Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T22:22:40.696Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The effects of acute tryptophan depletion on instrumental reward learning in anorexia nervosa – an fMRI study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 March 2022

Julius Steding
Affiliation:
Translational Developmental Neuroscience Section, Division of Psychological and Social Medicine and Developmental Neurosciences, Faculty of Medicine, TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany
Franziska Ritschel
Affiliation:
Translational Developmental Neuroscience Section, Division of Psychological and Social Medicine and Developmental Neurosciences, Faculty of Medicine, TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany
Ilka Boehm
Affiliation:
Translational Developmental Neuroscience Section, Division of Psychological and Social Medicine and Developmental Neurosciences, Faculty of Medicine, TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany
Daniel Geisler
Affiliation:
Translational Developmental Neuroscience Section, Division of Psychological and Social Medicine and Developmental Neurosciences, Faculty of Medicine, TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany
Joseph A. King
Affiliation:
Translational Developmental Neuroscience Section, Division of Psychological and Social Medicine and Developmental Neurosciences, Faculty of Medicine, TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany
Veit Roessner
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University Hospital C. G. Carus, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
Michael N. Smolka
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
Florian Daniel Zepf
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, Jena University Hospital, Friedrich Schiller University, Jena, Germany
Stefan Ehrlich*
Affiliation:
Translational Developmental Neuroscience Section, Division of Psychological and Social Medicine and Developmental Neurosciences, Faculty of Medicine, TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany Eating Disorder Treatment and Research Center, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
*
Author for correspondence: Stefan Ehrlich, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

The serotonin (5-HT) hypothesis of anorexia nervosa (AN) posits that individuals predisposed toward or recovered from AN (recAN) have a central nervous hyperserotonergic state and therefore restrict food intake as a means to reduce 5-HT availability (via diminished tryptophan-derived precursor supply) and alleviate associated negative mood states. Importantly, the 5-HT system has also been generally implicated in reward processing, which has also been shown to be altered in AN.

Methods

In this double-blind crossover study, 22 individuals recAN and 25 healthy control participants (HC) underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) while performing an established instrumental reward learning paradigm during acute tryptophan depletion (ATD; a dietary intervention that lowers central nervous 5-HT availability) as well as a sham depletion.

Results

On a behavioral level, the main effects of reward and ATD were evident, but no group differences were found. fMRI analyses revealed a group × ATD × reward level interaction in the ventral anterior insula during reward anticipation as well as in the medial orbitofrontal cortex during reward consumption.

Discussion

The precise pattern of results is suggestive of a ‘normalization’ of reward-related neural responses during ATD in recAN compared to HC. Our results lend further evidence to the 5-HT hypothesis of AN. Decreasing central nervous 5-HT synthesis and availability during ATD and possibly also by dieting may be a means to normalize 5-HT availability and associated brain processes.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Anorexia nervosa (AN) is a psychiatric disorder characterized by a relentless pursuit of thinness and, despite being significantly underweight, an intense fear of gaining weight. Despite its high mortality rate (Erskine, Whiteford, & Pike, Reference Erskine, Whiteford and Pike2016) and significant global burden (i.e. in terms of disability-adjusted life years) the etiology of AN is still a subject of ongoing research (Zipfel, Giel, Bulik, Hay, & Schmidt, Reference Zipfel, Giel, Bulik, Hay and Schmidt2015).

One potentially promising neurobiological model of AN is focused on the neurotransmitter serotonin (5-HT). While diminished central nervous system availability of the essential amino acid tryptophan (TRP), the precursor for 5-HT synthesis, has been proposed as a risk factor for depressive symptoms (Cowen & Browning, Reference Cowen and Browning2015), several lines of research point to increased 5-HT availability as a risk factor for AN. To identify potential markers for AN risk, previous studies have investigated weight-recovered individuals with a history of AN (recAN; Frank, Reference Frank2013). RecAN were shown to have increased cerebral spinal fluid (CSF) concentrations of the 5-HT metabolite 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA; Kaye, Reference Kaye1991; Kaye, Ebert, Raleigh, & Lake, Reference Kaye, Ebert, Raleigh and Lake1984). Additional evidence comes from studies investigating monoamine oxidase activity (Ehrlich et al., Reference Ehrlich, Franke, Schott, Salbach-Andrae, Pfeiffer, Lehmkuhl and Uebelhack2008), platelet 5-HT content (Ehrlich et al., Reference Ehrlich, Franke, Scherag, Burghardt, Schott, Schneider and Lehmkuhl2010) and cerebral 5-HT2 receptors (Frank et al., Reference Frank, Kaye, Meltzer, Price, Greer, McConaha and Skovira2002) in recAN. Interestingly, an increased central nervous 5-HT synthesis rate seems to be also associated with anxiety symptoms, and dieting may be a way to reduce the intake of the TRP and subsequently lower (or potentially normalize) central 5-HT synthesis and 5-HT availability (Bailer & Kaye, Reference Bailer, Kaye, Adan and Kaye2010; Kaye et al., 2003; Kaye, Bailer, Frank, Wagner, & Henry, Reference Kaye, Bailer, Frank, Wagner and Henry2005a; Kaye et al., Reference Kaye, Frank, Bailer, Henry, Meltzer, Price and Wagner2005b).

While, as described above, it has been hypothesized that premorbidly as well as after recovery from AN, (future/former) patients are in a hyperserotonergic state, i.e. they show an increased central nervous 5-HT availability, during the acute illness, the availability of TRP decreases due to restrictive eating. This may eventually ‘tip the scale’ toward a hyposerotonergic state, i.e. the central nervous 5-HT availability is decreased. Indeed, acutely underweight patients with AN have been shown to have reduced TRP plasma levels (Attia, Wolk, Cooper, Glasofer, & Walsh, Reference Attia, Wolk, Cooper, Glasofer and Walsh2005; Ehrlich et al., Reference Ehrlich, Franke, Schneider, Salbach-Andrae, Schott, Craciun and Lehmkuhl2009; Gauthier et al., Reference Gauthier, Hassler, Mattar, Launay, Callebert, Steiger and Godart2014) as well as reduced CSF concentrations of 5-HIAA (Demitrack, Heyes, Altemus, Pigott, & Gold, Reference Demitrack, Heyes, Altemus, Pigott and Gold1995; Kaye, Gwirtsman, George, Jimerson, & Ebert, Reference Kaye, Gwirtsman, George, Jimerson and Ebert1988), a reduction in platelet imipramine (Weizman, Carmi, Tyano, Apter, & Rehavi, Reference Weizman, Carmi, Tyano, Apter and Rehavi1986) and paroxetine (Bruce, Steiger, Ng Ying Kin, & Israel, Reference Bruce, Steiger, Ng Ying Kin and Israel2006; Ramacciotti, Coli, Paoli, Marazziti, & Dell'Osso, Reference Ramacciotti, Coli, Paoli, Marazziti and Dell'Osso2003) binding, a lower density of peripheral 5-HT2a receptors (Sigurdh, Allard, Spigset, & Hägglöf, Reference Sigurdh, Allard, Spigset and Hägglöf2013) and decreased central 5-HT2a receptor binding (Audenaert et al., Reference Audenaert, Van Laere, Dumont, Vervaet, Goethals, Slegers and Dierckx2003). Of note, dieting at the beginning of AN has been associated with (temporary) positive effects on mood and affect lability (Fitzsimmons-Craft et al., Reference Fitzsimmons-Craft, Accurso, Ciao, Crosby, Cao, Pisetsky and Wonderlich2015; Selby et al., Reference Selby, Cornelius, Fehling, Kranzler, Panza, Lavender and Grange2015). While a reduction of food intake leading to lower TRP availability and a subsequently reduced 5-HT synthesis in healthy controls (HCs) triggers hyperphagia (Burke & Heisler, Reference Burke and Heisler2015), patients with AN show continued low energy intake as well as low body weight.

