Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T14:17:44.327Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Talk with me! Parental linguistic input to toddlers with moderate hearing loss

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 November 2019

Evelien DIRKS*
Affiliation:
Department of Developmental Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
Angela STEVENS
Affiliation:
Dutch Foundation for the Deaf and Hard of Hearing Child, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Sigrid KOK
Affiliation:
Dutch Foundation for the Deaf and Hard of Hearing Child, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Johan FRIJNS
Affiliation:
Department of Otorhinolaryngology and Head & Neck Surgery, Leiden University Medical Center, Leiden, The Netherlands Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, Leiden University Medical Center, Leiden, The Netherlands
Carolien RIEFFE
Affiliation:
Dutch Foundation for the Deaf and Hard of Hearing Child, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Department of Developmental Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands School of Psychology and Human Development, Institute of Education, University College London, London, United Kingdom
*
*Corresponding author: Developmental Psychology, Leiden University, PO Box 9555, 2300RB Leiden, The Netherlands. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This study examined the quantity and quality of parental linguistic input to toddlers with moderate hearing loss (MHL) compared with toddlers with normal hearing (NH). The linguistic input to eighteen toddlers with MHL and twenty-four toddlers with NH was examined during a 10-minute free-play activity in their home environment. Results showed that toddlers with MHL were exposed to an equivalent amount of parental linguistic input compared to toddlers with NH. However, parents of toddlers with MHL used less high-level facilitative language techniques, used less mental state language, and used shorter utterances than parents of toddlers with NH. Quantity and quality measures of parental linguistic input were positively related to the expressive language abilities of toddlers with MHL.

Type
Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2019

Introduction

Language acquisition occurs by means of interactions with knowledgeable others (Vygotsky, Reference Vygotsky1978). Parents have a crucial role in the language development of their young children. By talking to children about what they are seeing or doing, parents promote children's language abilities. Both the quantity and quality of parental linguistic input can impact a child's language development (Hart & Risley, Reference Hart and Risley1995; Rowe, Reference Rowe2012; Weizman & Snow, Reference Weizman and Snow2001).

When a child has a moderate hearing loss (40–60 dB HL) (MHL), parents may encounter more challenges in providing optimal language input (in terms of amount and quality of talk) to their children. Since most children with hearing loss (HL) have hearing parents (Mitchell & Karchmer, Reference Mitchell and Karchmer2004), parents often have no experience with HL, which may hamper their intuitive parent behavior. Given that children with MHL are more at risk for language difficulties (Moeller, Tomblin, Yoshinaga-Itano, Connor, & Jerger, Reference Moeller, Tomblin, Yoshinaga-Itano, Connor and Jerger2007; Tomblin et al., Reference Tomblin, Harrison, Ambrose, Walker, Oleson and Moeller2015), parents may need to adapt their linguistic input in order to enhance their child's language development. In the current study, the relationship between the quantity and quality of parental linguistic input and the language abilities of toddlers with MHL was examined.

Previous studies on linguistic input in children with HL have focused on children with a broader range of HL than solely MHL, and were conducted at research centers using pictures or story-book reading activities (Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson, & Moeller, Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015; DesJardin et al., Reference DesJardin and Eisenberg2014). The current research adds to these two studies by looking specifically at MHL toddlers in their natural home environment during a free-play setting. The amount and quality of talk directed to children with MHL and in relation to their language abilities give insight that is relevant to supporting parents in providing an optimal linguistic environment. In addition, in contrast to work done in previous studies, the present study included an exploration of the use of complex abstract language (mental state language) as an indication of the quality of talk.

Parental linguistic input

A large body of research suggests that parental communication with children is related to children's language development (Hart & Risley, Reference Hart and Risley1995; Huttenlocher, Waterfall, Vasilyeva, Vevea, & Hedges, Reference Huttenlocher, Waterfall, Vasilyeva, Vevea and Hedges2010; Rowe, Reference Rowe2012; Weizman & Snow, Reference Weizman and Snow2001). The quantity of parental linguistic input (i.e., the number of word tokens, or the total number of words, or utterances, spoken) is an important determinant of children's vocabulary development (Huttenlocher, Haight, Bryk, Seltzer, & Lyons, Reference Huttenlocher, Haight, Bryk, Seltzer and Lyons1991; Weizman & Snow, Reference Weizman and Snow2001). Children have better language skills when their parents talk more frequently to them and expose them to a larger number of words. The more talk a child is exposed to, the more opportunities they have to become familiar with certain words, and to practice skills important for word learning (Weisleder & Fernald, Reference Weisleder and Fernald2013).

The quality of parental talk is also associated with child language development (Huttenlocher et al., Reference Huttenlocher, Waterfall, Vasilyeva, Vevea and Hedges2010). Quality of talk is reflected by linguistic characteristics such as vocabulary diversity, grammatical complexity (mean length of utterance), and the complexity of words (e.g., mental state language), but quality is also reflected in the use of conversational skills such as the use of language-evoking techniques (Ambrose et al., Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015; Golinkoff, Hoff, Rowe, Tamis-LeMonda, & Hirsh-Pasek, Reference Golinkoff, Hoff, Rowe, Tamis-LeMonda and Hirsh-Pasek2019; Rowe, Reference Rowe2012; Taumoepeau, Reference Taumoepeau2016). The mean length of utterances (MLU) reflects the degree of complexity of language and as such is considered an indication of the richness of the linguistic input (Hoff & Naigles, Reference Hoff and Naigles2002). Children who are exposed to longer utterances by their parents have better comprehension and expressive language vocabularies (Bornstein, Haynes, & Painter, Reference Bornstein, Haynes and Painter1998). The diversity of parent vocabulary input is also positively related to children's vocabulary (Hart & Risley, Reference Hart and Risley1995; Hoff & Naigles, Reference Hoff and Naigles2002; Taumoepeau, Reference Taumoepeau2016).

Complexity of parent vocabulary is also a feature of the quality of linguistic input; for example the use of mental state language (Rowe, Reference Rowe2013). This is an abstract form of language referring to cognitive terms (e.g., ‘think’, ‘know’, ‘believe’), desires (e.g., ‘want’, ‘like’, ‘hope’), and emotions (e.g., ‘happy’, ‘sad’, ‘angry’). Because mental state language does not pertain to concrete, visible objects, understanding its meaning can be particularly challenging for children. In the development of mental state language parents first talk about a child's desires (“You want milk”) and emotions (“You are happy”), and later about their knowledge or thinking (“You think there is milk in the fridge”) (Taumoepeau & Ruffman, Reference Taumoepeau and Ruffman2008). Between the age of 15 months through 33 months, there is an increase in mental state language uttered by the mother and child. Taumoepeau and Ruffman also found that maternal mental state language (particularly related to cognitive terms) was a predictor of a child's later use of mental state language.

