families from high-adversity environments. Nonetheless, our data indicate considerable exposure to SES hardship (29.7% with income < $40K), revealing significant variability in risk and adversity. Lastly, because we weren’t aware of previous studies on the hormetic effects in the development of psychopathology and in a brain-by-development context, some analyses were exploratory and guided by data-driven approach. For example, we tested multiple brain regions associated with DMN to test the hormesis hypothesis. However, these data are public, and our syntaxes are available for transparency and future replication efforts (https://github.com/YouthDevelopmentInstituteUGA?tab=repositories).

A central neurobiological marker in this study is DMN rsFC, defined here as the mean correlation value across all within-DMN connections. The DMN comprises (Gordon et al., Reference Gordon, Laumann, Marek, Raut, Gratton, Newbold, Greene, Coalson, Snyder, Schlaggar, Petersen, Dosenbach and Nelson2020; Raichle et al., Reference Raichle, MacLeod, Snyder, Powers, Gusnard and Shulman2001), multiple regions, including the bilateral medial parietal cortex, medial and superior PFC, angular gyrus, medial and lateral temporal lobes, and cerebellum. As such, DMN rsFC reflects the average functional association among all these regions. Studies examining DMN rsFC at the network level have demonstrated numerous associations with normative and non-normative cognitive functioning (Whitfield-Gabrieli & Ford, Reference Whitfield-Gabrieli and Ford2012). While the DMN is indeed identifiable during resting-state fMRI as a single, unitary network, recent work indicates the presence of more nuanced organizational and functional architectures—subnetworks—within the DMN as well as other resting-state functional networks (Doucet et al., Reference Doucet, Naveau, Petit, Delcroix, Zago, Crivello, Jobard, Tzourio-Mazoyer, Mazoyer, Mellet and Joliot2011; Uddin et al., Reference Uddin, Clare Kelly, Biswal, Xavier Castellanos and Milham2009). This is a limitation because it narrows our ability to fully utilize the rich heterogeneity in brain function found within and across individuals in the ABCD Study broadly and, more specifically, assess what role subnetworks may play in the complex associations between early-life stress, risk for psychopathology, and resilience. As such, future work in this area should conceptualize and examine brain networks functioning at multiple levels of operation, from regional brain activity to subnetworks to networks, and develop user-friendly methods allowing researchers to do so in large, publicly available datasets.

Funding statement

Work on this manuscript was supported in part by the National Institute of Health and the National Institute on Drug Abuse, awarded to Dr Assaf Oshri (PI-R01 DA055630-01, R01 DA058334-01A1, K01DA045219 and Co-I P50DA051361). Data used in this article was obtained from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study (https://abcdstudy.org), held in the NIMH Data Archive. The ABCD Study is supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041048, U01DA050989, U01DA051016, U01DA041022, U01DA051018, U01DA051037, U01DA050987, U01DA041174, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041028, U01DA041134, U01DA050988, U01DA051039, U01DA041156, U01DA041025, U01DA041120, U01DA051038, U01DA041148, U01DA041093, U01DA041089, U24DA041123, and U24DA041147. We wish to thank all the research staff for designing the study, collecting, and preprocessing data, as well as the children, parents, teachers, and schools who participated in this multisite, longitudinal study.

Competing interests

None.