A well-established experimental method to temporarily mimic changes of the central nervous 5-HT system is acute tryptophan depletion (ATD; Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Wong, Mahfouda, Morandini, Rao, Runions and Zepf2020). By ingesting mixtures of large neutral amino acids (LNAA) and high or low concentrations of TRP, the transport and availability of TRP and consequentially 5-HT synthesis in the brain can be modulated for several hours (Bell, Hood, & Nutt, Reference Bell, Hood and Nutt2005; Hood, Bell, & Nutt, Reference Hood, Bell and Nutt2005; Moja et al., Reference Moja, Stoff, Gessa, Castoldi, Assereto and Tofanetti1988). ATD has been used to study the malleability of cognitive functions such as memory, attention and cognitive control as well as reward and emotional processing (Fusar-Poli et al., Reference Fusar-Poli, Allen, McGuire, Placentino, Cortesi and Perez2006; Kanen et al., Reference Kanen, Arntz, Yellowlees, Christmas, Price, Apergis-Schoute and Robbins2021; Mendelsohn, Riedel, & Sambeth, Reference Mendelsohn, Riedel and Sambeth2009). It has also been employed to investigate 5-HT functioning in several psychiatric disorders, especially in mood and anxiety disorders (Bell, Abrams, & Nutt, Reference Bell, Abrams and Nutt2001; Comai, Bertazzo, Brughera, & Crotti, Reference Comai, Bertazzo, Brughera, Crotti and Eou2020). In the only published ATD study with AN to date, recAN participants showed reduced anxiety levels (Kaye et al., Reference Kaye, Barbarich, Putnam, Gendall, Fernstrom, Fernstrom and Kishore2003), supporting the hypothesis of a hyperserotonergic state as an antecedent of AN.

In addition to the aforementioned possible alterations in the central nervous 5-HT system, there is converging evidence for alterations in reward processing in AN (O'Hara, Campbell, & Schmidt, Reference O'Hara, Campbell and Schmidt2015). Most prominently, AN patients are able to suppress the drive to eat while being undernourished, but they also show anhedonic responses to other primary rewards such as sex and pleasant touch (Bischoff-Grethe et al., Reference Bischoff-Grethe, Wierenga, Berner, Simmons, Bailer, Paulus and Kaye2018; Crucianelli et al., Reference Crucianelli, Demartini, Goeta, Nisticò, Saramandi, Bertelli and Fotopoulou2020; Raboch & Faltus, Reference Raboch and Faltus1991) and in general a reduced sensitivity to reward in self-report studies (Harrison, O'Brien, Lopez, & Treasure, Reference Harrison, O'Brien, Lopez and Treasure2010). With respect to neural correlates, study results have shown altered neural responses to disorder-related (e.g. food) as well as disorder-unrelated (e.g. money) stimuli in typical regions of the brain reward system such as the ventral striatum (VS; Cowdrey, Park, Harmer, & McCabe, Reference Cowdrey, Park, Harmer and McCabe2011; Decker, Figner, & Steinglass, Reference Decker, Figner and Steinglass2015; Fladung et al. Reference Fladung, Grön, Grammer, Herrnberger, Schilly, Grasteit and von Wietersheim2010; Frank et al. Reference Frank, Reynolds, Shott, Jappe, Yang, Tregellas and O'Reilly2012; Wagner et al. Reference Wagner, Aizenstein, Mazurkewicz, Fudge, Frank, Putnam and Kaye2008; Wierenga et al. Reference Wierenga, Bischoff-Grethe, Melrose, Irvine, Torres, Bailer and Kaye2015) and the medial orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC; Frank et al., Reference Frank, Reynolds, Shott, Jappe, Yang, Tregellas and O'Reilly2012; Uher et al., Reference Uher, Brammer, Murphy, Campbell, Ng, Williams and Treasure2003).

While dopamine (DA) has long been regarded as the main neurotransmitter system involved in reward processing, a growing number of studies using experimental manipulations of 5-HT synthesis in humans have implicated the 5-HT system to have effects on reward (and punishment) processing (Cools, Roberts, & Robbins, Reference Cools, Roberts and Robbins2008; Crockett, Clark, & Robbins, Reference Crockett, Clark and Robbins2009; Evers et al., Reference Evers, Cools, Clark, van der Veen, Jolles, Sahakian and Robbins2005; Schweighofer et al., Reference Schweighofer, Bertin, Shishida, Okamoto, Tanaka, Yamawaki and Doya2008; Seymour, Daw, Roiser, Dayan, & Dolan, Reference Seymour, Daw, Roiser, Dayan and Dolan2012). Depending on the 5-HT receptor subtype, 5-HT can enhance or inhibit the dopaminergic effects in the brain reward system (Cools, Nakamura, & Daw, Reference Cools, Nakamura and Daw2011; Daw, Kakade, & Dayan, Reference Daw, Kakade and Dayan2002; Di Matteo, Di Giovanni, Pierucci, & Esposito, Reference Di Matteo, Di Giovanni, Pierucci and Esposito2008). In animal studies, there is evidence that the additive effect of DA and 5-HT results in a strongly rewarding sensation, whereas each of the neurotransmitters alone does not yield the same effects (Fischer & Ullsperger, Reference Fischer and Ullsperger2017). Taken together, these results dovetail with a prominent theory that posits that the reward system is also modulated by the 5-HT system (Cools et al., Reference Cools, Nakamura and Daw2011). According to this view, a possible underlying computational mechanism may include that decreasing 5-HT availability increases the expected (average) outcome and thereby modulate computational parameters of reward processing and reward-based decision-making. Although the literature on the neural correlates of reward processing during 5-HT manipulations in humans is overall relatively sparse, several pharmacological fMRI studies using ATD and selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRI) have shown blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal changes in regions typically involved in reward processing such as the VS, the OFC as well as the insula (for a review, see Macoveanu, Reference Macoveanu2014).

In order to probe serotonergic modulation of reward-related neural responses in the context of AN, the current double-blind crossover study used ATD in combination with an established instrumental motivation task (Bühler et al., Reference Bühler, Vollstädt-Klein, Kobiella, Budde, Reed, Braus and Smolka2010; Ehrlich et al., Reference Ehrlich, Geisler, Ritschel, King, Seidel, Boehm and Kroemer2015) using monetary rewards during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in a sample of recAN and carefully age-matched HC. We hypothesized that a hyperserotonergic state is an underlying risk factor in the pathogenesis of AN, which is (temporarily) lowered by food restriction at the onset of the disorder but, due to severe emaciation, deteriorates into a hyposerotonergic state in the acutely underweight state of the disorder. Therefore, an experimental reduction of 5-HT via ATD should ‘normalize’ altered brain responses to reward in recAN individuals, for which purpose we focused on three-way interactions of ATD condition, group and reward in the BOLD signal.

Methods

Sample description

For this study, we recruited a total of 49 female participants. The final sample consisted of 47 individuals (n = 22 recAN and n = 25 HC) who completed both study sessions (i.e. ATD and sham depletion). Two HC dropped out between the two sessions (n = 1 due to subjective intolerance of the ATD procedure and n = 1 due to scheduling difficulties). To be considered ‘recovered’, participants had to (1) maintain a body mass index (BMI) of >18.5 (age 19 to 29) or be above the 10th BMI percentile with respect to their age (age 12 to 18; Kromeyer-Hauschild et al., Reference Kromeyer-Hauschild, Wabitsch, Kunze, Geller, Geiß, Hesse and Korte2001) for at least 12 months, (2) menstruate, and (3) refrain from binging, purging or other substantially restrictive eating patterns. The control group consisted of normal-weight, eumenorrhoeic women with no history of psychiatric illness. We generally excluded participants if they had a lifetime history of any of the following clinical diagnoses: bulimia nervosa or binge eating disorder, schizophrenia, substance dependence, bipolar disorder and suicidality. Further exclusion criteria for all participants were incomplete ATD drink intake, nausea or other related intolerance of the ATD mixture, IQ lower than 85, organic brain syndrome, chronic medical or neurological illnesses that could affect appetite, eating behavior or body weight (e.g. diabetes), pregnancy, breast feeding, use of psychotropic medications or substances within the 4 weeks preceding the study. See Supplemental Material 1 (SM 1) for a full list of exclusion criteria.

This study was approved by the ethics committee of the Technische Universität Dresden and carried out in accordance with the latest version of the Declaration of Helsinki, and all participants (and their guardians if underage) gave written informed consent.

Clinical measures

For all participants, the presence or absence of current diagnoses of eating disorders was ascertained by evaluation of the expert form of the Structured interview of AN and bulimia nervosa (SIAB-EX; Fichter & Quadflieg, Reference Fichter and Quadflieg1999). Additionally, a structured clinical interview (Mini-DIPS; Margraf, Reference Margraf2013) was used to rule out other active psychiatric disorders. Interviews were conducted by clinically experienced and trained research assistants under the supervision of the attending child and adolescent psychiatrist.