Finally, the use of conversational skills such as language-evoking techniques reflects the quality of parental talk. The use of these techniques requires more complex responses by the child and they are positively related to child language skills (Rowe, Leech, & Cabrera, Reference Rowe, Leech and Cabrera2017). Parallel talk (talking about what a child is doing, seeing, or touching), expansion (restating and completing a child's utterance with correct grammar), recasting (changing a child's utterance into a question), and asking open questions (in and outside the immediate context, and sincere request) are examples of so-called high-level facilitative language techniques that are positively associated with receptive and expressive language skills (Cruz, Quittner, Marker, DesJardin, & Team, Reference Cruz, Quittner, Marker, DesJardin and Team2013; Girolametto, Weitzman, Wiigs, & Pearce, Reference Girolametto, Weitzman, Wiigs and Pearce1999). Examples of low-level facilitative language techniques are imitation (repeating a child's utterance), labeling (stating the name of an object or picture), linguistic mapping (putting into words what a child may be trying to communicate), and directive language (Ambrose et al., Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015; DesJardin et al., Reference DesJardin, Doll, Stika, Eisenberg, Johnson, Ganguly and Cunning2014; Rowe, Reference Rowe2008).

Research suggests that low-level facilitative language techniques promote language development in young children at the prelinguistic stage (Girolametto et al., Reference Girolametto, Weitzman, Wiigs and Pearce1999; Yoder & Warren, Reference Yoder and Warren2001), whereas high-level facilitative techniques enhance this development in older children who use more complex language structures (Rowe, Reference Rowe2012).

This implies that different levels of linguistic input matter differentially across child language development. Rowe (Reference Rowe2012) validated this perspective in a longitudinal study on vocabulary growth in children aged between 18 and 42 months. The total number of words that parents used was most important during the second year of life, whereas vocabulary diversity was more important in the third year, and the use of decontextualized language was most beneficial in the fourth year of life. Aside from the differential effect of quantity and quality input at different ages, these two measures are also inter-related (e.g., parents who talk more also produce more diverse talk; Hoff & Naigles, Reference Hoff and Naigles2002).

Parental linguistic input to children with MHL

Children with MHL are more at risk for language difficulties than children with normal hearing (NH) (Moeller et al., Reference Moeller, Tomblin, Yoshinaga-Itano, Connor and Jerger2007; Tomblin et al., Reference Tomblin, Harrison, Ambrose, Walker, Oleson and Moeller2015). Despite their use of hearing aids, most children with MHL have inconsistent access to speech, which may impact their language development (Bagatto, Moodie, Seewald, Bartlett, & Scollie, Reference Bagatto, Moodie, Seewald, Bartlett and Scollie2011; McCreery et al., Reference McCreery, Walker, Spratford, Bentler, Holte, Roush, Oleson, van Buren and Moeller2015; Stiles, Bentler, & McGregor, Reference Stiles, Bentler and McGregor2012). Early intervention programs for children with HL are often focused on optimizing children's language development; these programs emphasize the potential role that parents can play (Moeller, Carr, Seaver, Stredler-Brown, & Holzinger, Reference Moeller, Carr, Seaver, Stredler-Brown and Holzinger2013). The assumption is that rich conversations, consisting of a diverse vocabulary and of many opportunities for the child to engage, will boost the language abilities of children with MHL. It is therefore important to identify strategies that promote high-quality language use by parents in their interactions with children with MHL.

A limited number of studies have been published on parental linguistic input in children with HL. Most of these studies examined children with a range of hearing loss (20–90 dB HL) (Ambrose, VanDam, & Moeller, Reference Ambrose, VanDam and Moeller2014; Ambrose et al., Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015; DesJardin et al., Reference DesJardin, Doll, Stika, Eisenberg, Johnson, Ganguly and Cunning2014; VanDam, Ambrose, & Moeller, Reference VanDam, Ambrose and Moeller2012). The current study focused specifically on children with MHL (40–60 dB HL). Outcomes of the previous studies showed that children (two years of age and younger) with mild to severe HL were exposed to a similar amount of parental talk (number of words) than children with NH (Ambrose et al., Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015; VanDam et al., Reference VanDam, Ambrose and Moeller2012). These findings were in line with those of Nittrouer (Reference Nittrouer2009) and Aragon and Yoshinaga-Itano (Reference Aragon and Yoshinaga-Itano2012), who studied groups of children with a range of HL (both hard-of-hearing and deaf). Ambrose et al. (Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015), however, reported differences between three-year-olds with mild to severe HL in terms of the quantity of parental linguistic input. Three-year-old children with MHL in their study were exposed to fewer words than their peers with NH. Furthermore, they also found that these children were exposed to a limited variety of words and shorter utterances (quality of language). Ambrose et al. noted that parents of children with HL may have adapted their own language levels as a result of their sensitivity to the lower language abilities of their children. While language differences between young children with HL and young children with NH are less obvious, they become more apparent when children grow older, and consequently parents may adapt their language levels accordingly. This reasoning may explain why differences in the number of words, the variety of words, and length of utterance were found between three-year-old children with HL and NH but not in two-year-olds.

The quality of linguistic input is also reflected by parents’ use of facilitative language techniques. Ambrose et al. (Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015) and Desjardin et al. (Reference DesJardin, Doll, Stika, Eisenberg, Johnson, Ganguly and Cunning2014) examined the use of these techniques during parent–child interactions in children with mild to severe HL. Parents’ use of high-level facilitative language techniques such as recasting, asking open-ended questions, and expansion was positively related to children's oral language abilities. These findings are in line with research on children with cochlear implants (Cruz et al., Reference Cruz, Quittner, Marker, DesJardin and Team2013; DesJardin & Eisenberg, Reference DesJardin and Eisenberg2007) and on children with moderate to profound HL (Nittrouer, Reference Nittrouer2009). While parents’ use of high-level language techniques is thus important in promoting the language abilities of children with mild to severe HL, parents of these children used high-level language techniques less frequently than parents of children with NH (Ambrose et al., Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015).

Ambrose and colleagues (2015), however, also reported that parents of children with mild to severe HL used more directing language than parents of children with NH. This low-level language technique was negatively related to children's language abilities. Directing language is used to direct a child's attention or behavior (e.g., “Say Mummy”; “Look here”; “Don't touch that”). Not all low-level language techniques are (negatively) related to language abilities in children with mild to severe HL. Desjardin and colleagues (2014), for example, found that a composite score of low-level techniques (e.g., labeling, linguistic mapping, commenting, directing, asking closed-ended questions, and imitating) was not related to language abilities of children with mild to severe HL. The use of a composite score of low-level language techniques and the larger age range of the children included in the study may have resulted in different outcomes than those reported by Ambrose and colleagues (2015). Parents’ talk to children with mild to severe HL did seem to change over time: parents used less directive language and provided their children with more high-level language when their children were older (Ambrose et al., Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015).

