Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T01:55:39.787Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Auditory and visual hallucination prevalence in Parkinson's disease and dementia with Lewy bodies: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 November 2018

Charlotte Louise Eversfield
Affiliation:
School of Healthcare Science, Manchester Metropolitan University, John Dalton Building, Chester Street, Manchester, M1 5GD, UK
Llwyd David Orton*
Affiliation:
School of Healthcare Science, Manchester Metropolitan University, John Dalton Building, Chester Street, Manchester, M1 5GD, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Llwyd David Orton, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Non-motor features of Parkinson's disease (PD) and dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB), such as auditory hallucinations (AH), contribute to disease burden but are not well understood.

Methods

Systematic review and random-effects meta-analyses of studies reporting AH associated with PD or DLB. Prevalence of visual hallucinations (VH) in identified studies meeting eligibility criteria were included in meta-analyses, facilitating comparison with AH. Synthesis of qualitative descriptions of AH was performed. PubMed, Web of Science and Scopus databases were searched for primary journal articles, written in English, published from 1970 to 2017. Studies reporting AH prevalence in PD or DLB were screened using PRISMA methods.

Results

Searches identified 4542 unique studies for consideration, of which, 26 met inclusion criteria. AH pooled prevalence in PD was estimated to be 8.9% [95% confidence interval (CI) 5.3–14.5], while in DLB was estimated to be 30.8% (±23.4 to 39.3). Verbal hallucinations, perceived as originating outside the head, were the most common form of AH. Non-verbal AH were also common while musical AH were rare. VH were more prevalent, with an estimated pooled prevalence in PD of 28.2% (±19.1 to 39.5), while in DLB they were estimated to be 61.8% (±49.1 to 73.0). Meta-regression determined that the use of validated methodologies to identify hallucinations produced higher prevalence estimates.

Conclusions

AH and VH present in a substantial proportion of PD and DLB cases, with VH reported more frequently in both conditions. Both AH and VH are more prevalent in DLB than PD. There is a need for standardised use of validated methods to detect and monitor hallucinations.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018

Introduction

Parkinson's disease (PD) and dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) are neurodegenerative diseases associated with α-synuclein dysfunction. Estimates suggest PD prevalence is 1% in people over 60 (De Lau and Breteler, Reference De Lau and Breteler2006), while DLB has a prevalence of 0.4% in people over 65 (Vann Jones and O'Brien, Reference Vann Jones and O'Brien2014). Both conditions are characterised by motor dysfunction but non-motor features contribute extensively to their presentation and disease burden.

Hallucinations, spontaneous aberrant perceptions, occur in a significant proportion of cases (Diederich et al., Reference Diederich, Fénelon, Stebbins and Goetz2009). Hallucinations can be induced by medications such as anticholinergics (Celesia and Wanamaker, Reference Celesia and Wanamaker1972), dopamine agonists (Baker et al., Reference Baker, Silver, White, Kluger, Aberle, Patel and Coleman2009) and a range of medications modulating diverse neurochemical pathways (Porteous and Ross, Reference Porteous and Ross1956; Lees et al., Reference Lees, Clarke and Harrison1977; Gondim et al., Reference Gondim, Costa, Taunay, De Oliveira, Ferreira and Rola2010; Friedman et al., Reference Friedman, Agarwal, Alcalay, Black, Chou, Cote, Dayalu, Frank, Hartlein and Hauser2011; Wand, Reference Wand2012). This presents challenges to determining the causes and nature of AH in PD and DLB. The majority of hallucinations in PD and DLB are chronic, recurring and progressive in spite of stable medication regimens (Fénelon et al., Reference Fénelon, Mahieux, Huon and Ziégler2000). Indeed, cognitive, sensory and circadian aspects also contribute to hallucinosis (Mosimann et al., Reference Mosimann, Rowan, Partington, Collerton, Littlewood, O'Brien, Burn and Mckeith2006). Neuroleptics are often administered on presentation of AH, yet have moderate efficacy and potentially severe side effects, including increased mortality (McKeith et al., Reference Mckeith, Fairbairn, Perry, Thompson and Perry1992a; Weintraub et al., Reference Weintraub, Chiang, Kim, Wilkinson, Marras, Stanislawski, Mamikonyan and Kales2016).

Visual hallucinations (VH) constitute a core feature of DLB diagnosis (McKeith et al., Reference Mckeith, Boeve, Dickson, Halliday, Taylor, Weintraub, Aarsland, Galvin, Attems and Ballard2017) and have been described as a hallmark of PD (Onofrj et al., Reference Onofrj, Thomas and Bonanni2007). An associated, but distinct condition, PD dementia (PDD) (Dubois et al., Reference Dubois, Burn, Goetz, Aarsland, Brown, Broe, Dickson, Duyckaerts, Cummings and Gauthier2007), also presents with motor and non-motor features, including hallucinations, but is under-reported in the literature. In PD, PDD and DLB, auditory hallucinations (AH) are generally considered of secondary concern, in spite of the progressive nature of AH, their contribution to loss of insight, decreased quality of life and consequent influence on the decision to move patients into long-term care (Goetz and Stebbins, Reference Goetz and Stebbins1993; Aarsland et al., Reference Aarsland, Larsen, Tandberg and Laake2000).

Previous reports vary widely in reported prevalence of AH in PD from 2% (Leu-Semenescu et al., Reference Leu-Semenescu, De Cock, Le Masson, Debs, Lavault, Roze, Vidailhet and Arnulf2011) to 45% (Amar et al., Reference Amar, Yadav, Reddy and Pal2014; Llorca et al., Reference Llorca, Pereira, Jardri, Chereau-Boudet, Brousse, Misdrahi, Fénelon, Tronche, Schwan and Lançon2016). Prevalence rates also range widely in DLB, from 18% (Suárez-González et al., Reference Suárez-González, Serrano-Pozo, Arroyo-Anlló, Franco-Macías, Polo, García-Solís and Gil-Néciga2014) to 43% (Piggott et al., Reference Piggott, Ballard, Rowan, Holmes, Mckeith, Jaros, Perry and Perry2007). Previous studies reporting prevalence of AH have predominantly been cross-sectional, with limited focus on the nature of AH reported. Furthermore, methods of determining the presence of hallucinations are diverse, potentially leading to differing reporting rates.

Aims of this study: This complex picture suggested a need to characterise, with increased precision, the prevalence and nature of AH in PD and DLB. Therefore, in this study, we aimed to conduct a systematic review and meta-analysis of studies reporting AH prevalence in PD and DLB. Furthermore, we aimed to assess the types of AH in both conditions and compared their prevalence with that of VH, which are more commonly investigated.