In addition to the clinical interviews, eating disorder-specific psychopathology was assessed with the German version of the Eating Disorders Inventory (EDI-2; Paul & Thiel, Reference Paul and Thiel2005). Furthermore, depressive symptoms were examined using the Beck Depression Inventory II (BDI-II; Hautzinger, Kühner, & Keller, Reference Hautzinger, Kühner and Keller2009). IQ was assessed with a short version of the German adaption of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (von Aster, Neubauer, & Horn, Reference von Aster, Neubauer and Horn2006). BMI standard deviation scores (BMI-SDS) were calculated for each participant, which is controlled for both age and sex (Kromeyer-Hauschild et al., Reference Kromeyer-Hauschild, Wabitsch, Kunze, Geller, Geiß, Hesse and Korte2001).

Procedure

The ATD procedure entailed a double-blind, randomized crossover design with each participant undergoing both the depletion and the sham depletion within a period of 7–14 days to avoid potential carry-over effects (M = 9.64, s.d. = 3.21). On the day prior to the study appointments, participants were asked to restrict meals to low protein food and given a list with meal suggestions. Immediately before the ATD and sham depletion intake at 8.30 a.m., participants were weighed. The ATD and sham depletion drinks were then prepared using a weight-adapted dosing regimen (Moja et al., Reference Moja, Stoff, Gessa, Castoldi, Assereto and Tofanetti1988; Zepf et al., Reference Zepf, Sánchez, Biskup, Kötting, Bubenzer, Helmbold and Kuhn2014; for a more detailed description of the ATD mixture, see the Supplements 2). After the ATD/sham depletion intake, participants were given a TRP free breakfast. 4 h after the ATD/sham depletion intake, the MRI session was performed. Participants were continuously supervised by trained staff.

Biochemical measures

Blood samples were taken before the ATD/sham depletion intake as well as approximately 5 h later immediately after the MRI session using a venous catheter (Braunüle®) and ethylenediaminetetraacetic (EDTA) tubes (Sarstedt, Germany). For more details on the biochemical measures and analyses, see SM 3.

To quantify the depletion effect, the ratio of plasma TRP to LNAAs (namely valine, methionine, isoleucine, leucine, phenylalanine, thyrosine) was calculated for each participant using the values of the blood samples immediately after the MRI session (5 h after ATD or sham depletion intake).

Instrumental motivation task

During the fMRI session, participants performed the same instrumental motivation task (Bühler et al., Reference Bühler, Vollstädt-Klein, Kobiella, Budde, Reed, Braus and Smolka2010; Kroemer et al., Reference Kroemer, Guevara, Ciocanea Teodorescu, Wuttig, Kobiella and Smolka2014) that we have used in previous studies in both acute (Steding et al., Reference Steding, Boehm, King, Geisler, Ritschel, Seidel and Ehrlich2019) and weight-recovered (Ehrlich et al., Reference Ehrlich, Geisler, Ritschel, King, Seidel, Boehm and Kroemer2015) AN samples. In addition to allowing for measurement of event-related brain activity in response to stimuli predicting monetary reward (reward anticipation) and feedback about the magnitude of the reward received, this particular task variant has the advantage of providing behavioral assessment of motivation operationalized as instrumental responding (number of button presses, #bp) to maximize reward. Each trial included an anticipation phase, a motor response phase, and a feedback (receipt) phase (Fig. 1 for details). The scanning session started with an eight-trial test run to determine each individual's maximum #bp. This information was used to standardize the cumulative monetary gain to ≈ €10 in the subsequent main run, irrespective of inter-individual performance differences in motor speed (for more information, see SM 4).

Fig. 1. Instrumental motivation task. Instrumental motivation task during event-related functional MRI (fMRI). During the anticipation phase a visual cue was presented for 3 s to inform the participant about the reward level of this trial [reward levels: 0 (no reward), 1, 10, 100]. The motor (or instrumental) response phase started after a 2 s fixation period. Monetary reward per trial increased with reward level and higher effort and was determined by multiplying number of button presses × reward level × an individual adjustment factor (calculated based on the individual maximum #bp in the test run; for details see Bühler et al., Reference Bühler, Vollstädt-Klein, Kobiella, Budde, Reed, Braus and Smolka2010). Acoustic feedback for button presses was provided through headphones. After another fixation period of 4 s, feedback was provided for 3 s by displaying the amount of money gained in this trial and the cumulative amount. Between trials, participants fixated on crosshairs for 3 s (75%) or 7.44 s in 25% of all trials, which improves design efficiency by jittering. The fMRI main run had a total duration of 15.5 min and comprised 48 trials in total (4 reward levels × 12 pseudorandomized repetitions).

Instrumental response data analysis

We compared average #bp and reaction times (RT) of initial responses at each reward level between recAN and HC participants using linear mixed-effects models for the analysis of repeated measurement treating participants as random effects. We assumed a compound symmetry covariance structure for changes of instrumental response by reward level (0, 1, 10, 100) and included group (recAN, HC) and individual TRP/LNAA ratios as indicators of the ATD condition as factors as well as interaction effects (slope) between all variables. Since the variable indicating reward level was centered (and HC were used as a reference for the factor group) the intercept in HC (and intercept + main effect of group in recAN) represents not instrumental responses at reward level 0 but the ‘typical’ response, i.e. it correlates highly with the response rate at an average reward level.

Structural and functional image acquisition, data processing and analysis

Images were acquired using standard sequences with a 3 T whole-body MRI scanner (TRIO; Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) equipped with a standard head coil (SM 5). Functional and structural images were processed with SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) within the Nipype framework (http://nipy.sourceforge.net/nipype/; Gorgolewski, Storkey, Bastin, & Pernet, Reference Gorgolewski, Storkey, Bastin and Pernet2012) following standard procedures (SM 6). We evaluated fMRI data quality by manual inspection and using the artifact detection tool (ART; Whitfield-Gabrieli et al., Reference Whitfield-Gabrieli, Thermenos, Milanovic, Tsuang, Faraone, McCarley and Seidman2009). Volumes that exceed an intensity threshold of three standard deviations or a threshold of 2 mm normalized movement in any direction were classified as outliers and excluded from statistical models (motion-outlier: recAN individuals, day 1: 2.47 ± 10.31, day 2: 0.35 ± 0.87; HC, day 1: 0.05 ± 2.13, day 2: 0.18 ± 0.66; intensity-outlier: recAN individuals, day 1: 6.84 ± 6.87, day 2: 7.75 ± 6.23; HC, day 1: 5.27 ± 4.47, day 2: 8.14 ± 6.66).

On the single-subject level, a general linear model (GLM) was fit to model the hemodynamic response to increasing reward levels. We modeled all four reward levels of the anticipation, motor response and feedback phase as single events (12 regressors). Additional regressors included six motion parameters as well as one regressor for each motion or intensity outlier volume (see SM 6).

To test our hypothesis regarding the effect of ATD on reward processing in AN in comparison to HC with second-level whole-brain analyses, we used neuropointillist (http://ibic.github.io/neuropointillist/; Madhyastha et al., Reference Madhyastha, Peverill, Koh, McCabe, Flournoy, Mills and McLaughlin2018) to fit linear mixed models in R Studio (nlme package) to each voxel for each participant separately for the anticipation and feedback phase during both time points. The mixed model to investigate the three-way interaction of ATD, group and reward comprised the four centered reward levels, group as well as individual TRP/LNAA ratios as indicators of the ATD condition. This procedure then yields statistical parameter maps for each main and interaction effect. To control for false-positives, family-wise error (FWE) correction was performed using 3DClustSim (http://afni.nimh.nih.gov/afni; ‘fixed’ version compiled June 2017). Afterward, to aid interpretation of significant whole-brain results and since neuropointillist only yields statistical parameter maps, we obtained extracted beta values averaged from significant clusters of three-way interaction effects using the MarsBaR toolbox for SPM (Brett, Anton, Valabregue, & Poline, Reference Brett, Anton, Valabregue and Poline2002) and ran the same linear mixed models (in a post-hoc approach) with the extracted indices using R Studio (nlme package).

Results

Sample characteristics

There were no significant differences between the groups regarding age, IQ, BMI, BMI-SDS as well as BDI-II. However, AN symptoms as measured by EDI-2 were higher in the recAN group compared to the HC (Table 1).

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the sample

Notes. All data of this table refers to day 1 of the study. recAN = Individuals recovered from anorexia nervosa, HC = healthy control participants. Group differences were tested using Student's t tests. IQ = intelligence quotient, BMI = body mass index, BMI-SDS = body mass index standard deviation score, EDI-2 = Eating Disorder Inventory 2, BDI-II = Beck Depression Inventory-II. For time since recovery, values ranged from 12 to 140 months. Eighteen recAN individuals were of the restrictive subtype and four of the binge/purge subtype. For duration of illness, the values for n = 4 recAN participants were not obtainable; values ranged from 0.5 to 9.8 years.