Parental use of mental state language is another feature of the quality of the linguistic input. The exposure to this complex abstract language is not only beneficial for children's language development, but also for their social-emotional development. This is especially of interest in children with MHL since they encounter difficulties in their social-emotional development (Dirks et al., Reference Dirks, Ketelaar, van der Zee, Netten, Frijns and Rieffe2017; Laugen, Jacobsen, Rieffe, & Wichstrom, Reference Laugen, Jacobsen, Rieffe and Wichstrom2016, Reference Laugen, Jacobsen, Rieffe and Wichstrom2017; Netten et al., Reference Netten, Rieffe, Theunissen, Soede, Dirks, Korver and Frijns2015, Reference Netten, Rieffe, Soede, Dirks, Korver, Konings and Frijns2017). Talking with children about their own and others’ thoughts, desires, and feelings promotes their social-emotional development (Devine & Hughes, Reference Devine and Hughes2018; Drummond, Paul, Waugh, Hammond, & Brownell, Reference Drummond, Paul, Waugh, Hammond and Brownell2014; Dunn, Brown, Slomkowski, Tesla, & Youngblade, Reference Dunn, Brown, Slomkowski, Tesla and Youngblade1991; Meins, Reference Meins2013; Peterson & Slaughter, Reference Peterson and Slaughter2003).

Until now, two studies have examined parents’ use of mental state language in interactions with children with HL (Moeller & Schick, Reference Moeller and Schick2006; Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Meristo, Mann, Hjelmquist, Surian and Siegal2014). In their study of deaf five-year-old children, Moeller and Schick (Reference Moeller and Schick2006) coded signed references to mental states during mother–child interactions. Verbal expressions were coded for the hearing controls. Although there were no differences in the total number of utterances, mothers of deaf children made less frequent and less varied references to mental states than mothers of children with NH. The study also reported that children with mothers who made more mental state references showed a better false belief understanding. Reduced access to mental state language was also reported in a younger group of children with HL (Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Meristo, Mann, Hjelmquist, Surian and Siegal2014). Mothers of deaf children included in this study referred less often to mental states during spoken conversations about pictures showing social situations than mothers of children with NH. As far as we know, the use of mental state language by parents and children has not yet been examined in children with MHL.

Early intervention

Several studies emphasized the importance of early detection of hearing loss and an early start of intervention by showing better language outcomes in children with HL enrolled early in intervention (Meinzen-Derr, Wiley, & Choo, Reference Meinzen-Derr, Wiley and Choo2011; Moeller, Reference Moeller2000; Yoshinaga-Itano, Sedey, Coulter, & Mehl, Reference Yoshinaga-Itano, Sedey, Coulter and Mehl1998). For example, for infants with congenital hearing loss, early hearing aid fitting (before six months of life) has been shown to be related to better speech and language outcomes compared to later fitting (Ambrose et al., Reference Ambrose, VanDam and Moeller2014; Tomblin et al., Reference Tomblin, Harrison, Ambrose, Walker, Oleson and Moeller2015). Hearing aids provide children with better access to spoken language and thus better access to parental talk. Additionally, early enrollment in family-centered intervention programs in which parents are guided in promoting their child's language development result in better language outcomes than later enrollment (Ching et al., Reference Ching, Dillon, Button, Seeto, Van Buynder, Marnane and Leigh2017; Holzinger, Fellinger, & Beitel, Reference Holzinger, Fellinger and Beitel2011; Meinzen-Derr et al., Reference Meinzen-Derr, Wiley and Choo2011; Moeller, Reference Moeller2000; Yoshinaga-Itano et al., Reference Yoshinaga-Itano, Sedey, Coulter and Mehl1998).

Present study

Recently, studies on young children with mild to severe HL have received increasing attention in the literature (e.g., Koehlinger, Van Horne, & Moeller, Reference Koehlinger, Van Horne and Moeller2013; Laugen et al., Reference Laugen, Jacobsen, Rieffe and Wichstrom2016; Moeller & Tomblin, Reference Moeller and Tomblin2015; Netten et al., Reference Netten, Rieffe, Soede, Dirks, Korver, Konings and Frijns2017; Stika et al., Reference Stika, Eisenberg, Johnson, Henning, Colson, Ganguly and DesJardin2015). These studies indicate that even children with less severe HL are at risk for language delays (Koehlinger et al., Reference Koehlinger, Van Horne and Moeller2013; Netten et al., Reference Netten, Rieffe, Theunissen, Soede, Dirks, Korver and Frijns2015; Tomblin et al., Reference Tomblin, Harrison, Ambrose, Walker, Oleson and Moeller2015). It is therefore essential that further research is conducted in order to gain insights into how the language development of children with less severe HL (MHL) can be facilitated. Parents play a crucial role in enhancing children's language development. The focus of the current study is on the quality and quantity of parental linguistic input in relation to the language abilities of 30-month-old toddlers with MHL.

In order to examine the quality of parental linguistic input, a number of factors were considered, including the diversity of parent vocabulary and the mean length of utterances parents employed, as well as parents’ use of mental state language and use of language-evoking techniques. Quantity of linguistic input was reflected by the number of utterances and words used by parents. Although there are some relevant differences between the current study and the one of Ambrose and colleagues (2015), such as our specific focus on children with MHL, the free-play setting in the home environment (current study) instead of using pictures at the research center, and our additional focus on complex language use (mental state language), it was of interest to use their coding system to examine the use of language-evoking techniques.

In line with the findings of Ambrose et al. (Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015), we expected that parents of toddlers with MHL would use more low-level language and expose their children to less high-level language than parents of children with NH. Further, we expected the parents to use less mental state language during the interactions (Moeller & Schick, Reference Moeller and Schick2006; Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Meristo, Mann, Hjelmquist, Surian and Siegal2014). Positive relations between language ability and high-level language use were expected, in addition to negative relations between language ability and low-level language.

We expected that parents of children with MHL would use fewer words during parent–child interactions than parents of children with NH (Ambrose et al., Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015). Furthermore, positive relations were expected between the quantity of parental linguistic input and language abilities (Ambrose et al., Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015). In addition to the relation between linguistic input and language abilities, the relation between hearing loss-related variables (e.g., degree of HL and start intervention) and linguistic input was examined.

Method

Participants

This study is part of a larger study on the psychosocial functioning of toddlers with MHL and their families (Dirks & Rieffe, Reference Dirks and Rieffe2019; Dirks, Uilenburg, & Rieffe, Reference Dirks, Uilenburg and Rieffe2016; Dirks et al., Reference Dirks, Ketelaar, van der Zee, Netten, Frijns and Rieffe2017). In total, 42 children between 29 and 33 months of age participated in this study. The 18 children with MHL were recruited from two family-centered early intervention centers in the Netherlands. The control group of 24 children with NH was recruited via a well-baby clinic. The children with NH were included in the study if they had passed the neonatal hearing screening and had no known medical or developmental disabilities. Children with MHL were included in the study if they were diagnosed with congenital moderate hearing losses (40–60 dB HL) in the better ear (residual hearing was calculated by averaging unaided hearing thresholds at 500, 1,000, and 2,000 Hz) and they had no other medical or developmental disability such as mental retardation, visual impairment, or speech-motor problems. Characteristics of the samples are reported in Table 1. Age, gender and maternal education level did not differ between the groups.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the MHL and NH groups

Notes. Abbreviations: MHL Moderate Hearing Loss; NH Normal Hearing; SD Standard Deviation; NA Not Available; FCEI, Family-Centered Early Intervention; HA Hearing Aid. *1 (1 = no/primary education, 2 = lower general secondary education, 3 = higher general education, 4 = college/university).