Methods

Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines were used to standardise the conduct and reporting of this study. The protocol for this study was registered in advance on PROSPERO (registration number: CRD42017067337). Ethical approval for this study was awarded by the Faculty of Science and Engineering Ethics Committee at Manchester Metropolitan University (EthOS Reference Number: 0240).

Search strategy

Literature searches for candidate studies were undertaken in the following databases: PubMed, Web of Science and Scopus. Search terms were text words: auditory, auditory hallucinations, hearing, dementia, Lewy bodies, dementia with Lewy bodies, Lewy body dementia, Parkinson's disease. The Boolean operator AND was used to maximise the number of identified papers containing combinations of search terms. A search matrix was used to ensure all paired combinations of search terms were searched for in each database.

Study selection

The search was conducted from December 2016 to November 2017. Papers published from 1 January 1970 to 13 November 2017 were considered for inclusion. Titles and abstracts were examined to remove duplications and irrelevant studies. To be included, studies needed to (i) be written in English, (ii) report measures of AH prevalence in patients with PD or DLB, and (iii) be structured as a prospective cohort, case–control or cross-sectional study. Unpublished data were not pursued or included. Both investigators examined full-text versions of studies meeting the above criteria to assess compliance with inclusion criteria and extract data. We reviewed reference lists of all included articles to identify other potentially eligible studies.

Data extraction and risk of bias assessment

Both authors extracted data from included papers, including study authors; publication year; study title; journal; volume; issue; pages; study design; number of participants; number of female participants; number of participants diagnosed with PD/DLB; mean age of disease onset; number of participants with AH and/or VH; qualitative descriptions of AH; time window for hallucination presentation; method of hallucination assessment. Both reviewers independently evaluated risk of bias for each study using criteria adapted from the NIH Quality Assessment Tool for Observational Cohort and Cross-Sectional Studies (https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-topics/study-quality-assessment-tools). One point was awarded for each question we felt could be answered in the affirmative. Our implementation of this tool assessed study quality across nine domains: aims/objectives stated; experimental protocol appropriately described; selection bias (see Results for details); participant inclusion/exclusion criteria (selected from similar populations/appropriate diagnosis of PD or DLB); statistical analyses appropriate; condition assessed prior to outcomes; dropouts reported; timeframe for hallucination presentation sufficient; outcome measures clearly defined, valid, reliable and implemented consistently across all study participants; and presence of detailed qualitative description of AH. Scores were summed for each study to provide an overall score of bias and quality. No weighting was used in bias assessment. Studies were then grouped into those of highest (scores = 9/9), high (score = 8/9), moderate-to-high (score = 7/9), moderate (score = 6/9) and poor (score = 1–5/9) quality. Poor quality studies were excluded (Inzelberg et al., Reference Inzelberg, Kipervasser and Korczyn1998; Katzen et al., Reference Katzen, Myerson, Papapetropoulos, Nahab, Gallo and Levin2010; Goetz et al., Reference Goetz, Stebbins and Ouyang2011; Grau-Rivera et al., Reference Grau-Rivera, Gelpi, Rey, Valldeoriola, Tolosa, Compta and Martí2013). A comparison of all binary decisions made found 77.4% agreement between the authors. Discrepancies were settled by discussion and consensus.

Data analysis

Our primary outcome measure was AH prevalence in patients with PD or DLB. All included studies also reported prevalence of VH, which we also extracted as a secondary outcome. Where studies reported longitudinal data, we extracted the maximum values reported. Prevalence estimates in longitudinal studies were not higher than other study designs. Indeed, one longitudinal estimate (Goetz et al., Reference Goetz, Vogel, Tanner and Stebbins1998) reported the lowest prevalence of both auditory and VH in PD, suggesting this method did not bias our findings.

We conducted meta-analyses of AH and VH in Lewy body disease (LBD; pooled PD and DLB), and PD and DLB. Due to the range of study designs and different patient populations we anticipated would be included in our study, and the consequent assumption that effect sizes would be sampled from a population of effect sizes that could vary due to factors other than just sampling error, we planned to carry out random-effects models a priori.

We constructed random-effects models using Comprehensive Meta-Analyses software (Borenstein et al., Reference Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins and Rothstein2009). We calculated pooled prevalence estimates with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) and assessed heterogeneity using the I 2 statistic. Possible publication bias was assessed via the fail-safe N, Begg's funnel plot and Begg and Mazumdar's rank correlation tests. If publication bias was suspected, we used Duval and Tweedie's trim and fill to adjust our prevalence estimates. Outputs from these analyses were imported to an online forest plot generator to create figures (https://www.evidencepartners.com/resources/forest-plot-generator/).

Meta-regression models were created to investigate the potential contribution of study-level covariates to the observed heterogeneity in our pooled prevalence estimates. Log pooled prevalence estimate was the dependent variable, while study quality score, mean age at disease onset and the use of validated methods to detect hallucinations were set as predictive variables. Due to the diversity of methods employed to detect hallucinations, it was not possible to compare each technique. However, a clear distinction could be drawn between those studies that employed validated methods [NeuroPsychiatric Inventory (NPI), Manchester and Oxford Universities Scale for the Psychopathological Assessment of Dementia (MOUSPAD), Columbia University Scale for Psychopathology in Alzheimer's Disease (CUSPAD), Psycho-Sensory hAllucinations Scale (PSAS), University of Miami Parkinson's Disease Hallucinations Questionnaire (UM-PDHQ), Parkinson's Psychosis Rating Scale (PPRS) or Queen Square Visual Hallucination Inventory (QSVHI)] and those that did not (Rush Hallucination Inventory, semi-structured interview, questionnaire, screening hospital records or diagnostic interview and checklist).

We undertook sensitivity analyses to assess the robustness of our pooled prevalence estimate of AH and VH. We investigated the effect of year of publication by sequentially excluding studies published before 2000, 2005 and 2010. We also examined whether study design influenced outcomes by examining cross-sectional studies only and examined the effect of different quality score cut-off values for inclusion by sequentially excluding studies with scores of less than seven or eight out of nine.

Results

Study selection

After duplicate removal, we identified 4542 unique articles through primary database searches. Screening titles and abstracts led to the elimination of 4499 irrelevant articles. Full-text versions of the remaining 43 potentially eligible articles were assessed. Of these, 13 did not meet inclusion criteria, leaving 30 articles, published between 1992 and 2016, in the qualitative synthesis. A further four articles were excluded from quantitative meta-analyses due to low-quality assessment scores (Fig. 1). This produced 26 studies eligible for inclusion in the meta-analysis of AH prevalence (online Supplementary Table S1).