Plasma tryptophan levels

Demonstrating a successful tryptophan manipulation in both groups, tryptophan levels were significantly lower during the ATD condition than during the sham depletion (F (1, 44) = 334.4, p < 0.0001; Figure S.1 in SM 7).

Instrumental response data

As in previous studies with the employed paradigm (Bühler et al., Reference Bühler, Vollstädt-Klein, Kobiella, Budde, Reed, Braus and Smolka2010; Ehrlich et al., Reference Ehrlich, Geisler, Ritschel, King, Seidel, Boehm and Kroemer2015; Steding et al., Reference Steding, Boehm, King, Geisler, Ritschel, Seidel and Ehrlich2019), RTs decreased (F (1, 312) = 70.02, p < 0.0001) and #bp increased (F (1, 312) = 104.48, p < 0.0001) with ascending reward level as expected, but no significant group differences were evident (for both F (1, 44)<0.7, n.s.). For #bp, there was a significant main effect for the ATD treatment (F (1, 312) = 13.47, p < 0.001) with higher #bp values during the depletion condition (M = 13.4, s.d. = 2.88; sham depletion: M = 12.75, s.d. = 2.3). For both behavioral measures, none of the interactions were significant. For more details, see Fig. 2 as well as Table S.1 in SM 8.

Fig. 2. Behavioral data of both groups during both conditions. Each dot represents the mean value of one participant for each reward level (0, 1, 10, 100). The left panel shows the distribution of reaction times and the right one the number of button presses (plus smooth regression lines). DEPL = acute tryptophan depletion; SHAM = sham depletion; recAN = individuals with a history of AN; HC = healthy control participants.

Neuroimaging data

Anticipation phase

As a proof of concept, we first investigated the main effect of reward level during reward anticipation and found a parametric linear increase in both the left and the right v. (see Fig. 3) as in our previous studies (Ehrlich et al., Reference Ehrlich, Geisler, Ritschel, King, Seidel, Boehm and Kroemer2015; Steding et al., Reference Steding, Boehm, King, Geisler, Ritschel, Seidel and Ehrlich2019). Regarding our motivating hypothesis of a group difference in the effect of ATD on reward processing, we uncovered significant results in our mixed models in both the left (peak voxel at x = −38, y = −4, z = −8; F = 15.41) and the right (x = 38, y = 4, z = −14; F = 16.02) ventral anterior insula during reward anticipation (three-way interaction of group × depletion condition × reward level; Figure 3). BOLD parameter estimates extracted from the identified bilateral clusters revealed a significant main effect of reward level (left: F (1, 319) = 27.94, p < 0.0001; right: F (1, 323) = 10.75, p = 0.001) in addition to confirming the aforementioned three-way interaction (left: F (1, 319) = 22.80, p < 0.001; right: F (1, 323) = 15.24, p < 0.001; Figure 3) as expected. Post-hoc t tests of the reward-related BOLD signal slopes suggested that the recAN group during the sham depletion is the main factor contributing to the three-way interaction (Table S.2 in SM 1). More specifically, the BOLD signal slopes of the recAN group during the sham depletion condition were significantly flatter (i.e. less negative) compared to the slopes of the HC group in the same condition as well as compared to the same recAN group during the ATD condition. Additionally, recAN during ATD showed no significant difference compared to HC during the sham condition. Taken together, this could be an indication of an ATD-dependent normalization of BOLD responses in former AN patients.

Fig. 3. (a) Results of the whole-brain voxelwise mixed model of the reward anticipation phase. Left: significant three-way interaction of group × depletion condition × reward level within the left ventral anterior insula (x = −39). Right: significant three-way interaction of intervention × group × reward level within the right ventral anterior insula (x = 39). FWE corrected with p < 0.05. (b) Mean activation in each group in each reward level (β estimates plus a smooth regression line) in the left and right ventral anterior insula during the reward anticipation phase at both time points (ATD and sham depletion). DEPL = acute tryptophan depletion; SHAM = sham depletion; recAN = individuals with a history of AN; HC = healthy control participants; BOLD = blood oxygen level dependent.

Feedback phase

We also found a group difference in the effect of ATD on reward processing during feedback (three-way interaction) in our mixed model analyses in the mOFC (peak voxel at x = 4, y = 60, z = −16; F = 15.16; Fig. 4). BOLD parameter estimates extracted from the identified cluster revealed a main effect of reward level (F (1, 323) = 82.23, p < 0.0001) and a depletion condition × reward level interaction (F (1, 323) = 7.74, p = 0.006) in addition to confirming, as expected, the aforementioned three-way interaction (F (1, 323) = 16.06, p = 0.0001; Fig. 4). Similar to the anticipation phase, post-hoc t tests of the reward-related BOLD signal slopes suggested that the recAN group during the sham depletion is the main factor contributing to the three-way interaction, thus indicating that under ATD, reward-related BOLD responses seem to normalize in former AN patients (Table S.3 in SM 11).

Fig. 4. Results of the whole-brain voxelwise mixed model of the reward feedback phase. Left: Significant three-way interaction of group × depletion condition × reward level within the mOFC (x = 5). FWE corrected with p < 0.05. Right: Mean activation in each group for each reward level (β estimates plus a smooth regression line) in the mOFC during the reward feedback phase during both time points (ATD and sham depletion). mOFC = medial orbitofrontal cortex; DEPL = acute tryptophan depletion; SHAM = sham depletion; recAN = individuals with a history of AN; HC = healthy control participants; BOLD = blood oxygen level dependent.

Exploratory brain behavior correlations

Given that the observed group differences during reward anticipation and consumption are seemingly attributable to the abnormal activation in recAN during sham depletion, we explored potential brain-behavior relationships between recAN group activation under this condition (slope of each participant) and age, BMI-SDS, EDI-2 total score as well as BDI-II. However, there were no significant correlations (for more detailed information, see Table S.4 in SM 12).

Discussion

In this fMRI study, we investigated the influence of an experimental modulation of central nervous 5-HT availability (ATD) on reward processing in weight-recovered women with a history of AN. While there were only group-independent effects of ATD on behavioral responses, we found a three-way interaction of the group, ATD condition and reward level in both the bilateral ventral anterior insula during the reward anticipation phase as well as in the mOFC during the feedback phase. This pattern of results was mainly attributable to BOLD responses in recAN participants during sham depletion. In the depletion condition, however, recAN individuals showed BOLD signal slopes similar to those of HC during depletion and the sham depletion. Based on the 5-HT hypothesis of AN that AN-related food restriction mitigates a hyperserotonergic state (Kaye et al., Reference Kaye, Barbarich, Putnam, Gendall, Fernstrom, Fernstrom and Kishore2003, Reference Kaye, Frank, Bailer, Henry, Meltzer, Price and Wagner2005b), one possible interpretation of these findings is that ATD reduces (potentially ‘normalizes’) hyperserotonergic functioning in recAN.

On the behavioral level, we found the main effect of ATD treatment on instrumental responses indicating higher #bp in the depletion relative to sham condition. This is in line with previous studies reporting generally faster response speed and relative disinhibition after ATD (Cools et al., Reference Cools, Nakamura and Daw2011; Crockett et al., Reference Crockett, Clark and Robbins2009). Although we did not find a faster average RT of the initial response, potentially due to floor effects, the general increase in average #bp is indicative of faster responding for the following button presses. This supports the notion of a behavioral disinhibition effect of tryptophan depletion (Walderhaug et al., Reference Walderhaug, Lunde, Nordvik, Landrø, Refsum and Magnusson2002), even though it has been proposed to be more prominent in response to aversive stimuli and punishment (Faulkner & Deakin, Reference Faulkner and Deakin2014).