References

Achenbach, T. M., Dumenci, L., & Rescorla, L. A. (2003). Ratings of relations between DSM-IV diagnostic categories and items of the Adult Self-Report (ASR) and Adult Behavior Checklist (ABCL). Research Center for Children, Youth and Families.Google Scholar
Achenbach, T. M., McConaughy, S., Ivanova, M., & Rescorla, L. (2011). Manual for the ASEBA brief problem monitor (BPM) (pp. 33). ASEBA.Google Scholar
Afzali, M. H., Dagher, A., Bourque, J., Spinney, S., & Conrod, P. (2022). Cross-lagged relationships between depressive symptoms and altered default mode network connectivity over the course of adolescence. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 7(8), 774781.Google ScholarPubMed
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Smallwood, J., & Spreng, R. N. (2014). The default network and self-generated thought: Component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1316(1), 2952.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bassett, D. S., & Sporns, O. (2017). Network neuroscience. Nature Neuroscience, 20(3), 353364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bassett, D. S., Xia, C. H., & Satterthwaite, T. D. (2018). Understanding the emergence of neuropsychiatric disorders with network neuroscience. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 3(9), 742753.Google ScholarPubMed
Beauchaine, T. P., Klein, D. N., Crowell, S. E., Derbidge, C., & Gatzke-Kopp, L. (2009). Multifinality in the development of personality disorders: A biology× sex× environment interaction model of antisocial and borderline traits. Development and Psychopathology, 21(3), 735770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berman, I. S., McLaughlin, K. A., Tottenham, N., Godfrey, K., Seeman, T., Loucks, E., Suomi, S., Danese, A., & Sheridan, M. A. (2022). Measuring early life adversity: A dimensional approach. Development and Psychopathology, 34(2), 499511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolland, K. A., Bolland, J. M., Tomek, S., Devereaux, R. S., Mrug, S., & Wimberly, J. C. (2016). Trajectories of adolescent alcohol use by gender and early initiation status. Youth & Society, 48(1), 332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borawski, E. A., Ievers-Landis, C. E., Lovegreen, L. D., & Trapl, E. S. (2003). Parental monitoring, negotiated unsupervised time, and parental trust: The role of perceived parenting practices in adolescent health risk behaviors. Journal of Adolescent Health, 33(2), 6070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyce, W. T. (2016). Differential susceptibility of the developing brain to contextual adversity and stress. Neuropsychopharmacology, 41(1), 142162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broyd, S. J., Demanuele, C., Debener, S., Helps, S. K., James, C. J., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2009). Default-mode brain dysfunction in mental disorders: A systematic review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 33(3), 279296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burke, J. D., Pardini, D. A., & Loeber, R. (2008). Reciprocal relationships between parenting behavior and disruptive psychopathology from childhood through adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36(5), 679692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calabrese, E. J., & Baldwin, L. A. (2000). Chemical hormesis: Its historical foundations as a biological hypothesis. Human & Experimental Toxicology, 19(1), 231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, B. J., Cannonier, T., Conley, M. I., Cohen, A. O., Barch, D. M., Heitzeg, M. M., Soules, M. E., Teslovich, T., Dellarco, D. V., Garavan, H., Orr, C. A., Wager, T. D., Banich, M. T., Speer, N. K., Sutherland, M. T., Riedel, M. C., Dick, A. S., Bjork, J. M., Thomas, K. M., Chaarani, B., … Dale, A. M. (2018). The adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study: Imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 4354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, Y., & Baram, T. Z. (2016). Toward understanding how early-life stress reprograms cognitive and emotional brain networks. Neuropsychopharmacology, 41(1), 197206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. (1990). A historical perspective on the discipline of developmental psychopathology. In Rolf, J., Masten, A. S., Cicchetti, D., Nüchterlein, K. H., & Weintraub, S. (Eds.), Risk and protective factors in the development of psychopathology (pp. 228). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (2008). A multiple-levels-of-analysis perspective on research in development and psychopathology. In Beauchaine, T. P., & Hinshaw, S. P. (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychopathology (pp. 2757). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (2010). Resilience under conditions of extreme stress: A multilevel perspective. World Psychiatry, 9(3), 145154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Curtis, W. J. (2015). The developing brain and neural plasticity: Implications for normality, psychopathology, and resilience. In Developmental psychopathology: Volume two: Developmental neuroscience (pp. 164). Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8(4), 597600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2009). The past achievements and future promises of developmental psychopathology: The coming of age of a discipline. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(1-2), 1625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2016). Child maltreatment and developmental psychopathology: A multilevel perspective. Developmental Psychopathology, 3, 457512.Google Scholar