Fig. 1. PRISMA flow diagram demonstrating study selection process.

Characteristics of included studies

Of the included studies, 19 were cross-sectional studies, four were longitudinal studies and three were case–control studies. These studies represent data from 10 countries, the majority of which were conducted in Europe (n = 15), while others were undertaken in North America (n = 6) and Asia (n = 5).

Five studies (Ballard et al., Reference Ballard, Holmes, Mckeith, Neill, O'Brien, Cairns, Lantos, Perry, Ince and Perry1999; Aarsland et al., Reference Aarsland, Larsen, Tandberg and Laake2000; Ballard et al., Reference Ballard, O'Brien, Swann, Thompson, Neill and Mckeith2001; Piggott et al., Reference Piggott, Ballard, Rowan, Holmes, Mckeith, Jaros, Perry and Perry2007; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Warren and Lees2008) diagnosed DLB using McKeith et al. (Reference Mckeith, Galasko, Kosaka, Perry, Dickson, Hansen, Salmon, Lowe, Mirra and Byrne1996) consensus criteria, two (Suárez-González et al., Reference Suárez-González, Serrano-Pozo, Arroyo-Anlló, Franco-Macías, Polo, García-Solís and Gil-Néciga2014; Shea et al., Reference Shea, Ha and Chu2015) employed the McKeith (Reference Mckeith2006) consensus criteria, while one (Klatka et al., Reference Klatka, Louis and Schiffer1996) used post-mortem neuropathologic diagnosis.

Fifteen studies (Fernandez et al., Reference Fernandez, Stern and Lees1992; Graham et al., Reference Graham, Grünewald and Sagar1997; Fénelon et al., Reference Fénelon, Mahieux, Huon and Ziégler2000; Holroyd et al., Reference Holroyd, Currie and Wooten2001; Gupta et al., Reference Gupta, Singh, Khwaja and Mehndiratta2004; de Maindreville et al., Reference De Maindreville, Fénelon and Mahieux2005; Pacchetti et al., Reference Pacchetti, Manni, Zangaglia, Mancini, Marchioni, Tassorelli, Terzaghi, Ossola, Martignoni and Moglia2005; Papapetropoulos et al., Reference Papapetropoulos, Katzen, Schrag, Singer, Scanlon, Nation, Guevara and Levin2008; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Warren and Lees2008; Fénelon et al., Reference Fénelon, Soulas, Zenasni and De Langavant2010; Mack et al., Reference Mack, Rabins, Anderson, Goldstein, Grill, Hirsch, Lehmann, Little, Margolis and Palanci2012; Svetel et al., Reference Svetel, Smiljković, Pekmezović and Kostić2012; Amar et al., Reference Amar, Yadav, Reddy and Pal2014; de Chazeron et al., Reference De Chazeron, Pereira, Chereau-Boudet, Brousse, Misdrahi, Fénelon, Tronche, Schwan, Lançon and Marques2015; Llorca et al., Reference Llorca, Pereira, Jardri, Chereau-Boudet, Brousse, Misdrahi, Fénelon, Tronche, Schwan and Lançon2016) diagnosed PD according to a version of the United Kingdom Parkinson's Disease Society Brain Bank Criteria (Gibb and Lees, Reference Gibb and Lees1988). Five other studies used different diagnostic criteria, including definite or probable PD based on Larsen et al. (Reference Larsen, Dupont and Tandberg1994) criteria (Aarsland et al., Reference Aarsland, Larsen, Tandberg and Laake2000); Calne et al. (Reference Calne, Snow and Lee1992) criteria (Paleacu et al., Reference Paleacu, Schechtman and Inzelberg2005); Gelb et al. (Reference Gelb, Oliver and Gilman1999) criteria (Lee and Weintraub, Reference Lee and Weintraub2012); Core Assessment Program for Intracerebral Transplantations; Langston et al. (Reference Langston, Koller and Giron1992) criteria (Goetz et al., Reference Goetz, Vogel, Tanner and Stebbins1998); or criteria of Hughes et al. (Reference Hughes, Daniel, Kilford and Lees1992) for idiopathic PD (Leu-Semenescu et al., Reference Leu-Semenescu, De Cock, Le Masson, Debs, Lavault, Roze, Vidailhet and Arnulf2011).

Our quality assessment rated four studies as highest quality (score = 9/9), 12 as high quality (8/9), seven as moderate-to-high quality (7/9) and four as moderate quality (6/9) (online Supplementary Table S2). As a population, study quality was weakest in reporting of qualitative descriptions of hallucinations, with other areas of quality assessment scoring being consistently high among included studies.

Demographics of dataset

The mean age at onset of diagnosis was 61.9 years (s.d. = 7.6). We attempted to conduct group wise comparisons between diagnoses of PD without dementia (PDWD), PDD and DLB; however, PDD and PDWD were only separately reported in one study; consequently, these data were pooled into one PD group. Data from 3774 patients were identified, of which 3420 (90.6%) had PD, with the remainder DLB, and 1178 (31.2%) were female (range = 3.3–60.4%). A higher proportion of females were found in the DLB group (mean = 48.6%; range = 20.0–56.1%), than PD (mean = 30.6%; range = 3.3–60.4%). Mean age at onset for DLB was 74.1 years (s.d. = 8.1), while mean age at onset for PD was 58.8 years (s.d. = 10.6).

Overall pooled prevalence of AH

The overall random-effects model pooled prevalence of AH in LBD (Fig. 2) was 11.9% (95% CI ±7.9 to 17.7). To compare the relative prevalence of AH in DLB and PD, two further random-effects models were constructed for each condition, independent of the other. Pooled prevalence of AH in DLB (Fig. 3a) was 30.8% (±23.4 to 39.3), while in PD (Fig. 3b), it was 8.9% (±5.3 to 14.5).

Fig. 2. Forest plot showing random-effects model estimate of AH overall pooled prevalence of 11.9% (±7.9 to 17.7) in Lewy body disease (LBD = DLB, PDD and PDND combined). I 2 = between-study heterogeneity; POP = study population; PREV = prevalence; LCL = lower confidence level; UCL = upper confidence level; WGHT = weight under random effects model.

Fig. 3. Forest plot showing random-effects model estimates of AH prevalence of 30.8% (±23.4 to 39.3) in DLB and 8.9% (±5.3 to 14.5) in PD.