Our neuroimaging findings in the insula and mOFC fit well with several previous studies using manipulations of 5-HT availability which have reported alterations in both of these regions (for a review, see Macoveanu, Reference Macoveanu2014). For example, Seymour and colleagues (Seymour et al., Reference Seymour, Daw, Roiser, Dayan and Dolan2012) found an interaction of ATD and reward indicating a dependence of the reward outcome value on 5-HT signaling in both the mOFC as well as the anterior insula in healthy participants performing a probabilistic instrumental learning task. The (anterior) insula has long been overlooked in the reward literature despite being connected to limbic regions such as the mOFC, v. and the amygdala (Sescousse, Caldú, Segura, & Dreher, Reference Sescousse, Caldú, Segura and Dreher2013). It is this interconnectedness that led to the hypothesis of the insula being one of the most relevant brain regions with respect to the psychopathology as well as the neuropsychological and neural alterations regularly observed in AN patients (Nunn, Frampton, Gordon, & Lask, Reference Nunn, Frampton, Gordon and Lask2008). For example, studies investigating sensory-interoceptive reward signals in individuals recovered from AN (recAN) found altered anterior insula activation in response to sweet taste (Oberndorfer et al., Reference Oberndorfer, Frank, Simmons, Wagner, McCurdy, Fudge and Kaye2013a; Wagner et al., Reference Wagner, Aizenstein, Mazurkewicz, Fudge, Frank, Putnam and Kaye2008) and food images (Oberndorfer et al., Reference Oberndorfer, Simmons, McCurdy, Strigo, Matthews, Yang and Kaye2013b).

In line with the hypothesis that vulnerability toward AN may be associated with a hyperserotonergic state (Bailer & Kaye, Reference Bailer, Kaye, Adan and Kaye2010; Kaye et al., Reference Kaye, Barbarich, Putnam, Gendall, Fernstrom, Fernstrom and Kishore2003, Reference Kaye, Bailer, Frank, Wagner and Henry2005a, Reference Kaye, Frank, Bailer, Henry, Meltzer, Price and Wagner2005b), we found altered BOLD signal patterns during sham depletion but an apparent relative normalization of neural activity in the ventral anterior insula as well as the mOFC in recAN following ATD. In accord with this account, pharmacological studies using SSRIs to treat AN failed to show significant clinical effects on both body weight and mood (for a review, see Frank, Reference Frank2020); potentially because pharmacologically increasing 5-HT availability in the brain may worsen a pre-existing hyperserotonergic state. This is consistent with the finding by Kaye et al. (Reference Kaye, Barbarich, Putnam, Gendall, Fernstrom, Fernstrom and Kishore2003) of an anxiolytic effect in recAN compared to HC following an ATD intervention which laid the foundation of the 5-HT hypothesis in AN. This may explain why AN patients report dysphoric mood with increasing food intake during and after treatment (Frank & Kaye, Reference Frank and Kaye2012) as well as positive effects on mood (Fitzsimmons-Craft et al., Reference Fitzsimmons-Craft, Accurso, Ciao, Crosby, Cao, Pisetsky and Wonderlich2015) and positive affect lability (Selby et al., Reference Selby, Cornelius, Fehling, Kranzler, Panza, Lavender and Grange2015) in association with restrictive eating. Interestingly, the absence of significant correlations in our exploratory analyses of brain-behavior relationships of the clinical markers in recANs during sham depletion hints toward the presence of a trait marker in recAN since the neural responses were not related to residual symptoms. Taken together, the 5-HT hypothesis could be of interest with respect to clinical implications. For example, pharmacological agents that lower or modulate 5-HT such as 5-HT antagonists could prove to be useful in treating AN. However, more research is needed since several of already existing drugs are not only antagonists to 5-HT, but also to other neurotransmitters such as dopamine and histamine which makes causal conclusions difficult (Frank, Reference Frank2020). Additionally, this research must be extended to other eating disorders in which the central 5-HT system has been implicated as well such as bulimia nervosa (Kaye et al., Reference Kaye, Gendall, Fernstrom, Fernstrom, McConaha and Weltzin2000).

The interpretation of the findings of this study should be seen in the context of several limitations. Although a within-subject design has greater statistical power than between-subject designs and may thus detect effects with smaller samples, future studies with larger samples are needed to replicate and confirm our results. It should also be noted that although monetary rewards enable generalization with respect to reward processing, they tend to show mixed results in AN samples (Haynos, Lavender, Nelson, Crow, & Peterson, Reference Haynos, Lavender, Nelson, Crow and Peterson2020) and additional studies using other rewards (e.g. positive social feedback) might be particularly informative to the 5-HT hypothesis. Furthermore, while being widely used and validated for both adults and adolescents (for a review, see Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Wong, Mahfouda, Morandini, Rao, Runions and Zepf2020), the ATD intervention may have certain weaknesses. The success and the impact of ATD on brain 5-HT availability is judged by plasma TRP/LNAA ratios, which is only an indirect measure. Accordingly, conclusions about selective serotonergic effects should be made with caution (van Donkelaar et al., Reference van Donkelaar, Blokland, Ferrington, Kelly, Steinbusch and Prickaerts2011; but see also the response by Crockett et al., Reference Crockett, Clark, Roiser, Robinson, Cools, Chase and Robbins2012; as well as this review Young, Reference Young2013). However, Nishizawa and colleagues have measured the actual influx of TRP into the brain after ATD using PET and confirmed its effectiveness (Nishizawa et al., Reference Nishizawa, Benkelfat, Young, Leyton, Mzengeza, de Montigny and Diksic1997) and Ardis et al. (Reference Ardis, Cahir, Elliott, Bell, Reynolds and Cooper2009) reported evidence for a relatively selective serotonergic effect of ATD since it did not alter concentrations of other monoaminergic neurotransmitters. Regarding the definition of AN recovery, the lack of a broad consensus has been criticized. In our sample, one of the inclusion criteria was the resumption of mensis and the duration of recovery exceeded the recommended time period of 12 months (mean recovery time of 55.73 months; Wade & Lock, Reference Wade and Lock2020). Lastly, with respect to the notion that a hyperserotonergic state might be relevant in the pathogenesis of AN, it should be noted that with the design of this study, it can not be concluded whether this is a trait or a scar effect (Frank, Reference Frank2013; Seidel et al., Reference Seidel, Ehrlich, Breithaupt, Welch, Wiklund, Hübel and Bulik2020).

To conclude, this study adds further evidence supporting the 5-HT hypothesis of AN (Bailer & Kaye, Reference Bailer, Kaye, Adan and Kaye2010; Kaye et al., Reference Kaye, Barbarich, Putnam, Gendall, Fernstrom, Fernstrom and Kishore2003, Reference Kaye, Frank, Bailer, Henry, Meltzer, Price and Wagner2005b) by showing an apparent ‘normalization’ of neural patterns (BOLD signal) in the ventral anterior insula and the mOFC during reward processing in individuals recAN during ATD. Since this is the first study that investigated reward processing in AN in combination with ATD, further studies are needed to confirm our findings (Horster et al., Reference Horster, Nickel, Holovics, Schmidt, Endres, Tebartz van Elst and Joos2020). If confirmed, such a mechanism might help to explain why dieting is often reported as increasing the subjective well-being (at least initially) of individuals with or at risk for AN (Miyasaka et al., Reference Miyasaka, Yoshiuchi, Yamanaka, Sasaki, Kumano and Kuboki2003). Furthermore, these findings also have clinical implications. For example, treatment approaches using pharmacological agents such as (partial) serotonin antagonists may be an interesting avenue for future research with the aim to balance out potentially increasing 5-HT availability during refeeding (Frank & Shott, Reference Frank and Shott2016).

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721005493.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Dr Marina Stopsack of the Institute for Clinical Chemistry and Laboratory Medicine of the Technische Universität Dresden for support with the laboratory blood analysis and the Center for Information Services and High Performance Computing (ZIH) at TU Dresden for generous allocations of computer time. Lastly, the authors would like to thank the numerous medical students and other interns for their assistance with participant recruitment and data collection all participants for their time and cooperation.

Financial support

This work was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (EH 367/5-1, EH 367/7-1 and SFB 940).

Conflicts of interest

VR has received payment for consulting and writing activities from Lilly, Novartis and Shire Pharmaceuticals/Takeda, lecture honoraria from Lilly, Novartis, Shire Pharmaceuticals/Takeda, and Medice Pharma, and support for research from Shire/Takeda and Novartis. VR has carried out clinical trials in cooperation with the Novartis, Shire Pharmaceuticals/Takeda and Otsuka companies. VR has no financial relationship with the organizations that sponsored the research. FDZ was the recipient of an unrestricted award donated by the American Psychiatric Association (APA), the American Psychiatric Institute for Research and Education (APIRE) and AstraZeneca (Young Minds in Psychiatry Award). FDZ has also received research support from the European Union, German Federal Ministry for Economics and Technology, the German Society for Social Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, the Paul and Ursula Klein Foundation, the Dr August Scheidel Foundation, the IZKF fund of the University Hospital of RWTH Aachen University, the Telethon Perth Children's Hospital Research Fund (TPCHR); the Princess Margaret Foundation, and a travel stipend donated by the GlaxoSmithKline Foundation. FDZ is the recipient of an unrestricted educational grant, travel support and speaker honoraria by Shire pharmaceuticals, Germany. FDZ also received speaker honoraria by Takeda Pharmaceuticals, Germany. In addition, FDZ has received support from the Raine Foundation for Medical Research (Raine Visiting Professorship), and editorial fees from Co-Action Publishing (Sweden). JS, FR, IB, DG, JAK, MNS and SE have nothing to declare.