The hearing children were born to parents with NH. Of the sample of children with MHL, four fathers and one mother had MHL and one father was deaf. None of the children had more than one parent with hearing loss. At home the children used spoken language in the interactions with their parents (6 parents and children supported their spoken language often with signs, 1 always, 9 sometimes, and 2 never). All children with MHL wore hearing aids and received care from an audiologist. A total of 87% of the children began wearing hearing aids within the first six months of life. Furthermore, all children with MHL participated in a family-centered early intervention program for children with HL, and 78% of the children started in the program within their first six months of life. The family-centered early intervention program included frequent house visits by early interventionists and speech-language therapists. During these house visits, professionals provided support to parents and offered strategies of engagement to promote their child's development. For example, the professionals gave information about the use of hearing aids and the beneficial effects of consistent auditory access on children's language development. Professionals also guided parents in obtaining and directing their child's attention to achieve joint attention, and they discussed language facilitating strategies. In addition to these house visits, parents attended several courses (e.g., sign courses, communication courses, and interactive reading courses) at the center together with other parents. The children attended specialized treatment groups for toddlers with HL

Procedure

Members of the research team visited the families at home. The children and their parents engaged in a 10-minute free-play session with standardized toys. The toys were age appropriate and included building blocks (that could be used to build and as a puzzle), animal figures, and a tea set. Parents were asked to play with their child the way they usually did. All interactions were videotaped. The majority of parent–child interaction videos (93%) included the mother, and the remaining interactions included the father (MHL = 2 and NH = 1). In the majority of videotaped interactions, parents with NH interacted with their child; one parent–child interaction included a mother with MHL.

Parents were asked to fill in a questionnaire about their family's background. Additional information, such as degree of hearing loss and age at amplification was obtained from medical records. Speech and language therapists assessed the language ability of the children. The study was carried out in accordance with the standards set by the Declaration of Helsinki and informed consent was obtained for all children.

Measures

Language ability

Linguists assessed the children's receptive and expressive language abilities via two language tests that have been developed and standardized for children between two and five years of age. These tests are widely used for children with and without HL within the Netherlands. Receptive language ability was assessed with the Reynell Developmental Language Scales – Dutch Version (Schaerlaekens & Van Ommeslaeghe, Reference Schaerlaekens A and Van Ommeslaeghe1993). Expressive language ability was assessed with the Sentence Development Scale of the Schlichting Expressive Language Test (Schlichting, Eldik, Lutje Spelberg, Van der Meulen, & Van der Meulen, Reference Schlichting, Eldik, Lutje Spelberg, Van der Meulen and Van der Meulen1995). Raw scores are converted to age equivalents and language quotients. The quotient scores are normally distributed scores, with a mean score of 100 and a standard deviation of 15.

Parental linguistic input

Three research assistants transcribed video-recordings of the parent–child interactions. They followed conventions that allow for coding and transcribing speech using the Codes for the Human Analysis of Transcripts (CHAT). The Computerized Language Analysis (CLAN) software (MacWhinney, Reference MacWhinney2000) was used to analyze the transcriptions in CHAT format. CLAN was used to calculate the number of total utterances (NTU) and number of total words (NTW) for the quantity of talk, and the number of different words (NDW) and mean length of utterances (MLU) for the quality of parent talk. Because the parent–child interactions were between 9.5 and 12 minutes in length (mean: 10.55 min; SD: 0.39) the counts were divided by the number of minutes in the sample and then multiplied by ten to normalize all count variables to 10 minutes. Ten percent of the videos were transcribed independently from each other to calculate inter-rater reliability. The percent agreement of transcribed utterances ranged from 81 to 98% (mean: 90%).

Using a coding procedure developed by Ambrose et al. (Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015), the quality of parent utterances were coded as serving one of ten mutually exclusive functions: basic acknowledgments, clarification questions, informative statements, informative questions, simple social phrases, test questions, directing utterances, conversational-eliciting utterances that were open ended, conversational-eliciting utterances referencing topics outside the immediate context, and real utterances. Ambrose et al. used the latter four types of utterances (directing, two conversational-eliciting types, and real utterances) in their paper because previous literature had indicated that these utterances may specifically enhance or hinder language development (Cruz et al., Reference Cruz, Quittner, Marker, DesJardin and Team2013; DesJardin & Eisenberg, Reference DesJardin and Eisenberg2007; Hoff-Ginsberg, Reference Hoff-Ginsberg1985; Taumoepeau & Ruffman, Reference Taumoepeau and Ruffman2006; Zimmerman et al., Reference Zimmerman, Gilkerson, Richards, Christakis, Xu, Gray and Yapanel2009). In line with Ambrose et al. (Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015) we used these four types of utterances in the current study. Directing utterances are considered lower-level functions which are used to direct a child's attention and/or to tell a child to do something (e.g., “look”, “don't touch”, or “bring me that cup”) (Ambrose et al., Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015; Cruz et al., Reference Cruz, Quittner, Marker, DesJardin and Team2013; DesJardin & Eisenberg, Reference DesJardin and Eisenberg2007). Conversational-eliciting utterances (open or outside) and real utterances are considered high-level functions. Conversational-eliciting utterances were divided into open utterances and outside utterances. Open utterances invite the child to talk; for example “What kinds of animals do you see?”, “Who is in this picture?”. Outside utterances require the child to think outside the immediate context to respond (e.g., Ambrose et al., Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015; Cruz et al., Reference Cruz, Quittner, Marker, DesJardin and Team2013; DesJardin & Eisenberg, Reference DesJardin and Eisenberg2007). Examples of outside utterances are “tell me about the animals at grandparents’ home”, “Who has a boat?”, “Do you have a toy like this at home?”. Real utterances are sincere requests for information to which the parent does not necessarily know the answer; for example “What color is your favorite toy?” or “Why do you think the dog is hungry?”. These three types of utterances were added together to calculate the number of high-level utterances. Then, the proportion of high-level utterances and directing utterances were calculated.

Two research assistants (linguists) who also transcribed the video-recordings used the 10-level mutual exclusive coding system to code parents’ utterances. Intra-class coefficients (ICC) were calculated separately for each variable to assess the absolute agreement between the independent raters. ICC was used because the data was measured on a continuous scale (Mandrekar, Reference Mandrekar2011). The research assistants coded 20% of the sample independently. The intra-class reliability coefficients ranged from .81 to .95 (mean .87).