Corlis, M., & Damashek, A. (2021). Examining parental factors as moderators between maltreatment and adolescent delinquency. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 36(17-18), NP9391NP9415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cummings, E. M., & Davies, P. T. (2010). Marital conflict and children: An emotional security perspective. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Cummings, E. M., Koss, K. J., & Davies, P. T. (2015). Prospective relations between family conflict and adolescent maladjustment: Security in the family system as a mediating process. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43(3), 503515.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curtis, W. J., & Cicchetti, D. (2003). Moving research on resilience into the 21st century: Theoretical and methodological considerations in examining the biological contributors to resilience. Development and Psychopathology, 15(3), 773810.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, R. J., & McEwen, B. S. (2012). Social influences on neuroplasticity: Stress and interventions to promote well-being. Nature Neuroscience, 15(5), 689695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, P. T., & Cummings, E. M. (1994). Marital conflict and child adjustment: An emotional security hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 116(3), 387411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, P. T., Thompson, M. J., Hentges, R. F., Parry, L. Q., & Sturge-Apple, M. L. (2022). Interparental conflict as a quadratic predictor of children’s reactivity to interparental conflict and school adjustment: Steeling effects or risk saturation? Child Development, 93(2), 594611.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, P. T., Thompson, M. J., Martin, M. J., & Cummings, E. M. (2021). The vestiges of childhood interparental conflict: Adolescent sensitization to recent interparental conflict. Child Development, 92(3), 11541172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeCross, S. N., Sambrook, K. A., Sheridan, M. A., Tottenham, N., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2022). Dynamic alterations in neural networks supporting aversive learning in children exposed to trauma: Neural mechanisms underlying psychopathology. Biological Psychiatry, 91(7), 667675.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeWitt, S. J., Aslan, S., & Filbey, F. M. (2014). Adolescent risk-taking and resting state functional connectivity. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 222(3), 157164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doucet, G., Naveau, M., Petit, L., Delcroix, N., Zago, L., Crivello, F., Jobard, G. B. l, Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Mazoyer, B., Mellet, E., & Joliot, M. (2011). Brain activity at rest: A multiscale hierarchical functional organization. Journal of Neurophysiology, 105(6), 27532763.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, B. J., & Boyce, W. T. (2008). Biological sensitivity to context. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(3), 183187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Essex, M. J., & Boyce, W. T. (2005). Biological sensitivity to context: II. Empirical explorations of an evolutionary-developmental theory. Development and Psychopathology, 17(2), 303328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, B. J., Figueredo, A. J., Brumbach, B. H., & Schlomer, G. L. (2009). Fundamental dimensions of environmental risk: The impact of harsh versus unpredictable environments on the evolution and development of life history strategies. Human Nature, 20(2), 204268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enders, C. K., & Bandalos, D. L. (2001). The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling, 8(3), 430457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenneman, J., & Frankenhuis, W. E. (2020). Is impulsive behavior adaptive in harsh and unpredictable environments? A formal model. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(4), 261273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, K. T., Cassells, R. C., MacAllister, J. W., & Evans, G. W. (2013). The physical environment and child development: An international review. International Journal of Psychology, 48(4), 437468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fosco, G. M., & Feinberg, M. E. (2015). Cascading effects of interparental conflict in adolescence: Linking threat appraisals, self-efficacy, and adjustment. Development and Psychopathology, 27(1), 239252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, J. M., Abram, S. V., Reilly, J. L., Eack, S., Goldman, M. B., Csernansky, J. G., Wang, L., & Smith, M. J. (2017). Default mode functional connectivity is associated with social functioning in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126(4), 392405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fu, M., & Zuo, Y. (2011). Experience-dependent structural plasticity in the cortex. Trends in Neurosciences, 34(4), 177187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuentes-Claramonte, P., Martín-Subero, M., Salgado-Pineda, P., Alonso-Lana, S., Moreno-Alcázar, A., Argila-Plaza, I., Santo-Angles, A., Albajes-Eizagirre, A., Anguera-Camós, M., Capdevila, A., Sarró, S., McKenna, P. J., Pomarol-Clotet, E., Salvador, R., & Chao, L. (2019). Shared and differential default-mode related patterns of activity in an autobiographical, a self-referential and an attentional task. PloS One, 14(1), e0209376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fulong, X., Spruyt, K., Chao, L., Dianjiang, Z., Jun, Z., & Fang, H. (2020). Resting-state brain network topological properties and the correlation with neuropsychological assessment in adolescent narcolepsy. Sleep, 43(8), zsaa018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gabard-Durnam, L., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2020). Sensitive periods in human development: Charting a course for the future. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 36, 120128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabard-Durnam, L. J., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2019). Do sensitive periods exist for exposure to adversity? Biological Psychiatry, 85(10), 789791.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garavan, H., Bartsch, H., Conway, K., Decastro, A., Goldstein, R., Heeringa, S., Jernigan, T., Potter, A., Thompson, W., & Zahs, D. (2018). Recruiting the ABCD sample: Design considerations and procedures. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 1622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gard, A. M., Shaw, D. S., Forbes, E. E., & Hyde, L. W. (2018). Amygdala reactivity as a marker of differential susceptibility to socioeconomic resources during early adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 54(12), 23412355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geiser, C. (2020). Longitudinal structural equation modeling with Mplus: A latent state-trait perspective. Guilford publications.Google Scholar
Glynn, L. M., & Baram, T. Z. (2019). The influence of unpredictable, fragmented parental signals on the developing brain. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 53, 100736.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, E. M., Laumann, T. O., Adeyemo, B., Huckins, J. F., Kelley, W. M., & Petersen, S. E. (2016). Generation and evaluation of a cortical area parcellation from resting-state correlations. Cerebral Cortex, 26(1), 288303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, E. M., Laumann, T. O., Marek, S., Raut, R. V., Gratton, C., Newbold, D. J., Greene, D. J., Coalson, R. S., Snyder, A. Z., Schlaggar, B. L., Petersen, S. E., Dosenbach, N. U. F., & Nelson, S. M. (2020). Default-mode network streams for coupling to language and control systems. Proceedings of The National Academy of Sciences of The United States of America, 117(29), 1730817319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenough, W. T., Black, J. E., & Wallace, C. S. (1987). Experience and brain development. Child Development, 58(3), 539559. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130197 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greicius, M. D., Supekar, K., Menon, V., & Dougherty, R. F. (2009). Resting-state functional connectivity reflects structural connectivity in the default mode network. Cerebral Cortex, 19(1), 7278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grych, J. H., Fincham, F. D., Jouriles, E. N., & McDonald, R. (2000). Interparental conflict and child adjustment: Testing the mediational role of appraisals in the cognitive-contextual framework. Child Development, 71(6), 16481661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M. R., & Bowen, M. (2021). What was learned from studying the effects of early institutional deprivation. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 210, 173272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hagler, D. J. Jr, Hatton, S. N., Cornejo, M. D., Makowski, C., Fair, D. A., Dick, A. S., Sutherland, M. T., Casey, B. J., Barch, D. M., Harms, M. P., Watts, R., Bjork, J. M., Garavan, H. P., Hilmer, L., Pung, C. J., Sicat, C. S., Kuperman, J., Bartsch, H., Xue, F., Heitzeg, M. M., … Dale, A. M. (2019). Image processing and analysis methods for the adolescent brain cognitive development study. Neuroimage, 202, 116091.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartley, C. A., & Frankenhuis, W. E. (2020). Editorial overview: Sensitive and critical periods. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 36, iiiv.