Overall pooled prevalence of VH

We also extracted information on VH from the 26 included studies. The study by Leu-Semenescu et al. (Reference Leu-Semenescu, De Cock, Le Masson, Debs, Lavault, Roze, Vidailhet and Arnulf2011) reported selection bias for this analysis as all 100 PD patients had VH due to the study design, and was therefore excluded leaving 25 studies in this analysis. The overall random-effects pooled prevalence of VH in LBD (Fig. 4) was 35.9% (±26.2 to 47.0). Pooled prevalence of VH in DLB (Fig. 5a) was 61.8% (±49.1 to 73.0), while in PD (Fig. 5b), it was 28.2% (±19.1 to 39.5).

Fig. 4. Forest plot showing random-effects model estimate of VH prevalence of 35.9% (±26.2 to 47.0) in LBD.

Fig. 5. Forest plot showing random-effects model estimates of VH prevalence of 61.8% (±49.1 to 73.0) in DLB and 28.2% (±19.1 to 39.5) in PD.

Low prevalence of single modality auditory or VH

Pure sensory hallucinations (i.e. those in only one single sensory domain) were less common. There were two (0.6%) reports of pure AH in DLB and 23 (0.7%) in PD. Pure VH were more common than auditory in both conditions, being found in 36 (10.2%) DLB cases and 122 (3.6%) PD cases. Qualitative analysis of included longitudinal studies (Graham et al., Reference Graham, Grünewald and Sagar1997; Goetz et al., Reference Goetz, Vogel, Tanner and Stebbins1998; Ballard et al., Reference Ballard, O'Brien, Swann, Thompson, Neill and Mckeith2001; de Maindreville et al., Reference De Maindreville, Fénelon and Mahieux2005) revealed that pure VH tended to predate AH in both PD and DLB. As each condition progressed, AH tended to bind with recurrent complex VH to form multi-modal hallucinations that increased in prevalence from 1.5 to 10 years post baseline assessment (Goetz et al., Reference Goetz, Stebbins and Ouyang2011).

Types of AH

The rate of reporting qualitative descriptions of AH was low, with data only available from six studies; five of which described data from PD patients, while the other described data from DLB patients.

The most commonly reported type of AH were verbal, which were reported in all six studies. Verbal AH were reported as human voices originating from outside the patient's head, often indistinct or incomprehensible, originating outside the visual field. Verbal AH were described as ‘non-threatening’, ‘non-imperative’, ‘non-congruent’ and ‘non-paranoid’. Three studies reported the proportion of AH that were verbal: Amar et al. (Reference Amar, Yadav, Reddy and Pal2014) found 11/18 (61.1%) of patents had verbal AH in PD; Fénelon et al. (Reference Fénelon, Mahieux, Huon and Ziégler2000) reported a similar proportion in PD – 13/21 (61.9%); while Suárez-González et al. (Reference Suárez-González, Serrano-Pozo, Arroyo-Anlló, Franco-Macías, Polo, García-Solís and Gil-Néciga2014) found 5/14 (35.7%) had verbal AH in DLB.

Non-verbal sounds, such as inanimate (bullet fired, doorbell ringing, tinkling of bells, walking on steps, cracking sounds or squeaking) or animate sounds (dogs barking, tigers and lions roaring) were also common. Musical hallucinations were rare, being reported in only three patients by Fénelon et al. (Reference Fénelon, Mahieux, Huon and Ziégler2000), two of whom were described as ‘deaf’.

Hallucination assessment

Across the 26 studies, there were 10 different methods employed to determine the presence of hallucinations (online Supplementary Table S1). The most common method was a semi-structured interview (n = 10), followed by NPI (n = 5), CUSPAD (n = 2), PSAS (n = 2), unnamed questionnaire (n = 2), UM-PDHQ (n = 1), MOUSPAD (n = 1), PPRS (n = 1), QSVHI (n = 1), screening of hospital records (n = 1) and not stated (=1).

Meta-regression analyses

We observed considerable heterogeneity in all meta-analyses (I 2 range = 51.2–96.3), suggesting a large proportion of the observed variance may be due to real differences between studies. To investigate whether some of the observed heterogeneity could be explained by moderator variables, such as study quality score, mean age of disease onset or method of hallucination assessment, we constructed meta-regression models for those meta-analyses comprised of sufficient study numbers (LBD and PD but not DLB). The results of these four models (online Supplementary Table S3) revealed the use of validated hallucination assessment methods could explain a significant proportion of the variance for each meta-regression (R 2 range = 0.09–0.38), but study quality, mean age at disease onset and disease duration could not.

The fail-safe N for our meta-analysis of AH in LBD was 5649 (Z = −28.9; p < 0.0001), while there was only minor asymmetry in Begg's funnel plot (online Supplementary Fig. S6A). Begg and Mazumdar's rank correlation test suggested that publication bias was not present (Kendall's Τ-b = −0.23; p = 0.09) and employing Duval and Tweedie's trim and fill did not modify the random-effects effect size estimate. Similar values were found for our meta-analyses of AH in PD (online Supplementary Fig. S6B) and DLB (online Supplementary Fig. S6C), though trim and fill on the latter analysis imputed three studies that increased the pooled prevalence estimate to 37.5% (±27.7 to 48.5).

The fail-safe N for our meta-analysis of VHs in LBD was 1280 (Z = −14.16; p < 0.0001), while there was only minor asymmetry in Begg's funnel plot (online Supplementary Fig. S6D). Begg and Mazumdar's rank correlation test suggested that publication bias was not present (Kendall's Τ-b = −0.11; p = 0.44) and employing Duval and Tweedie's trim and fill did not modify the random-effects estimate. Similar values were found for our meta-analyses of VH in PD (online Supplementary Fig. S6E) and DLB (online Supplementary Fig. S6F), though trim and fill on the former analysis imputed four studies and increased the pooled prevalence estimate to 33.4% (±31.4 to 35.4).

We further investigated the potential role of cognition in hallucination status. Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores were reported in 16 studies (10 PD cases only; four DLB only; two both conditions). Pooled MMSE scores ranged from 13.2 to 29.2 (mean = 24.1). For PD, MMSE scores were lower (mean = 25.9; s.d.± = 2.3) for cases with hallucinations than those without (27.6 ± 1.2). For DLB, MMSE scores were similar for cases with hallucinations (17.3 ± 3.0) and those without hallucinations (17.2 ± 3.1). A correlation analysis found a negative relationship between MMSE scores and AH prevalence [r (s) = −0.61; p = 0.008].