Mental state language

The normalized 10-minute parent–child interaction transcriptions were also used to code the number of genuine mental state terms used by both parents and children during interaction. Mental state terms included references to cognitive terms (e.g., ‘think’, ‘know’, ‘remember’, or ‘believe’), desires (e.g., ‘want’, ‘like’, ‘don't like’, ‘hope’, or ‘wish’), and emotions (e.g., ‘happy’, ‘sad’, ‘angry’, or ‘worried’) (Ensor & Hughes, Reference Ensor and Hughes2008; Moeller & Schick, Reference Moeller and Schick2006; Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Meristo, Mann, Hjelmquist, Surian and Siegal2014). In the analyses we combined the three categories of mental state terms into one category for two reasons: parents rarely mentioned emotion words, and there were no differences within either group between the number of utterances for desires or beliefs. Because the parent–child interactions were between 9.5 and 12 minutes in length (mean: 10.55 min; SD: 0.39), the counts were divided by the number of minutes in the sample and then multiplied by ten to normalize all count variables to 10 minutes. All of the videos were coded independently from each other by two members of the research team to calculate inter-rater reliability. The intra-class reliability coefficient was r = .97.

Statistical analyses

Because of the small sample size and the fact that not all variables met the assumptions of parametric testing, non-parametric tests were used. Group demographics were compared using Mann–Whitney tests. These tests were also used to test for differences between groups in language ability and parental linguistic input. Effect size was estimated with Cohen's d. Correlations between the measures were calculated with Spearman's rho correlations. The Bonferroni method was used to control for Type 1 error at the .05 level across comparisons and correlations.

Results

Between-group differences

Table 2 shows the summary statistics and between-group differences in child language abilities and parental linguistic input. Children with MHL had lower receptive and expressive language scores and used less mental state language than the children with NH. No significant differences were found for the quantity of parental linguistic input to children with MHL and NH. Parents of children with MHL used a similar number of words and utterances during the interactions compared to parents of children with NH. Differences between groups were found for quality measures of language input. Children with MHL were exposed to shorter utterances, less high-level language, and less mental state language. No significant differences were found in the exposure to low-level language (directing utterances) and number of different words.

Table 2. Summary statistics of child language and parental linguistic input

Notes. Abbreviations: MHL Moderate Hearing Loss; NH Normal Hearing; SD Standard Deviation; NTU Number of Total Utterances; NTW Number of Total Words; NDW Number of Different Words; MLU Mean Length of Utterance. In bold are the significant differences between groups after Bonferroni correction. a,b proportion of parental utterances that were high/low; c number of mental state references.

Associations between linguistic input and hearing loss related variables

The associations between parental linguistic input and hearing loss related variables are shown in Table 3. No significant associations were found between these variables with the exception of the degree of HL and high-level language. The degree of HL was negatively related to high-level language input; children with more decibels HL were exposed to less high-level language.

Table 3. Spearman's rho correlations for HL-related variables and parental linguistic input variables and child language scores for MHL children (n = 18)

Notes. Abbreviations: HA Hearing Aid; FCEI Family-Centered Early Intervention; *p < .005; a,b proportion of parental utterances that were high/low; c number of mental state references.

Associations between parental linguistic input and child language abilities

Table 4 shows the associations between parental linguistic input and children's language abilities by group. First, no associations were found between children's receptive language and parental talk. Second, for children's expressive language abilities, positive associations with parental talk were found but only in the MHL group. Parents who used more words, longer utterances, and more mental state language had children with better expressive language abilities. Third, parents’ use of mental state language was positively related to children's use of mental state language, but only in the MHL group.

Table 4. Spearman's rho correlations for parental linguistic input variables and child language scores

Notes. Abbreviations: NTU Number of Total Utterances; NTW Number of Total Words; NDW Number of Different Words; MLU Mean Length of Utterance. a,b proportion of parental utterances that were high/low; c number of mental state references; *p < .007.

Associations between quantity and quality measures of parental linguistic input

Table 5 shows the associations between the quantity and quality of parental linguistic input measures by group (with the MHL group above the diagonal gap and the NH group below the diagonal). Quantity of linguistic input in terms of total numbers of words was positively related to all quality measures in the MHL group except to high- and low-level language techniques. In the NH group no significant correlations between quantity and quality measures were found.

Table 5. Spearman's rho correlations for parental linguistic measures by group, with the MHL group above the diagonal gap and the NH group below the diagonal

Notes. Abbreviations: NTU Number of Total Utterances; NTW Number of Total Words; NDW Number of Different Words; MLU Mean Length of Utterance. a,b proportion of parental utterances that were high/low; c number of mental state references; *p < .002.

Discussion

Having a MHL puts children at risk for language difficulties (Tomblin et al., Reference Tomblin, Harrison, Ambrose, Walker, Oleson and Moeller2015) and therefore it is important to optimize their language environment. Parents play a crucial role in promoting young children's language abilities (Hart & Risley, Reference Hart and Risley1995). This study examined the quantity and quality of parental linguistic input to toddlers with MHL and toddlers with NH in relation to their language abilities. The outcomes revealed that parents of toddlers with MHL were as talkative to their children as parents of children with NH. However, the quality of their linguistic input differed from that of parents of children with NH. Parents of toddlers with MHL used less high-level language, fewer mental states references, and shorter utterances than parents of toddlers with NH. The exposure to low-level language (directing utterances) did not differ between toddlers with MHL and NH.

Quantity and some quality measures of parents’ linguistic input were positively related to the expressive language abilities of children with MHL and to each other. Only the quality measures reflected by linguistic characteristics were related to the quantity of talk, as parents of children with MHL who used more different words, longer utterances, and more mental state language also used more total words. The quality measures reflected by parents’ conversational skills (e.g., the use of language-evoking techniques) were not related to the quantity of parental talk. The fact that significant associations between quality and quantity of talk were only found in the MHL group and not in the NH group might be due to a lack of power because of the small sample size. The correlations in the NH group tended to be significant. On the other hand, it may be that parents of children with MHL repeat their utterances more because they are not sure their child heard them well enough. In that case a difference between groups in the number of diverse words would be expected. Again, this difference between groups tended to be significant. To conclude, a larger sample size is needed to find out if the associations between quantity and quality parental talk are indeed differential between groups.

Children with MHL were exposed to similar numbers of (diverse) words and utterances compared to children with NH. These findings are in line with Ambrose et al. (Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015), who reported no differences between 18- and/or 36-month-old children with mild to severe HL and NH in the number of exposed utterances. However, Ambrose and colleagues reported that three-year-olds with HL in their study were exposed to fewer (diverse) words in the interactions with their parents. This difference was not found in the 18-month-olds in the Ambrose et al. study, or in the 30-month-olds in the present study. Parents of children with NH might increase their vocabulary during parent–child interactions at an earlier time in their child's life than parents of children with MHL. This may explain why differences between groups were found in three-year-olds but not in the younger children. In addition, the language difficulties of children with HL become more prominent with age, and parents may adapt their language input accordingly.