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heflin, C., Sandberg, J., & Rafail, P. (2009). The structure of material hardship in US households: An examination of the coherence behind common measures of well-being. Social Problems, 56(4), 746764.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, E. A., Clark, D. B., Orendain, N., Hudziak, J., Squeglia, L. M., & Dowling, G. J. (2019). Stress exposures, neurodevelopment and health measures in the ABCD study. Neurobiology of Stress, 10, 100157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horowitz-Kraus, T., Farah, R., Hajinazarian, A., Eaton, K., Rajagopal, A., Schmithorst, V. J., Altaye, M., Vannest, J. J., & Holland, S. K. (2017). Maturation of brain regions related to the default mode network during adolescence facilitates narrative comprehension. Journal of Child and Adolescent Behavior, 5(1), 328. https://doi.org/10.4172/2375-4494.1000328 Google Scholar
Hu, L.t, & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, C., Vinograd, M., Glynn, L. M., Davis, E. P., Baram, T. Z., Stern, H., Nievergelt, C., Cuccurazzu, B., Napan, C., Delmar, D., Baker, D. G., & Risbrough, V. B. (2023). Childhood unpredictability is associated with increased risk for long-and short-term depression and anhedonia symptoms following combat deployment. Journal of Mood & Anxiety Disorders, 100045. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.xjmad.2023.100045 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyde, L. W., Gard, A. M., Tomlinson, R. C., Burt, S. A., Mitchell, C., & Monk, C. S. (2020). An ecological approach to understanding the developing brain: Examples linking poverty, parenting, neighborhoods, and the brain. American Psychologist, 75(9), 12451259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inuggi, A., Sanz-Arigita, E., González-Salinas, C., Valero-García, A. V., García-Santos, J. M., & Fuentes, L. J. (2014). Brain functional connectivity changes in children that differ in impulsivity temperamental trait. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kievit, R. A., Brandmaier, A. M., Ziegler, G., van Harmelen, A.-L., de Mooij, S. M. M., Moutoussis, M., Goodyer, I. M., Bullmore, E., Jones, P. B., Fonagy, P., Lindenberger, U., & Dolan, R. J. (2018). Developmental cognitive neuroscience using latent change score models: A tutorial and applications. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 33, 99117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knudsen, E. I. (2004). Sensitive periods in the development of the brain and behavior. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(8), 14121425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kolb, B., & Gibb, R. (2014). Searching for the principles of brain plasticity and behavior. Cortex, 58, 251260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, S., Oshri, A., Kogan, S. M., Wickrama, K. A. S., & Sweet, L. (2021). Amygdalar activation as a neurobiological marker of differential sensitivity in the effects of family rearing experiences on socioemotional adjustment in youths. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 6(11), 10521062.Google ScholarPubMed
Lopez-Vergara, H. I., Spillane, N. S., Merrill, J. E., & Jackson, K. M. (2016). Developmental trends in alcohol use initiation and escalation from early to middle adolescence: Prediction by urgency and trait affect. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 30(5), 578587.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LoPilato, A. M., Addington, J., Bearden, C. E., Cadenhead, K. S., Cannon, T. D., Cornblatt, B. A., Mathalon, D. H., McGlashan, T. H., Perkins, D. O., Tsuang, M. T., Woods, S. W., & Walker, E. F. (2020). Stress perception following childhood adversity: Unique associations with adversity type and sex. Development and Psychopathology, 32(1), 343356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luthar, S. S., & Latendresse, S. J. (2005). Comparable “risks” at the socioeconomic status extremes: Preadolescents’ perceptions of parenting. Development and Psychopathology, 17(1), 207230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luthar, S. S., & Zelazo, L. B. (2003). Research on resilience: An integrative review. Resilience and Vulnerability: Adaptation in the Context of Childhood Adversities, 2, 510549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience processes in development. American Psychologist, 56(3), 227238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S., Best, K. M., & Garmezy, N. (1990). Resilience and development: Contributions from the study of children who overcome adversity. Development and Psychopathology, 2(4), 425444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2016). Resilience in development: Progress and transformation. Developmental Psychopathology, 4(3), 271333.Google Scholar