References

Ardis, T. C., Cahir, M., Elliott, J. J., Bell, R., Reynolds, G. P., & Cooper, S. J. (2009). Effect of acute tryptophan depletion on noradrenaline and dopamine in the rat brain. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 23(1), 5155. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881108089597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Attia, E., Wolk, S., Cooper, T., Glasofer, D., & Walsh, B. T. (2005). Plasma tryptophan during weight restoration in patients with anorexia nervosa. Biological Psychiatry, 57(6), 674678. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.045.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Audenaert, K., Van Laere, K., Dumont, F., Vervaet, M., Goethals, I., Slegers, G., … Dierckx, R. A. (2003). Decreased 5-HT2a receptor binding in patients with anorexia nervosa. Journal of Nuclear Medicine: Official Publication, Society of Nuclear Medicine, 44(2), 163169.Google ScholarPubMed
Bailer, U. F., & Kaye, W. H. (2010). Serotonin: Imaging findings in eating disorders. In Adan, R. A. H., & Kaye, W. H. (Eds.), Behavioral neurobiology of eating disorders (pp. 5979). Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2010_78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell, C., Abrams, J., & Nutt, D. (2001). Tryptophan depletion and its implications for psychiatry. British Journal of Psychiatry, 178(5), 399405. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.178.5.399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, C., Hood, S. D., & Nutt, D. J. (2005). Acute tryptophan Depletion. Part II: Clinical effects and implications. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 39(7), 565574. https://doi.org/10.1080/j.1440-1614.2005.01628.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bischoff-Grethe, A., Wierenga, C. E., Berner, L. A., Simmons, A. N., Bailer, U., Paulus, M. P., & Kaye, W. H. (2018). Neural hypersensitivity to pleasant touch in women remitted from anorexia nervosa. Translational Psychiatry, 8(1), 161. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-018-0218-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brett, M., Anton, J.-L., Valabregue, R., & Poline, J.-B. (2002). Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox. 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain, 16, 497.Google Scholar
Bruce, K. R., Steiger, H., Ng Ying Kin, N. M. K., & Israel, M. (2006). Reduced platelet [3H]paroxetine binding in anorexia nervosa: Relationship to eating symptoms and personality pathology. Psychiatry Research, 142(2–3), 225232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2005.08.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bühler, M., Vollstädt-Klein, S., Kobiella, A., Budde, H., Reed, L. J., Braus, D. F., … Smolka, M. N. (2010). Nicotine dependence Is characterized by disordered reward processing in a network driving motivation. Biological Psychiatry, 67(8), 745752. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.10.029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burke, L. K., & Heisler, L. K. (2015). 5-Hydroxytryptamine medications for the treatment of obesity. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 27(6), 389398. https://doi.org/10.1111/jne.12287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Comai, S., Bertazzo, A., Brughera, M., & Crotti, S. (2020). Tryptophan in health and disease. In Eou, E. (Ed.), Advances in clinical chemistry (Vol. 95, pp. 165218). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.acc.2019.08.005.Google Scholar
Cools, R., Nakamura, K., & Daw, N. D. (2011). Serotonin and dopamine: Unifying affective, activational, and decision functions. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36(1), 98113. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2010.121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cools, R., Roberts, A. C., & Robbins, T. W. (2008). Serotoninergic regulation of emotional and behavioural control processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(1), 3140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.10.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowdrey, F. A., Park, R. J., Harmer, C. J., & McCabe, C. (2011). Increased neural processing of rewarding and aversive food stimuli in recovered anorexia nervosa. Biological Psychiatry, 70(8), 736743. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.05.028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowen, P. J., & Browning, M. (2015). What has serotonin to do with depression? World Psychiatry, 14(2), 158160. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crockett, M. J., Clark, L., & Robbins, T. W. (2009). Reconciling the role of serotonin in behavioral inhibition and aversion: Acute tryptophan depletion abolishes punishment-induced inhibition in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(38), 1199311999. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2513-09.2009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crockett, M. J., Clark, L., Roiser, J. P., Robinson, O. J., Cools, R., Chase, H. W., … Robbins, T. W. (2012). Converging evidence for central 5-HT effects in acute tryptophan depletion. Molecular Psychiatry, 17(2), 121123. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2011.106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crucianelli, L., Demartini, B., Goeta, D., Nisticò, V., Saramandi, A., Bertelli, S., … Fotopoulou, A. (2020). The anticipation and perception of affective touch in women with and recovered from anorexia nervosa. Neuroscience, 464, S0306452220305790. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2020.09.013.Google ScholarPubMed
Daw, N. D., Kakade, S., & Dayan, P. (2002). Opponent interactions between serotonin and dopamine. Neural Networks, 15(4–6), 603616. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0893-6080(02)00052-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decker, J. H., Figner, B., & Steinglass, J. E. (2015). On weight and waiting: Delay discounting in anorexia nervosa pretreatment and posttreatment. Biological Psychiatry, 78(9), 606614. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.12.016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demitrack, M. A., Heyes, M. P., Altemus, M., Pigott, T. A., & Gold, P. W. (1995). Cerebrospinal fluid levels of kynurenine pathway metabolites in patients with eating disorders: Relation to clinical and biochemical variable. Biological Psychiatry, 37(8), 512520. https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-3223(94)00173-Z.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Di Matteo, V., Di Giovanni, G., Pierucci, M., & Esposito, E. (2008). Serotonin control of central dopaminergic function: Focus on in vivo microdialysis studies. In Progress in brain research (Vol. 172, pp. 744). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(08)00902-3.Google Scholar
Ehrlich, S., Franke, L., Scherag, S., Burghardt, R., Schott, R., Schneider, N., … Lehmkuhl, U. (2010). The 5-HTTLPR polymorphism, platelet serotonin transporter activity and platelet serotonin content in underweight and weight-recovered females with anorexia nervosa. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 260(6), 483490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehrlich, S., Franke, L., Schneider, N., Salbach-Andrae, H., Schott, R., Craciun, E. M., … Lehmkuhl, U. (2009). Aromatic amino acids in weight-recovered females with anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 42(2), 166172. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.20575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehrlich, S., Franke, L., Schott, R., Salbach-Andrae, H., Pfeiffer, E., Lehmkuhl, U., & Uebelhack, R. (2008). Platelet monoamine oxidase activity in underweight and weight-recovered females with anorexia nervosa. Pharmacopsychiatry, 41(06), 226231. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2008-1078749.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehrlich, S., Geisler, D., Ritschel, F., King, J., Seidel, M., Boehm, I., … Kroemer, N. (2015). Elevated cognitive control over reward processing in recovered female patients with anorexia nervosa. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 40(5), 307315. https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.140249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erskine, H. E., Whiteford, H. A., & Pike, K. M. (2016). The global burden of eating disorders. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 29(6), 346353. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evers, E. A. T., Cools, R., Clark, L., van der Veen, F. M., Jolles, J., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2005). Serotonergic modulation of prefrontal cortex during negative feedback in probabilistic reversal learning. Neuropsychopharmacology, 30(6), 11381147. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1300663.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faulkner, P., & Deakin, J. F. W. (2014). The role of serotonin in reward, punishment and behavioural inhibition in humans: Insights from studies with acute tryptophan depletion. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 46, 365378. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.07.024.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fichter, M., & Quadflieg, N. (1999). SIAB. Strukturiertes inventar für anorektische und bulimische essstörungen nach DSM-IV und ICD-10. Bern: Huber.Google Scholar
Fischer, A. G., & Ullsperger, M. (2017). An update on the role of serotonin and its interplay with dopamine for reward. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 484. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitzsimmons-Craft, E. E., Accurso, E. C., Ciao, A. C., Crosby, R. D., Cao, L., Pisetsky, E. M., … Wonderlich, S. A. (2015). Restrictive eating in anorexia nervosa: Examining maintenance and consequences in the natural environment: Restrictive eating in anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 48(7), 923931. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fladung, A.-K., Grön, G., Grammer, K., Herrnberger, B., Schilly, E., Grasteit, S., … von Wietersheim, J. (2010). A neural signature of anorexia nervosa in the ventral striatal reward system. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(2), 206212. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09010071.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, G. K. W. (2013). Altered brain reward circuits in eating disorders: Chicken or egg? Current Psychiatry Reports, 15(10), 17. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-013-0396-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, G. K. W. (2020). Pharmacotherapeutic strategies for the treatment of anorexia nervosa – too much for one drug? Expert Opinion on Pharmacotherapy, 21(9), 10451058. https://doi.org/10.1080/14656566.2020.1748600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, G. K. W., & Kaye, W. H. (2012). Current status of functional imaging in eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 45(6), 723736. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, G. K. W., Kaye, W. H., Meltzer, C. C., Price, J. C., Greer, P., McConaha, C., & Skovira, K. (2002). Reduced 5-HT2A receptor binding after recovery from anorexia nervosa. Biological Psychiatry, 52(9), 896906. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01378-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, G. K. W., Reynolds, J. R., Shott, M. E., Jappe, L., Yang, T. T., Tregellas, J. R., & O'Reilly, R. C. (2012). Anorexia nervosa and obesity are associated with opposite brain reward response. Neuropsychopharmacology, 37(9), 20312046. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2012.51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, G. K. W., & Shott, M. E. (2016). The role of psychotropic medications in the management of anorexia nervosa: Rationale, evidence and future prospects. CNS Drugs, 30(5), 419442. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40263-016-0335-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fusar-Poli, P., Allen, P., McGuire, P., Placentino, A., Cortesi, M., & Perez, J. (2006). Neuroimaging and electrophysiological studies of the effects of acute tryptophan depletion: A systematic review of the literature. Psychopharmacology, 188(2), 131143. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-006-0493-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gauthier, C., Hassler, C., Mattar, L., Launay, J.-M., Callebert, J., Steiger, H., … Godart, N. (2014). Symptoms of depression and anxiety in anorexia nervosa: Links with plasma tryptophan and serotonin metabolism. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 39, 170178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.09.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gorgolewski, K. J., Storkey, A. J., Bastin, M. E., & Pernet, C. R. (2012). Adaptive thresholding for reliable topological inference in single subject fMRI analysis. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 114. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrison, A., O'Brien, N., Lopez, C., & Treasure, J. (2010). Sensitivity to reward and punishment in eating disorders. Psychiatry Research, 177(1–2), 111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2009.06.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hautzinger, M., Kühner, C., & Keller, F. (2009). BDI-II Beck-Depressions-Inventar. Frankfurt am Main, Germany: Pearson.Google Scholar
Haynos, A. F., Lavender, J. M., Nelson, J., Crow, S. J., & Peterson, C. B. (2020). Moving towards specificity: A systematic review of cue features associated with reward and punishment in anorexia nervosa. Clinical Psychology Review, 79, 101872. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101872.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hood, S. D., Bell, C., & Nutt, D. J. (2005). Acute tryptophan depletion. Part I: Rationale and methodology. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 39(7), 558564. https://doi.org/10.1080/j.1440-1614.2005.01627.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horster, I., Nickel, K., Holovics, L., Schmidt, S., Endres, D., Tebartz van Elst, L., … Joos, A. (2020). A neglected topic in neuroscience: Replicability of fMRI results with specific reference to ANOREXIA NERVOSA. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 11, 777. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanen, J. W., Arntz, F. E., Yellowlees, R., Christmas, D. M., Price, A., Apergis-Schoute, A. M., … Robbins, T. W. (2021). Effect of tryptophan depletion on conditioned threat memory expression: Role of intolerance of uncertainty. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 6(5), 590598. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.12.012.Google ScholarPubMed