Sensitivity analyses

We assessed the impact of year of study, study design and quality score on the robustness of our pooled prevalence estimates of auditory and VH in LBD. These analyses indicated that our estimates were robust, but were a few percentage points lower than analyses only including studies published from 2010 onwards or analyses only including moderate-to-high and high-quality studies (online Supplementary Table S4).

Discussion

We report that AH and VH present in a significant proportion of PD and DLB cases, with both forms of hallucination being more prevalent in DLB. We found that VH have a higher prevalence than AH in both conditions, both occurring at rates much higher than those found in the general population (Ohayon, Reference Ohayon2000; Waters et al., Reference Waters, Blom, Jardri, Hugdahl and Sommer2018). Of note were the wide variety of methods used to determine the presence of hallucinations. We found that more recently published studies, using validated methods, produced higher estimates of hallucination prevalence, suggesting a need for wider adoption of such approaches. Taken together, these data demonstrate that AH have a higher prevalence in PD and DLB than commonly assumed.

Challenges to existing models of hallucinations

Existing models of recurrent complex VH have considered VH to exist in isolation from other modalities. Some models highlight dysfunctional attentional, cognitive and perceptual networks (Collerton et al., Reference Collerton, Perry and Mckeith2005; Diederich et al., Reference Diederich, Goetz and Stebbins2005; Shine et al., Reference Shine, Halliday, Naismith and Lewis2011). Our data suggest that in PD and DLB, most cases of VH progress to become multi-modal hallucinations, incorporating a bound AH to the VH (e.g. hallucinations of people progress such that they can be heard talking). Attentional-cognitive models could account for these observations; however, sensory deficits seem incongruous with bottom-up perceptual elements of existing models. The contribution of bottom-up sensory aspects to VH has been shown via studies detailing ocular (Urwyler et al., Reference Urwyler, Nef, Killen, Collerton, Thomas, Burn, Mckeith and Mosimann2014) and occipital lobe dysfunction (Meppelink et al., Reference Meppelink, De Jong, Renken, Leenders, Cornelissen and Van Laar2009), while central, top-down contributions involving frontal (Sanchez-Castaneda et al., Reference Sanchez-Castaneda, Rene, Ramirez-Ruiz, Campdelacreu, Gascon, Falcon, Calopa, Jauma, Juncadella and Junque2010) and temporal (Harding et al., Reference Harding, Broe and Halliday2002) lobes also play a role.

However, hearing loss and auditory dysfunction are common at ages associated with PD and DLB diagnosis (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Niparko and Ferrucci2011). It is therefore challenging to account for VH progressing to multi-modal hallucinations, binding with AH, due to a visual perceptual deficit occurring in these cases, followed by an auditory deficit. Models of simple AH, such as tinnitus, incorporate loss of peripheral drive with adaptive changes in gain (Eggermont, Reference Eggermont1990), reductions in inhibition throughout the auditory pathway (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Brozoski and Caspary2011) and mismatches with central predictive coding (Sedley et al., Reference Sedley, Friston, Gander, Kumar and Griffiths2016), yet rarely do these changes lead to more complex AH.

Attentional networks may tend to be directed more towards the visual scene, leading to more prevalent reporting of VH when these networks dysfunction. This may be due to attentional focus being more easily directed towards visual than auditory objects (Shinn-Cunningham, Reference Shinn-Cunningham2008). As widespread degeneration progresses, attentional deficits may facilitate widespread connectivity and phantom binding of VH with AH, perhaps acting via hyperexcitable cortical and subcortical networks (Grossberg, Reference Grossberg2000; Robson et al., Reference Robson, Tweedy, Manzanza, Taylor, Atkinson, Randall, Reeve, Clowry and Lebeau2018).

Strengths and limitations

Our findings are supported by the large proportion of moderate-to-high quality studies included in our meta-analyses (online Supplementary Table S2). The accuracy of our prevalence estimates are supported by the majority of studies being cross-sectional, the best experimental design by which to estimate prevalence (Mann, Reference Mann2003). Furthermore, our sensitivity analyses showed that only including cross-sectional studies had little effect on our estimates (online Supplementary Table S4). However, the time window for detection of auditory or VH ranged up to 30 years post-diagnosis (Graham et al., Reference Graham, Grünewald and Sagar1997), limiting the temporal precision of our estimates. A selection bias may exist in our estimates due to studies selecting patients from movement disorder clinics with few community-based samples; future studies of different populations may allow insights into hallucinations in different populations.

The use of various methods to identify hallucinations was a major contributor to the high degree of heterogeneity in our meta-analyses (online Supplementary Table S3). Meta-regression models found that validated methods produced higher prevalence estimates than non-validated, suggesting that there are advantages to such approaches. However, within the range of validated measures reported in our sample (UM-PDHQ, MOUSPAD, CUSPAD, PSAS, PPRS, NPI and QSVHI) exist substantial differences in approach and outcomes. Comparisons between these approaches are beyond the scope of this study. Future work comparing these methods within the same study population may be useful. Method of hallucination assessment is an important issue, as most patients do not report AH when they first perceive them (Chou et al., Reference Chou, Messing, Oakes, Feldman, Breier and Friedman2005). This may be due to AH being less easily identified than visual, but could also be due to patient knowledge that AH are commonly associated with psychiatric conditions.

Our initial focus was to estimate AH prevalence in PD and DLB. As all studies included in this analysis also reported the number of participants who had VH, we also extracted and analysed these data. Numerous papers in the literature report VH but do not report other forms of hallucinations. Consequently, our estimates of VH do not contain all available evidence, but do provide comparison data that allow us to have confidence that VH are more common than AH.

Across the timespan of included studies, diagnostic criteria for PD have largely remained unchanged; while there have been multiple iterations of the consensus criteria for DLB diagnosis, each of which has modified the specificity and sensitivity of this diagnosis (Rizzo et al., Reference Rizzo, Arcuti, Copetti, Alessandria, Savica, Fontana, Liguori and Logroscino2017). This may be another source of between-studies heterogeneity; however, the low number of DLB studies included in our analyses did not allow meta-regression models to be constructed for these data. The lowest I 2 values we observed in our meta-analyses were found for DLB studies (Figs 3a and 5a), which argues against the possible contribution of diagnostic criteria to the observed heterogeneity.