McLaughlin, K. A., & Sheridan, M. A. (2016). Beyond cumulative risk: A dimensional approach to childhood adversity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(4), 239245. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721416655883 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLaughlin, K. A., Sheridan, M. A., & Lambert, H. K. (2014). Childhood adversity and neural development: Deprivation and threat as distinct dimensions of early experience [Review article]. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 47, 578591. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menon, V. (2011). Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: A unifying triple network model. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(10), 483506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J. W., Stromeyer, W. R., & Schwieterman, M. A. (2013). Extensions of the Johnson-Neyman technique to linear models with curvilinear effects: Derivations and analytical tools. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 48(2), 267300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyagi, T., Oishi, N., Kobayashi, K., Ueno, T., Yoshimura, S., Murai, T., & Fujiwara, H. (2020). Psychological resilience is correlated with dynamic changes in functional connectivity within the default mode network during a cognitive task. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 17760.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moos, R. H., & Moos, B. S. (1994). Family environment scale manual: Development, applications, research. Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Muthén, B., & Muthén, B. O. (2009). Statistical analysis with latent variables (Vol. 123, No. 6). Wiley.Google Scholar
Nikolaidis, A., Heleniak, C., Fields, A., Bloom, P. A., VanTieghem, M., Vannucci, A., Camacho, N. L., Choy, T., Gibson, L., Harmon, C., Hadis, S. S., Douglas, I. J., Milham, M. P., & Tottenham, N. (2022). Heterogeneity in caregiving-related early adversity: Creating stable dimensions and subtypes. Development and Psychopathology, 34(2), 621634.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oshri, A. (2023). The hormesis model for building resilience through adversity: Attention to mechanism in developmental context. Review of General Psychology, 27(3), 245259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oshri, A., Cui, Z., Carvalho, C., & Liu, S. (2022). Is perceived stress linked to enhanced cognitive functioning and reduced risk for psychopathology? Testing the hormesis hypothesis. Psychiatry Research, 314, 114644.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oshri, A., Kogan, S. M., Kwon, J. A., Wickrama, K., Vanderbroek, L., Palmer, A. A., & Mackillop, J. (2018). Impulsivity as a mechanism linking child abuse and neglect with substance use in adolescence and adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, 30(2), 417435.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pessoa, L. (2014). Understanding brain networks and brain organization. Physics of Life Reviews, 11(3), 400435.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of The National Academy of Sciences of The United States of America, 98(2), 676682.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team (2022). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna. https://www.R-project.org/ Google Scholar
Rebello, K., Moura, L. M., Pinaya, W. H., Rohde, L. A., & Sato, J. R. (2018). Default mode network maturation and environmental adversities during childhood. Chronic Stress, 2, 2470547018808295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riley, J. R., & Masten, A. S. (2005). Resilience in context. In Resilience in children, families, and communities: Linking context to practice and policy (pp. 1325). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutter, M. (2006). Implications of resilience concepts for scientific understanding. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094(1), 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M. (2008). Developing concepts in developmental psychopathology. In Hudziak, J. J. (Ed.), Developmental psychopathology and wellness: Genetic and environmental influences (pp. 322). American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (2012). Resilience as a dynamic concept. Development and Psychopathology, 24(2), 335344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryan, J. P., Williams, A. B., & Courtney, M. E. (2013). Adolescent neglect, juvenile delinquency and the risk of recidivism. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42(3), 454465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schore, A. N. (1996). The experience-dependent maturation of a regulatory system in the orbital prefrontal cortex and the origin of developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8(1), 5987.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shonkoff, J. P., Garner, A. S., Dobbins, M. I., Earls, M. F., Garner, A. S., McGuinn, L., Pascoe, J., & Wood, D. L. (2012). The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics, 129(1), e232e246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sripada, C. S., Kessler, D., & Angstadt, M. (2014). Lag in maturation of the brain’s intrinsic functional architecture in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Proceedings of The National Academy of Sciences of The United States of America, 111(39), 1425914264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tashjian, S. M., Goldenberg, D., & Galván, A. (2017). Neural connectivity moderates the association between sleep and impulsivity in adolescents. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 27, 3544.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tashjian, S. M., Goldenberg, D., Monti, M. M., & Galván, A. (2018). Sleep quality and adolescent default mode network connectivity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 13(3), 290299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uddin, L. Q., Clare Kelly, A., Biswal, B. B., Xavier Castellanos, F., & Milham, M. P. (2009). Functional connectivity of default mode network components: Correlation, anticorrelation, and causality. Human Brain Mapping, 30(2), 625637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Von Bertalanffy, L. (1972). The history and status of general systems theory. Academy of Management Journal, 15(4), 407426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wade, M., Prime, H., Jenkins, J. M., Yeates, K. O., Williams, T., & Lee, K. (2018). On the relation between theory of mind and executive functioning: A developmental cognitive neuroscience perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(6), 21192140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, H., Verkes, R.-J., Roozendaal, B., & Hermans, E. J. (2019). Toward understanding developmental disruption of default mode network connectivity due to early life stress. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 4(1), 57.Google ScholarPubMed
Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., & Ford, J. M. (2012). Default mode network activity and connectivity in psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8(1), 4976.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whittle, S., Vijayakumar, N., Simmons, J. G., Dennison, M., Schwartz, O., Pantelis, C., Sheeber, L., Byrne, M. L., & Allen, N. B. (2017). Role of positive parenting in the association between neighborhood social disadvantage and brain development across adolescence. JAMA Psychiatry, 74(8), 824832.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wyman, P. A. (2003). Emerging perspectives on context specificity of children’s adaptation and resilience: Evidence from a decade of research with urban children in adversity. In Luthar, S. S. (Ed.), Resilience and vulnerability: Adaptation in the context of childhood adversities (pp. 293317). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511615788.014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xu, Y., Harms, M. B., Green, C. S., Wilson, R. C., & Pollak, S. D. (2023). Childhood unpredictability and the development of exploration. Proceedings of The National Academy of Sciences of The United States of America, 120(49), e2303869120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yaesoubi, M., Allen, E. A., Miller, R. L., & Calhoun, V. D. (2015). Dynamic coherence analysis of resting fMRI data to jointly capture state-based phase, frequency, and time-domain information. Neuroimage, 120, 133142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, L., Cui, Z., Huffman, L. G., & Oshri, A. (2023). Sleep mediates the effect of stressful environments on youth development of impulsivity: The moderating role of within default mode network resting-state functional connectivity. Sleep Health, 9(4), 503511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., & Skinner, E. A. (2016). The development of coping: Implications for psychopathology and resilience. In Cicchetti, D. (Ed.), Developmental psychopathology: Risk, resilience, and intervention (3rd ed., pp. 485545). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119125556.devpsy410 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zucker, R. A., Gonzalez, R., Ewing, S. W. F., Paulus, M. P., Arroyo, J., Fuligni, A., Morris, A. S., Sanchez, M., & Wills, T. (2018). Assessment of culture and environment in the adolescent brain and cognitive development study: Rationale, description of measures, and early data. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 107120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. The psychosocial hormesis model.

Figure 1

Table 1. Items factor loadings for family unpredictability, deprivation, and threat

Figure 2

Table 2. Longitudinal cubic regression parameters on internalizing and externalizing symptoms

Figure 3

Figure 2. Probing hormesis of family threat effect on internalizing symptoms.

Figure 4

Figure 3. Probing hormesis of deprivation on externalizing symptoms.

Figure 5

Figure 4. Simple slopes of the cubic family threat X change DMN rsFC interaction predicting youth internalizing problems in the full sample.

Figure 6

Figure 5. (a) Probing interaction by DMN LC in the strengthening zone. (b) Simple slopes of the family threat2 x DMN LC predicting internalizing problems in the toxic zone.

Figure 7

Figure 6. (a) Johnson-Neyman plot of cubic interaction by DMN LC strengthening zone. (b) Johnson-Neyman plot of family threat across values of the DMN LC when X is fixed at its mean (0).

Figure 8

Figure 7. Theoretical visualization of multifinality and equifinality.