Kaye, W. H. (1991). Altered serotonin activity in anorexia nervosa after long-term weight restoration: Does elevated cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid level correlate with rigid and obsessive behavior? Archives of General Psychiatry, 48(6), 556. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1991.01810300068010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaye, W. H., Bailer, U. F., Frank, G. K. W., Wagner, A., & Henry, S. E. (2005a). Brain imaging of serotonin after recovery from anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Physiology & Behavior, 86(1–2), 1517. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.06.019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaye, W. H., Barbarich, N. C., Putnam, K., Gendall, K. A., Fernstrom, J., Fernstrom, M., … Kishore, A. (2003). Anxiolytic effects of acute tryptophan depletion in anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 33(3), 257267. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.10135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaye, W. H., Ebert, M. H., Raleigh, M., & Lake, R. (1984). Abnormalities in CNS monoamine metabolism in anorexia nervosa. Archives of General Psychiatry, 41(4), 350. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1984.01790150040007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaye, W. H., Frank, G. K. W., Bailer, U., Henry, S., Meltzer, C., Price, J., … Wagner, A. (2005b). Serotonin alterations in anorexia and bulimia nervosa: New insights from imaging studies. Physiology & Behavior, 85(1), 7381. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.04.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaye, W. H., Gendall, K. A., Fernstrom, M. H., Fernstrom, J. D., McConaha, C. W., & Weltzin, T. E. (2000). Effects of acute tryptophan depletion on mood in bulimia nervosa. Biological Psychiatry, 47(2), 151157. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00108-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaye, W. H., Gwirtsman, H. E., George, D. T., Jimerson, D. C., & Ebert, M. H. (1988). CSF 5-HIAA concentrations in anorexia nervosa: Reduced values in underweight subjects normalize after weight gain. Biological Psychiatry, 23(1), 102105. https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-3223(88)90113-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kroemer, N. B., Guevara, A., Ciocanea Teodorescu, I., Wuttig, F., Kobiella, A., & Smolka, M. N. (2014). Balancing reward and work: Anticipatory brain activation in NAcc and VTA predict effort differentially. NeuroImage, 102, 510519. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.060.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kromeyer-Hauschild, K., Wabitsch, M., Kunze, D., Geller, F., Geiß, H. C., Hesse, V., … Korte, W. (2001). Perzentile für den body-mass-Index für das Kindes-und Jugendalter unter Heranziehung verschiedener deutscher Stichproben. Monatsschrift Kinderheilkunde, 149(8), 807818.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macoveanu, J. (2014). Serotonergic modulation of reward and punishment: Evidence from pharmacological fMRI studies. Brain Research, 1556, 1927. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2014.02.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madhyastha, T., Peverill, M., Koh, N., McCabe, C., Flournoy, J., Mills, K., … McLaughlin, K. A. (2018). Current methods and limitations for longitudinal fMRI analysis across development. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 33, 118128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.11.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Margraf, J. (2013). Mini-DIPS: Diagnostisches kurz-interview bei psychischen störungen; handbuch. Berlin Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Mendelsohn, D., Riedel, W. J., & Sambeth, A. (2009). Effects of acute tryptophan depletion on memory, attention and executive functions: A systematic review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 33(6), 926952. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.03.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyasaka, N., Yoshiuchi, K., Yamanaka, G., Sasaki, T., Kumano, H., & Kuboki, T. (2003). Relations among premorbid weight, referral weight, and psychological test scores for patients with anorexia nervosa. Psychological Reports, 92(1), 6774. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.2003.92.1.67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moja, E. A., Stoff, D. M., Gessa, G. L., Castoldi, D., Assereto, R., & Tofanetti, O. (1988). Decrease in plasma tryptophan after tryptophan-free amino acid mixtures in man. Life Sciences, 42(16), 15511556. https://doi.org/10.1016/0024-3205(88)90013-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nishizawa, S., Benkelfat, C., Young, S. N., Leyton, M., Mzengeza, S., de Montigny, C., … Diksic, M. (1997). Differences between males and females in rates of serotonin synthesis in human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 94(10), 53085313. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.94.10.5308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nunn, K., Frampton, I., Gordon, I., & Lask, B. (2008). The fault is not in her parents but in her insula-A neurobiological hypothesis of anorexia nervosa. European Eating Disorders Review, 16(5), 355360. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.890.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oberndorfer, T. A., Frank, G. K. W., Simmons, A. N., Wagner, A., McCurdy, D., Fudge, J. L., … Kaye, W. H. (2013a). Altered insula response to sweet taste processing after recovery from anorexia and Bulimia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(10), 11431151. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.11111745.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oberndorfer, T. A., Simmons, A., McCurdy, D., Strigo, I., Matthews, S., Yang, T., … Kaye, W. (2013b). Greater anterior insula activation during anticipation of food images in women recovered from anorexia nervosa versus controls. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 214(2), 132141. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.06.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Hara, C. B., Campbell, I. C., & Schmidt, U. (2015). A reward-centred model of anorexia nervosa: A focussed narrative review of the neurological and psychophysiological literature. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 52, 131152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.02.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paul, T., & Thiel, A. (2005). Eating disorder inventory-2 (EDI-2): Deutsche version. Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Raboch, J., & Faltus, F. (1991). Sexuality of women with anorexia nervosa. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 84(1), 911. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1991.tb01412.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramacciotti, C. E., Coli, E., Paoli, R., Marazziti, D., & Dell'Osso, L. (2003). Serotonergic activity measured by platelet [3H]paroxetine binding in patients with eating disorders. Psychiatry Research, 118(1), 3338. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-1781(03)00059-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schweighofer, N., Bertin, M., Shishida, K., Okamoto, Y., Tanaka, S. C., Yamawaki, S., & Doya, K. (2008). Low-serotonin levels increase delayed reward discounting in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(17), 45284532. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4982-07.2008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seidel, M., Ehrlich, S., Breithaupt, L., Welch, E., Wiklund, C., Hübel, C., … Bulik, C. M. (2020). Study protocol of comprehensive risk evaluation for anorexia nervosa in twins (CREAT): A study of discordant monozygotic twins with anorexia nervosa. BMC Psychiatry, 20(1), 507. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-020-02903-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Selby, E. A., Cornelius, T., Fehling, K. B., Kranzler, A., Panza, E. A., Lavender, J. M., … Grange, D. L. (2015). A perfect storm: Examining the synergistic effects of negative and positive emotional instability on promoting weight loss activities in anorexia nervosa. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 113. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sescousse, G., Caldú, X., Segura, B., & Dreher, J.-C. (2013). Processing of primary and secondary rewards: A quantitative meta-analysis and review of human functional neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(4), 681696. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seymour, B., Daw, N. D., Roiser, J. P., Dayan, P., & Dolan, R. (2012). Serotonin selectively modulates reward value in human decision-making. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(17), 58335842. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0053-12.2012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sigurdh, J., Allard, P., Spigset, O., & Hägglöf, B. (2013). Platelet serotonin transporter and 5-HT 2A receptor binding in adolescents with eating disorders. International Journal of Neuroscience, 123(5), 333338. https://doi.org/10.3109/00207454.2012.761215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steding, J., Boehm, I., King, J. A., Geisler, D., Ritschel, F., Seidel, M., … Ehrlich, S. (2019). Goal-directed vs. habitual instrumental behavior during reward processing in anorexia nervosa: An fMRI study. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 13529. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-49884-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stewart, R. M., Wong, J. W. Y., Mahfouda, S., Morandini, H. A. E., Rao, P., Runions, K. C., & Zepf, F. D. (2020). Acute tryptophan depletion Moja-De: A method to study central nervous serotonin function in children and adolescents. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10, 1007. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.01007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uher, R., Brammer, M. J., Murphy, T., Campbell, I. C., Ng, V. W., Williams, S. C. R., & Treasure, J. (2003). Recovery and chronicity in anorexia nervosa: Brain activity associated with differential outcomes. Biological Psychiatry, 54(9), 934942. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00172-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Donkelaar, E. L., Blokland, A., Ferrington, L., Kelly, P. A. T., Steinbusch, H. W. M., & Prickaerts, J. (2011). Mechanism of acute tryptophan depletion: Is it only serotonin? Molecular Psychiatry, 16(7), 695713. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2011.9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
von Aster, M. G., Neubauer, A. C., & Horn, R. (2006). WIE – wechsler intelligenztest für erwachsene. Bern: Huber.Google Scholar
Wade, T. D., & Lock, J. (2020). Developing consensus on the definition of remission and recovery for research. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 53(8), 12041208. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.23165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, A., Aizenstein, H., Mazurkewicz, L., Fudge, J., Frank, G. K. W., Putnam, K., … Kaye, W. H. (2008). Altered insula response to taste stimuli in individuals recovered from restricting-type anorexia nervosa. Neuropsychopharmacology, 33(3), 513523. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1301443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walderhaug, E., Lunde, H., Nordvik, J., Landrø, N., Refsum, H., & Magnusson, A. (2002). Lowering of serotonin by rapid tryptophan depletion increases impulsiveness in normal individuals. Psychopharmacology, 164(4), 385391. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-002-1238-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weizman, R., Carmi, M., Tyano, S., Apter, A., & Rehavi, M. (1986). High affinity [3H] imipramine binding and serotonin uptake to platelets of adolescent females suffering from anorexia nervosa. Life Sciences, 38(13), 12351242. https://doi.org/10.1016/0024-3205(86)90179-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Thermenos, H. W., Milanovic, S., Tsuang, M. T., Faraone, S. V., McCarley, R. W., … Seidman, L. J. (2009). Hyperactivity and hyperconnectivity of the default network in schizophrenia and in first-degree relatives of persons with schizophrenia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(4), 12791284. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0809141106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wierenga, C. E., Bischoff-Grethe, A., Melrose, A. J., Irvine, Z., Torres, L., Bailer, U. F., … Kaye, W. H. (2015). Hunger does not motivate reward in women remitted from anorexia nervosa. Biological Psychiatry, 77(7), 642652. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.09.024.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, S. (2013). Acute tryptophan depletion in humans: A review of theoretical, practical and ethical aspects. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 38(5), 294305. https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.120209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zepf, F. D., Sánchez, C. L., Biskup, C. S., Kötting, W. F., Bubenzer, S., Helmbold, K., … Kuhn, C. M. (2014). Acute tryptophan depletion – converging evidence for decreasing central nervous serotonin synthesis in rodents and humans. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 129(2), 157159. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.12215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zipfel, S., Giel, K. E., Bulik, C. M., Hay, P., & Schmidt, U. (2015). Anorexia nervosa: Aetiology, assessment, and treatment. The Lancet Psychiatry, 2(12), 10991111. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00356-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Instrumental motivation task. Instrumental motivation task during event-related functional MRI (fMRI). During the anticipation phase a visual cue was presented for 3 s to inform the participant about the reward level of this trial [reward levels: 0 (no reward), 1, 10, 100]. The motor (or instrumental) response phase started after a 2 s fixation period. Monetary reward per trial increased with reward level and higher effort and was determined by multiplying number of button presses × reward level × an individual adjustment factor (calculated based on the individual maximum #bp in the test run; for details see Bühler et al., 2010). Acoustic feedback for button presses was provided through headphones. After another fixation period of 4 s, feedback was provided for 3 s by displaying the amount of money gained in this trial and the cumulative amount. Between trials, participants fixated on crosshairs for 3 s (75%) or 7.44 s in 25% of all trials, which improves design efficiency by jittering. The fMRI main run had a total duration of 15.5 min and comprised 48 trials in total (4 reward levels × 12 pseudorandomized repetitions).