Two potentially confounding covariates were later age of diagnosis of DLB than PD and the inclusion of lower quality studies (online Supplementary Table S2). Importantly, neither of these was found to account for a significant proportion of the variance in our meta-regression models (online Supplementary Table S3). This does not exclude the possibility that age of diagnosis or age per se contributes to hallucinosis. Indeed, the presentation of VH in age-matched DLB and PDD patients shows extensive overlap, suggesting age-related changes may contribute to their generation (Mosimann et al., Reference Mosimann, Rowan, Partington, Collerton, Littlewood, O'Brien, Burn and Mckeith2006). We were able to include data from four longitudinal cohort studies; however, two of these studies reported data for 1-year post-diagnosis, meaning that estimates of point prevalence over time were not possible in the present study. There is evidence that hallucination point prevalence in PD increases over time to affect a majority of patients (Goetz et al., Reference Goetz, Wuu, Curgian and Leurgans2005; Hely et al., Reference Hely, Reid, Adena, Halliday and Morris2008). Indeed, there is evidence that hallucinations increase in prevalence with age, post-PD diagnosis (Graham et al., Reference Graham, Grünewald and Sagar1997; Biglan et al., Reference Biglan, Holloway, Mcdermott, Richard and Investigators2007), with VH progressing towards polysensory phenotypes (Goetz et al., Reference Goetz, Stebbins and Ouyang2011). Furthermore, once perceived, hallucinations generally recur and are accompanied by a lack of insight (Goetz et al., Reference Goetz, Fan, Leurgans, Bernard and Stebbins2006), leading to increased risk of requiring placement in care facilities (Goetz and Stebbins, Reference Goetz and Stebbins1993; Aarsland et al., Reference Aarsland, Larsen, Tandberg and Laake2000).

Types of AH

AH in LDB are complex, typically polymodal and varied in their presentation, although there is a paucity of high-quality, qualitative descriptions. Most common were verbal AH, perceived as originating outside the head, which differentiates verbal AH in PD or DLB from those found in schizophrenia. Interestingly, of the three studies to report relative rates of different types of AHs, two found that verbal hallucinations formed the majority in PD (Fénelon et al., Reference Fénelon, Mahieux, Huon and Ziégler2000; Amar et al., Reference Amar, Yadav, Reddy and Pal2014) while Suárez-González et al. (Reference Suárez-González, Serrano-Pozo, Arroyo-Anlló, Franco-Macías, Polo, García-Solís and Gil-Néciga2014) reported that these were a minority in DLB. This may suggest a difference in AH presentation between the two conditions.

Non-verbal AH (acouasms) were also common and complex, whether animate or inanimate. These findings suggest auditory cortex and wider temporal and frontal lobe involvement in AH in PD and DLB, a speculation that is supported by neuroimaging data (Matsui et al., Reference Matsui, Nishinaka, Oda, Hara, Komatsu, Kubori and Udaka2006). Some reports of simpler acouasms were also reported. The potential overlap between acouasms and tinnitus suggests that our estimated pooled prevalence of AH may be lower than the true prevalence in the population. Indeed, a recent cross-sectional study found that in a sample of 1000 patients in a cognitive neurology clinic, verbal and musical hallucinations had a prevalence of 0.9%, while tinnitus was present in 6.9% (Bayón et al., Reference Bayón, De Sagrario and Sampedro2017).

AH were described as providing a soundtrack to VH (Fénelon et al., Reference Fénelon, Mahieux, Huon and Ziégler2000), such as when a patient hears conversations of visually hallucinated people talking. This presentation is of note, as numerous authors described VH as preceding AH, while the polymodal combination of VH and AH may provide diagnostic utility in differentiating cognitive and functional impairment in DLB from Alzheimer's disease (Suárez-González et al., Reference Suárez-González, Serrano-Pozo, Arroyo-Anlló, Franco-Macías, Polo, García-Solís and Gil-Néciga2014). These data agree with early operation criteria for DLB diagnosis (McKeith et al., Reference Mckeith, Perry, Fairbairn, Jabeen and Perry1992b). While the first consensus guidelines for DLB diagnosis included AH as supportive features (McKeith et al., Reference Mckeith, Galasko, Kosaka, Perry, Dickson, Hansen, Salmon, Lowe, Mirra and Byrne1996), more recent updates have removed them from consideration (McKeith, Reference Mckeith2006; McKeith et al., Reference Mckeith, Boeve, Dickson, Halliday, Taylor, Weintraub, Aarsland, Galvin, Attems and Ballard2017). A recent analysis found that the consensus criteria for DLB had become more sensitive but less specific through these iterations, with little change in diagnostic accuracy (Rizzo et al., Reference Rizzo, Arcuti, Copetti, Alessandria, Savica, Fontana, Liguori and Logroscino2017). Whether the use of validated methods to detect AH is of any diagnostic utility in DLB or PD requires further investigation.

It is interesting to note that two of the three participants who reported musical hallucinations in our sample were described as ‘deaf’ (Fénelon et al., Reference Fénelon, Mahieux, Huon and Ziégler2000). Musical hallucinations have been associated primarily with hearing impairment (Gordon, Reference Gordon1997; Cope and Baguley, Reference Cope and Baguley2009; Perez et al., Reference Perez, Garcia-Antelo and Rubio-Nazabal2017), though they have been reported in PD (Ergün et al., Reference Ergün, Bozbas, Akin and Inan2009) and DLB (Golden and Josephs, Reference Golden and Josephs2015) without hearing impairment. The contributions and potential interactions between hearing impairment and PD or DLB require further investigation, as there are suggestions that hearing impairment may present as a non-motor feature of PD (Lai et al., Reference Lai, Liao, Lin, Lin and Sung2014) and hearing impairment may be more common in PD than age-matched controls (Yýlmaz et al., Reference Yýlmaz, Karalý, Tokmak, Güçlü, Koçer and Öztürk2009).

Conclusion

This study is the first, to our knowledge, to summarise, synthesise and contrast evidence for AH and VH prevalence in PD and DLB. AH and VH contribute to disease burden in a significant proportion of LDB cases. Methods of identification and assessment of AH and VH requires investigation to standardise measurements. Successful developments in this field may improve the accuracy of hallucination diagnosis and inform disease progression monitoring and interventions.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718003161

Contributors

CLE and LDO were involved in the conception, design, planning, data extraction, analysis, writing and overseeing completeness of the manuscript. Both authors gave final approval of the manuscript.

Conflict of interest

None.

Financial support

This work was supported by an Action on Hearing Loss Flexi grant to LDO (grant number F68_ORTON).

Author ORCIDs

Llwyd David Orton https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8578-2296.