Unlike parents in the studies by Ambrose et al. (Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015) and Desjardin et al. (Reference DesJardin, Doll, Stika, Eisenberg, Johnson, Ganguly and Cunning2014), parents of children with and without MHL in our study used a similar amount of low-level language (directing utterances). However, Ambrose et al. (Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015) and DesJardin et al. (Reference DesJardin, Doll, Stika, Eisenberg, Johnson, Ganguly and Cunning2014) coded the parent–child communication during a structured art gallery task (Ambrose et al., Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015) or while reading a picture book (Desjardin et al., Reference DesJardin, Doll, Stika, Eisenberg, Johnson, Ganguly and Cunning2014). Both tasks may have elicited more directive behavior than the free-play activity that was used in the present study. Shifting the child's attention between the object of conversation (a picture or a book) and themselves in the other studies might have required parents to use more directive language. Another explanation for the inconsistency in the above findings may be related to differences between the studies in the degree of HL in the children studied. In the present study, only children with a HL between 40 and 60 dB were included (moderate hearing loss), while Ambrose et al. (Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015) and Desjardin et al. (Reference DesJardin, Doll, Stika, Eisenberg, Johnson, Ganguly and Cunning2014) included children between 20 and 90 dB HL.

Quality differences in parental talk were reflected by parents of toddlers with MHL using shorter utterances and less high-level language. These results were in line with the earlier study of three-year-old children with mild to severe HL (Ambrose et al., Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015). Another feature of the quality of linguistic input is the use of mental state language, an abstract form of language. In line with the findings of Moeller and Schick (Reference Moeller and Schick2006) and Morgan et al. (Reference Morgan, Meristo, Mann, Hjelmquist, Surian and Siegal2014), the results of the current study showed that parents of children with MHL used less mental state language than parents of children with NH. This is one of the first studies that also examined children's use of mental state language in a group of children with HL. Children with MHL used fewer mental state references than did children with NH, although neither group used many mental state terms during their interactions. Past research indicated an increase in normal hearing children's use of mental state language between one and four years of age (Taumoepeau & Ruffman, Reference Taumoepeau and Ruffman2008). Since the children in our sample were 30 months of age, an increase in their use of this language was expected.

In line with findings documented by Ruffman, Slade, and Crowe (Reference Ruffman, Slade and Crowe2002), parental use of mental state language was positively related to the MHL children's use of it themselves. The findings concerning mental state language are also of interest because of the association with social-emotional development (Devine & Hughes, Reference Devine and Hughes2018; Drummond et al., Reference Drummond, Paul, Waugh, Hammond and Brownell2014; Dunn et al., Reference Dunn, Brown, Slomkowski, Tesla and Youngblade1991; Meins, Reference Meins2013; Peterson & Slaughter, Reference Peterson and Slaughter2003). Since children with MHL more often encounter social-emotional difficulties (Dirks et al., Reference Dirks, Ketelaar, van der Zee, Netten, Frijns and Rieffe2017; Laugen et al., Reference Laugen, Jacobsen, Rieffe and Wichstrom2016, Reference Laugen, Jacobsen, Rieffe and Wichstrom2017; Netten et al., Reference Netten, Rieffe, Theunissen, Soede, Dirks, Korver and Frijns2015, 2017), early interventionists should guide parents in talking about beliefs, knowledge, desires, and emotions while interacting with their chilren.

In line with our expectations, and consistent with the results of other studies (Ambrose et al., Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015: Cruz et al., Reference Cruz, Quittner, Marker, DesJardin and Team2013; Desjardin et al., Reference DesJardin, Doll, Stika, Eisenberg, Johnson, Ganguly and Cunning2014; Nittrouer, Reference Nittrouer2009), children with MHL who had better expressive language abilities had parents who used more words and longer utterances. Interestingly, this association was only found for the children with MHL. This might indicate that, because of their inconsistent access to speech and sounds, parental input might be missed and more exposure to words be needed for children with MHL to develop their expressive language skills to their full potential.

Contrary to our expectations, no associations between high-level language exposure by the parents and children's language abilities were found. The 30-month-old children in the present sample might be too young to benefit from this quality feature of parental talk. Yet, as children grow and their language abilities increase, they may be more capable of taking advantage of this high-level input. In the study of Ambrose et al. (Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015), three-year-olds showed a beneficial effect of high-level language input on their language abilities while the 18-month-olds did not.

The reported negative association between the degree of HL and high-level language input by Ambrose et al. (Reference Ambrose, Walker, Unflat-Berry, Oleson and Moeller2015) was also found in this study, despite the smaller dB range in our study: children with more severe HL were exposed to less high-level language than those with less severe HL. It is unclear what the impact of this limited exposure to high-level language is on the future language development of children with MHL. One possibility is that it may impede further language development: parents of children with MHL may underestimate their capacities and therefore provide insufficient stimulation for children to attain the next level of development. Another possibility is that parents may appropriately modify their language use to fit the poorer language abilities of children with MHL, relative to children with NH. Parents of children with MHL may in fact be highly sensitive to their children's abilities and adapt their linguistic input accordingly (Dirks & Rieffe, Reference Dirks and Rieffe2019). From a social constructivist perspective, language learning takes place in the ‘zone of proximal development’ (Vygotsky, Reference Vygotsky1978): parental linguistic input should be sufficiently challenging for a child to learn new words, neither too simple nor too difficult. This requires parents to be sensitive enough to acknowledge a child's changing language abilities and to provide them with more complex input when appropriate. Longitudinal research is needed to examine whether parents of children with MHL are indeed that sensitive that they adapt their talk to their children's needs.

The current findings have several implications for family-centered early intervention programs for children with MHL and their families. The results suggest that parent–child interactions are related to the language development of children, and that the language abilities of children with MHL are lower than those of their hearing peers. Several implications for practice can be drawn from these findings. First, it is important to carefully monitor a child's language development so that their current level of language abilities can be determined. Next, observations of parent–child interactions are needed to gain insights into the current linguistic input by parents. Based on this information, the appropriate level of parental linguistic input that is needed at that moment to promote children's language development can be determined. Early interventionists may coach parents in providing this linguistic input during daily activities by modeling and video-feedback techniques. Interactive story-book reading may be one of the activities that could be used to elicit rich parent–child conversations to promote children's language and social-emotional development (Dirks & Wauters, Reference Dirks, Wauters, Knoors and Marschark2015).

Interventions in which story-books are used to promote mental state language are of interest because story-book reading enhances language development in general in children with HL (DesJardin et al., Reference DesJardin, Doll, Stika, Eisenberg, Johnson, Ganguly and Cunning2014; Fung, Chow, & McBride-Chang, Reference Fung, Chow and McBride-Chang2005). Research on reading story-books to promote mental state language is mostly focused on hearing children (Adrian, Clemente, Villanueva, & Rieffe, Reference Adrian, Clemente, Villanueva and Rieffe2005; Aram, Fine, & Ziv, Reference Aram, Fine and Ziv2013; Taumoepeau & Reese, Reference Taumoepeau and Reese2013). Aram et al. (Reference Aram, Fine and Ziv2013) examined the effect of an intervention to promote parents’ use of mental state language during story-book reading with hearing children. After the intervention, parents and children referred more often to mental state terms than parents and children who did not follow the intervention. Story-book reading could be useful in exposing MHL children to high-level language and mental state language; however, parents do not do this naturally and we need to support them (Dirks & Wauters, Reference Dirks and Wauters2018).