Figure 1

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the sample

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Behavioral data of both groups during both conditions. Each dot represents the mean value of one participant for each reward level (0, 1, 10, 100). The left panel shows the distribution of reaction times and the right one the number of button presses (plus smooth regression lines). DEPL = acute tryptophan depletion; SHAM = sham depletion; recAN = individuals with a history of AN; HC = healthy control participants.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. (a) Results of the whole-brain voxelwise mixed model of the reward anticipation phase. Left: significant three-way interaction of group × depletion condition × reward level within the left ventral anterior insula (x = −39). Right: significant three-way interaction of intervention × group × reward level within the right ventral anterior insula (x = 39). FWE corrected with p < 0.05. (b) Mean activation in each group in each reward level (β estimates plus a smooth regression line) in the left and right ventral anterior insula during the reward anticipation phase at both time points (ATD and sham depletion). DEPL = acute tryptophan depletion; SHAM = sham depletion; recAN = individuals with a history of AN; HC = healthy control participants; BOLD = blood oxygen level dependent.

Figure 4

Fig. 4. Results of the whole-brain voxelwise mixed model of the reward feedback phase. Left: Significant three-way interaction of group × depletion condition × reward level within the mOFC (x = 5). FWE corrected with p < 0.05. Right: Mean activation in each group for each reward level (β estimates plus a smooth regression line) in the mOFC during the reward feedback phase during both time points (ATD and sham depletion). mOFC = medial orbitofrontal cortex; DEPL = acute tryptophan depletion; SHAM = sham depletion; recAN = individuals with a history of AN; HC = healthy control participants; BOLD = blood oxygen level dependent.

Supplementary material: File

Steding et al. supplementary material

Steding et al. supplementary material 2

Download Steding et al. supplementary material(File)
File 15.7 KB
Supplementary material: File

Steding et al. supplementary material

Steding et al. supplementary material 2

Download Steding et al. supplementary material(File)
File 303.5 KB