References

Aarsland, D, Larsen, JP, Tandberg, E and Laake, K (2000) Predictors of nursing home placement in Parkinson's disease: a population-based, prospective study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 48, 938942.Google Scholar
Amar, B, Yadav, R, Reddy, YJ and Pal, PK (2014) A clinical profile of patients with Parkinson's disease and psychosis. Annals of Indian Academy of Neurology 17, 187.Google Scholar
Baker, WL, Silver, D, White, CM, Kluger, J, Aberle, J, Patel, AA and Coleman, CI (2009) Dopamine agonists in the treatment of early Parkinson's disease: a meta-analysis. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders 15, 287294.Google Scholar
Ballard, C, Holmes, C, Mckeith, I, Neill, D, O'Brien, J, Cairns, N, Lantos, P, Perry, E, Ince, P and Perry, R (1999) Psychiatric morbidity in dementia with Lewy bodies: a prospective clinical and neuropathological comparative study with Alzheimer's disease. American Journal of Psychiatry 156, 10391045.Google Scholar
Ballard, CG, O'Brien, JT, Swann, AG, Thompson, P, Neill, D and Mckeith, IG (2001) The natural history of psychosis and depression in dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer's disease: persistence and new cases over 1 year of follow-up. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 62, 4649.Google Scholar
Bayón, AR, De Sagrario, MT and Sampedro, FG (2017) Auditory hallucinations in cognitive neurology. Neurología (English Edition) 32, 345354.Google Scholar
Biglan, KM, Holloway, RG, Mcdermott, MP, Richard, IH and Investigators, PSGC-P (2007) Risk factors for somnolence, edema, and hallucinations in early Parkinson disease. Neurology 69, 187195.Google Scholar
Borenstein, M, Hedges, LV, Higgins, J and Rothstein, HR (2009) Introduction to Meta-Analysis. UK: Wiley Online Library.Google Scholar
Calne, D, Snow, B and Lee, C (1992) Criteria for diagnosing Parkinson's disease. Annals of Neurology 32, 125127.Google Scholar
Celesia, GG and Wanamaker, WM (1972) Psychiatric disturbances in Parkinson's disease. Diseases of the Nervous System 33, 577583.Google Scholar
Chou, KL, Messing, S, Oakes, D, Feldman, PD, Breier, A and Friedman, JH (2005) Drug-induced psychosis in Parkinson disease: phenomenology and correlations among psychosis rating instruments. Clinical Neuropharmacology 28, 215219.Google Scholar
Collerton, D, Perry, E and Mckeith, I (2005) Why people see things that are not there: a novel perception and attention deficit model for recurrent complex visual hallucinations. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28, 737757.Google Scholar
Cope, T and Baguley, D (2009) Is musical hallucination an otological phenomenon? A review of the literature. Clinical Otolaryngology 34, 423430.Google Scholar
De Chazeron, I, Pereira, B, Chereau-Boudet, I, Brousse, G, Misdrahi, D, Fénelon, G, Tronche, A-M, Schwan, R, Lançon, C and Marques, A (2015) Validation of a Psycho-Sensory hAllucinations Scale (PSAS) in schizophrenia and Parkinson's disease. Schizophrenia Research 161, 269276.Google Scholar
De Lau, LM and Breteler, MM (2006) Epidemiology of Parkinson's disease. The Lancet Neurology 5, 525535.Google Scholar
De Maindreville, AD, Fénelon, G and Mahieux, F (2005) Hallucinations in Parkinson's disease: a follow-up study. Movement Disorders 20, 212217.Google Scholar
Diederich, NJ, Goetz, CG and Stebbins, GT (2005) Repeated visual hallucinations in Parkinson's disease as disturbed external/internal perceptions: focused review and a new integrative model. Movement Disorders 20, 130140.Google Scholar
Diederich, NJ, Fénelon, G, Stebbins, G and Goetz, CG (2009) Hallucinations in Parkinson disease. Nature Reviews Neurology 5, 331.Google Scholar
Dubois, B, Burn, D, Goetz, C, Aarsland, D, Brown, RG, Broe, GA, Dickson, D, Duyckaerts, C, Cummings, J and Gauthier, S (2007) Diagnostic procedures for Parkinson's disease dementia: recommendations from the movement disorder society task force. Movement Disorders 22, 23142324.Google Scholar
Eggermont, JJ (1990) On the pathophysiology of tinnitus; a review and a peripheral model. Hearing Research 48, 111123.Google Scholar
Ergün, U, Bozbas, A, Akin, Ü and Inan, L (2009) Musical hallucinations and Parkinson disease. The Neurologist 15, 150152.Google Scholar
Fénelon, G, Mahieux, F, Huon, R and Ziégler, M (2000) Hallucinations in Parkinson's disease: prevalence, phenomenology and risk factors. Brain 123, 733745.Google Scholar
Fénelon, G, Soulas, T, Zenasni, F and De Langavant, LC (2010) The changing face of Parkinson's disease-associated psychosis: a cross-sectional study based on the new NINDS-NIMH criteria. Movement Disorders 25, 763766.Google Scholar
Fernandez, W, Stern, G and Lees, A (1992) Hallucinations and parkinsonian motor fluctuations. Behavioural Neurology 5, 8386.Google Scholar
Friedman, JH, Agarwal, P, Alcalay, R, Black, K, Chou, K, Cote, L, Dayalu, P, Frank, S, Hartlein, J and Hauser, R (2011) Clinical vignettes in Parkinson's disease: a collection of unusual medication-induced hallucinations, delusions, and compulsive behaviours. International Journal of Neuroscience 121, 472476.Google Scholar
Gelb, DJ, Oliver, E and Gilman, S (1999) Diagnostic criteria for Parkinson disease. Archives of Neurology 56, 3339.Google Scholar
Gibb, W and Lees, A (1988) The relevance of the Lewy body to the pathogenesis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 51, 745752.Google Scholar
Goetz, CG and Stebbins, GT (1993) Risk factors for nursing home placement in advanced Parkinson's disease. Neurology 43, 22222222.Google Scholar
Goetz, C, Vogel, C, Tanner, CM and Stebbins, GT (1998) Early dopaminergic drug-induced hallucinations in parkinsonian patients. Neurology 51, 811814.Google Scholar
Goetz, CG, Wuu, J, Curgian, LM and Leurgans, S (2005) Hallucinations and sleep disorders in PD. Six-year prospective longitudinal study. Neurology 64, 8186.Google Scholar
Goetz, CG, Fan, W, Leurgans, S, Bernard, B and Stebbins, GT (2006) The malignant course of ‘benign hallucinations’ in Parkinson disease. Archives of Neurology 63, 713716.Google Scholar