One limitation of this study is the cross-sectional design, due to which the causality of relationships between linguistic input and children's language abilities cannot be specified. Future studies could longitudinally examine the linguistic input to younger hearing brothers or sisters of children with MHL and compare them with the input to children with MHL at that age. Future studies could also include hearing children with the same language levels as those of children with MHL to compare their linguistic input.

Another limitation is the relatively small sample size, which prohibits the establishment of firm conclusions at this time. A larger sample size would enable researchers to examine which aspects of parental talk are most important to predict a child's language. It is recommended that future research incorporate larger samples to better address these questions. This was one of the first studies that examined parental and child mental state language in the interactions of young children with MHL. Future studies should investigate the relationship between mental state language and social-emotional development in this group of children.

In this study we examined the linguistic interactions of toddlers with MHL and their parents in their home environment. When children grow up, they also spend time with peers in daycare or playgroups. Given that early interactions between peers are important for children's development, future studies could examine the (linguistic) interactions of children with MHL and their peers.

Conclusions

In this first study on linguistic input to toddlers with MHL it was found that the amount and quality of parental talk was related to the expressive language abilities of these children. The quantity of parents talk to children with MHL is similar to that of parents of children with NH. The input is, however, of a lower quality, with parents using less high-level language, less mental state language, and shorter utterances. A question that requires further examination is whether these parents actually appropriately adapt their language use to their child's current capacities, or whether they could further challenge their child with MHL by using more high-level language. Early interventionists should carefully monitor children's language abilities and their exposure to (parental) linguistic input in order to optimize and promote their language development.

References

Adrian, J. E., Clemente, R. A., Villanueva, L., & Rieffe, C. (2005). Parent–child picturebook reading, mothers’ mental state language and children's theory of mind. Journal of Child Language, 32, 673–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ambrose, S. E., VanDam, M., & Moeller, M. P. (2014). Linguistic input, electronic media, and communication outcomes of toddlers with hearing loss. Ear and Hearing, 35(2), 139–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ambrose, S. E., Walker, E. A., Unflat-Berry, L. M., Oleson, J. J., & Moeller, M. P. (2015). Quantity and quality of caregivers’ linguistic input to 18-month and 3-year-old children who are hard of hearing. Ear and Hearing, 36, 48S59S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aragon, M., & Yoshinaga-Itano, C. (2012). Using Language ENvironment Analysis to improve outcomes for children who are deaf or hard of hearing. Seminars in Speech and Language, 33, 340–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aram, D., Fine, Y., & Ziv, M. (2013). Enhancing parent–child shared book reading interactions: promoting references to the book's plot and socio-cognitive themes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(1), 111–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bagatto, M. P., Moodie, S. T., Seewald, R. C., Bartlett, D. J., & Scollie, S. D. (2011). A critical review of audiological outcome measures for infants and children. Trends in Amplification, 15(1/2), 2333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, M. O., & Painter, K. M. (1998). Sources of child vocabulary competence: a multivariate model. Journal of Child Language, 25, 367–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ching, T. Y., Dillon, H., Button, L., Seeto, M., Van Buynder, P., Marnane, V., … & Leigh, G. (2017). Age at intervention for permanent hearing loss and 5-year language outcomes. Pediatrics, 140(3), e20164274CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cruz, I., Quittner, A. L., Marker, C., DesJardin, J. L., & Team, C. D. I. (2013). Identification of effective strategies to promote language in deaf children with cochlear implants. Child Development, 84(2), 543–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DesJardin, J. L., Doll, E. R., Stika, C. J., Eisenberg, L. S., Johnson, K. J., Ganguly, D. H., … & Cunning, S. C. (2014). Parental support for language development during joint book reading for young children with hearing loss. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 35(3), 167–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DesJardin, J. L., & Eisenberg, L. S. (2007). Maternal contributions: supporting language development in young children with cochlear implants. Ear and Hearing, 28(4), 456–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2018). Family correlates of false belief understanding in early childhood: a meta-analysis. Child Development, 89(3), 971–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dirks, E., Ketelaar, L., van der Zee, R., Netten, A. P., Frijns, J. H. M., & Rieffe, C. (2017). Concern for others: a study on empathy in toddlers with moderate hearing loss. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 22(2), 178–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dirks, E., & Rieffe, C. (2019). Are you there for me? Joint engagement and emotional availability in parent–child interactions for toddlers with moderate hearing loss. Ear and Hearing, 40(1), 1826.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dirks, E., Uilenburg, N., & Rieffe, C. (2016). Parental stress among parents of toddlers with moderate hearing loss. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 55, 2736.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dirks, E., & Wauters, L. (2015). Enhancing emergent literacy in preschool deaf and hard-of-hearing children through interactive reading. In Knoors, H. & Marschark, M. (Eds.), Educating deaf learners: creating a global evidence base (pp. 415–41). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dirks, E., & Wauters, L. (2018). It takes two to read: interactive reading with young deaf and hard-of-hearing children. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 23(3), 261–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drummond, J., Paul, E. F., Waugh, W. E., Hammond, S. I., & Brownell, C. A. (2014). Here, there and everywhere: emotion and mental state talk in different social contexts predicts empathic helping in toddlers. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, e00361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C., & Youngblade, L. (1991). Young children's understanding of other people's feelings and beliefs: individual differences and their antecedents. Child Development, 62(6), 1352–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ensor, R., & Hughes, C. (2008). Content or connectedness? Mother–child talk and early social understanding. Child Development, 79(1), 201–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fung, P. C., Chow, B. W. Y., & McBride-Chang, C. (2005). The impact of a dialogic reading program on deaf and hard-of-hearing kindergarten and early primary school-aged students in Hong Kong. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 10(1), 8295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Girolametto, L., Weitzman, E., Wiigs, M., & Pearce, P. S. (1999). The relationship between maternal language measures and language development in toddlers with expressive vocabulary delays. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 8(4), 364–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M., Hoff, E., Rowe, M. L., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2019). Language matters: denying the existence of the 30-million-word gap has serious consequences. Child Development, 90(3), 985–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Hoff, E., & Naigles, L. (2002). How children use input to acquire a lexicon. Child Development, 73(2), 418–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1985). Some contributions of mothers’ speech to their children's syntactic growth. Journal of Child Language, 12(2), 367–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holzinger, D., Fellinger, J., & Beitel, C. (2011). Early onset of family centred intervention predicts language outcomes in children with hearing loss. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 75(2), 256–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth – relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 236–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children's language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61(4), 343–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koehlinger, K. M., Van Horne, A. J. O., & Moeller, M. P. (2013). Grammatical outcomes of 3- and 6-year-old children who are hard of hearing. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(5), 1701–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laugen, N. J., Jacobsen, K. H., Rieffe, C., & Wichstrom, L. (2016). Predictors of psychosocial outcomes in hard-of-hearing preschool children. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 21(3), 259–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laugen, N. J., Jacobsen, K. H., Rieffe, C., & Wichstrom, L. (2017). Emotion understanding in preschool children with mild-to-severe hearing loss. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 22(2), 155–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: tools for analyzing talk (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mandrekar, J. N. (2011). Measures of interrater agreement. Journal of Thoracic Oncology, 6(1), 67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCreery, R. W., Walker, E. A., Spratford, M., Bentler, R., Holte, L., Roush, P., Oleson, J., van Buren, J., & Moeller, M. P. (2015). Longitudinal predictors of aided speech audibility in infants and children. Ear and Hearing, 36, 24S37S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meins, E. (2013). Sensitive attunement to infants’ internal states: operationalizing the construct of mind-mindedness. Attachment & Human Development, 15(5/6), 524–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meinzen-Derr, J., Wiley, S., & Choo, D. I. (2011). Impact of early intervention on expressive and receptive language development among young children with permanent hearing loss. American Annals of the Deaf, 155(5), 580–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, R. E., & Karchmer, M. A. (2004). Chasing the mythical ten percent: parental hearing status of deaf and hard of hearing students in the United States Sign Language Studies, 4(2), 138–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moeller, M. P. (2000). Early intervention and language development in children who are deaf and hard of hearing. Pediatrics, 106(3), e43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moeller, M. P., Carr, G., Seaver, L., Stredler-Brown, A., & Holzinger, D. (2013). Best practices in family-centered early intervention for children who are deaf or hard of hearing: an international consensus statement. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 18(4), 429–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moeller, M. P., & Schick, B. (2006). Relations between maternal input and theory of mind understanding in deaf children. Child Development, 77(3), 751–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moeller, M. P., & Tomblin, J. B. (2015). An introduction to the outcomes of children with hearing loss study. Ear and Hearing, 36(1), 4S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moeller, M. P., Tomblin, J. B., Yoshinaga-Itano, C., Connor, C. M., & Jerger, S. (2007). Current state of knowledge: language and literacy of children with hearing impairment. Ear and Hearing, 28(6), 740–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, G., Meristo, M., Mann, W., Hjelmquist, E., Surian, L., & Siegal, M. (2014). Mental state language and quality of conversational experience in deaf and hearing children. Cognitive Development, 29, 41–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Netten, A. P., Rieffe, C., Soede, W., Dirks, E., Korver, A. M. H., Konings, S., … & Frijns, J. H. M. (2017). Can you hear what I think? Theory of mind in young children with moderate hearing loss. Ear and Hearing, 38, 588–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Netten, A. P., Rieffe, C., Theunissen, S., Soede, W., Dirks, E., Korver, A. M. H., … & Frijns, J. H. M. (2015). Early identification: language skills and social functioning in deaf and hard of hearing preschool children. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 79(12), 2221–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nittrouer, S. (2009). Early development of children with hearing loss. San Diego, CA: Plural Publishing.Google Scholar
Peterson, C., & Slaughter, V. (2003). Opening windows into the mind: mothers’ preferences for mental state explanations and children's theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 18(3), 399429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowe, M. L. (2008). Child-directed speech: relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of Child Language, 35(1), 185205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. L. (2012). A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development, 83(5), 1762–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. L. (2013). Decontextualized language input and preschoolers’ vocabulary development. Seminars in Speech and Language, 34(4), 260–6.Google ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. L., Leech, K. A., & Cabrera, N. (2017). Going beyond input quantity: wh-questions matter for toddlers’ language and cognitive development. Cognitive Science, 41, 162–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruffman, T., Slade, L., & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between children's and mothers’ mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. Child Development, 73(3), 734–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaerlaekens A, Z. I., & Van Ommeslaeghe, K. (1993). Reynell Taalontwikkelingsschalen [Reynell Language Development Scales]. Nijmegen: Berkhout.Google Scholar
Schlichting, J., Eldik, M. v., Lutje Spelberg, H., Van der Meulen, B., & Van der Meulen, S. (1995). Schlichting Test voor Taalproductie [Schlichting Test for Language Production]. Nijmegen: Berkhout.Google Scholar
Stika, C. J., Eisenberg, L. S., Johnson, K. C., Henning, S. C., Colson, B. G., Ganguly, D. H., & DesJardin, J. L. (2015). Developmental outcomes of early-identified children who are hard of hearing at 12 to 18 months of age. Early Human Development, 91(1), 4755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stiles, D. J., Bentler, R. A., & McGregor, K. K. (2012). The speech intelligibility index and the pure-tone average as predictors of lexical ability in children fit with hearing aids. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55(3), 764–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taumoepeau, M. (2016). Maternal expansions of child language relate to growth in children's vocabulary. Language Learning and Development, 12(4), 429–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taumoepeau, M., & Reese, E. (2013). Maternal reminiscing, elaborative talk, and children's theory of mind: an intervention study. First Language, 33(4), 388410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taumoepeau, M., & Ruffman, T. (2006). Mother and infant talk about mental states relates to desire language and emotion understanding. Child Development, 77(2), 465–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taumoepeau, M., & Ruffman, T. (2008). Stepping stones to others’ minds: maternal talk relates to child mental state language and emotion understanding at 15, 24, and 33 months. Child Development, 79(2), 284302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomblin, J. B., Harrison, M., Ambrose, S. E., Walker, E. A., Oleson, J. J., & Moeller, M. P. (2015). Language outcomes in young children with mild to severe hearing loss. Ear and Hearing, 36, 76S91S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
VanDam, M., Ambrose, S. E., & Moeller, M. P. (2012). Quantity of parental language in the home environments of hard-of-hearing 2-year-olds. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 17(4), 402–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: the development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2143–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weizman, Z. O., & Snow, C. E. (2001). Lexical input as related to children's vocabulary acquisition: effects of sophisticated exposure and support for meaning. Developmental Psychology, 37(2), 265–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yoder, P. J., & Warren, S. F. (2001). Intentional communication elicits language-facilitating maternal responses in dyads with children who have developmental disabilities. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 106(4), 327–35.2.0.CO;2>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yoshinaga-Itano, C., Sedey, A. L., Coulter, D. K., & Mehl, A. L. (1998). Language of early-and later-identified children with hearing loss. Pediatrics, 102(5), 1161–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimmerman, F. J., Gilkerson, J., Richards, J. A., Christakis, D. A., Xu, D. X., Gray, S., & Yapanel, U. (2009). Teaching by listening: the importance of adult-child conversations to language development. Pediatrics, 124(1), 342–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the MHL and NH groups

Figure 1

Table 2. Summary statistics of child language and parental linguistic input

Figure 2

Table 3. Spearman's rho correlations for HL-related variables and parental linguistic input variables and child language scores for MHL children (n = 18)

Figure 3

Table 4. Spearman's rho correlations for parental linguistic input variables and child language scores

Figure 4

Table 5. Spearman's rho correlations for parental linguistic measures by group, with the MHL group above the diagonal gap and the NH group below the diagonal