Goetz, CG, Stebbins, GT and Ouyang, B (2011) Visual plus nonvisual hallucinations in Parkinson's disease: development and evolution over 10 years. Movement Disorders 26, 21962200.Google Scholar
Golden, EC and Josephs, KA (2015) Minds on replay: musical hallucinations and their relationship to neurological disease. Brain 138, 37933802.Google Scholar
Gondim, FDAA, Costa, HA, Taunay, TC, De Oliveira, GR, Ferreira, JM and Rola, FH (2010) Transient amantadine-induced musical hallucinations in a patient with Parkinson's disease. Movement Disorders 25, 15051506.Google Scholar
Gordon, A (1997) Insight into auditory hallucinations and psychosis. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 12, 410411.Google Scholar
Graham, JM, Grünewald, RA and Sagar, HJ (1997) Hallucinosis in idiopathic Parkinson's disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 63, 434440.Google Scholar
Grau-Rivera, O, Gelpi, E, Rey, M, Valldeoriola, F, Tolosa, E, Compta, Y and Martí, M (2013) Prominent psychiatric symptoms in patients with Parkinson's disease and concomitant argyrophilic grain disease. Journal of Neurology 260, 30023009.Google Scholar
Grossberg, S (2000) How hallucinations may arise from brain mechanisms of learning, attention, and volition. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 6, 583592.Google Scholar
Gupta, M, Singh, G, Khwaja, GA and Mehndiratta, M (2004) Hallucinations in Parkinson's disease – a study of forty three patients. JAPI 52, 703.Google Scholar
Harding, A, Broe, G and Halliday, G (2002) Visual hallucinations in Lewy body disease relate to Lewy bodies in the temporal lobe. Brain 125, 391403.Google Scholar
Hely, MA, Reid, WG, Adena, MA, Halliday, GM and Morris, JG (2008) The Sydney multicenter study of Parkinson's disease: the inevitability of dementia at 20 years. Movement Disorders 23, 837844.Google Scholar
Holroyd, S, Currie, L and Wooten, G (2001) Prospective study of hallucinations and delusions in Parkinson's disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 70, 734738.Google Scholar
Hughes, AJ, Daniel, SE, Kilford, L and Lees, AJ (1992) Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 55, 181184.Google Scholar
Inzelberg, R, Kipervasser, S and Korczyn, AD (1998) Auditory hallucinations in Parkinson's disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 64, 533535.Google Scholar
Katzen, H, Myerson, C, Papapetropoulos, S, Nahab, F, Gallo, B and Levin, B (2010) Multi-modal hallucinations and cognitive function in Parkinson's disease. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 30, 5156.Google Scholar
Klatka, LA, Louis, ED and Schiffer, RB (1996) Psychiatric features in diffuse Lewy body disease. A clinicopathologic study using Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease comparison groups. Neurology 47, 11481152.Google Scholar
Lai, SW, Liao, KF, Lin, CL, Lin, CC and Sung, FC (2014) Hearing loss may be a non-motor feature of Parkinson's disease in older people in Taiwan. European Journal of Neurology 21, 752757.Google Scholar
Langston, JW, Koller, W and Giron, L (1992) Etiology of Parkinson's disease. In Koller W (ed.), Handbook of Parkinson's Disease, 2nd Edn. New York: Marcel Dekker, pp. 369381.Google Scholar
Larsen, JP, Dupont, E and Tandberg, E (1994) Clinical diagnosis of Parkinson's disease. Proposal of diagnostic subgroups classified at different levels of confidence. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica 89, 242251.Google Scholar
Lee, AH and Weintraub, D (2012) Psychosis in Parkinson's disease without dementia: common and comorbid with other non-motor symptoms. Movement Disorders 27, 858863.Google Scholar
Lees, A, Clarke, C and Harrison, M (1977) Hallucinations after withdrawal of baclofen. The Lancet 309, 858.Google Scholar
Leu-Semenescu, S, De Cock, VC, Le Masson, VD, Debs, R, Lavault, S, Roze, E, Vidailhet, M and Arnulf, I (2011) Hallucinations in narcolepsy with and without cataplexy: contrasts with Parkinson's disease. Sleep Medicine 12, 497504.Google Scholar
Lin, FR, Niparko, JK and Ferrucci, L (2011) Hearing loss prevalence in the United States. Archives of Internal Medicine 171, 18511853.Google Scholar
Llorca, P, Pereira, B, Jardri, R, Chereau-Boudet, I, Brousse, G, Misdrahi, D, Fénelon, G, Tronche, A-M, Schwan, R and Lançon, C (2016) Hallucinations in schizophrenia and Parkinson's disease: an analysis of sensory modalities involved and the repercussion on patients. Scientific Reports 6, 38152.Google Scholar
Mack, J, Rabins, P, Anderson, K, Goldstein, S, Grill, S, Hirsch, ES, Lehmann, S, Little, JT, Margolis, RL and Palanci, J (2012) Prevalence of psychotic symptoms in a community-based Parkinson disease sample. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 20, 123132.Google Scholar
Mann, C (2003) Observational research methods. Research design II: cohort, cross sectional, and case-control studies. Emergency Medicine Journal 20, 5460.Google Scholar
Matsui, H, Nishinaka, K, Oda, M, Hara, N, Komatsu, K, Kubori, T and Udaka, F (2006) Hypoperfusion of the auditory and prefrontal cortices in Parkinsonian patients with verbal hallucinations. Movement Disorders 21, 21652169.Google Scholar
Mckeith, IG (2006) Consensus guidelines for the clinical and pathologic diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB): report of the Consortium on DLB International Workshop. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease 9, 417423.Google Scholar
Mckeith, , Fairbairn, A, Perry, R, Thompson, P and Perry, E (1992 a) Neuroleptic sensitivity in patients with senile dementia of Lewy body type. British Medical Journal 305, 673678.Google Scholar
Mckeith, , Perry, R, Fairbairn, A, Jabeen, S and Perry, E (1992 b) Operational criteria for senile dementia of Lewy body type (SDLT). Psychological Medicine 22, 911922.Google Scholar
Mckeith, IG, Galasko, D, Kosaka, K, Perry, E, Dickson, DW, Hansen, LA, Salmon, D, Lowe, J, Mirra, S and Byrne, E (1996) Consensus guidelines for the clinical and pathologic diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB). Report of the Consortium on DLB International Workshop. Neurology 47, 11131124.Google Scholar
Mckeith, IG, Boeve, BF, Dickson, DW, Halliday, G, Taylor, J-P, Weintraub, D, Aarsland, D, Galvin, J, Attems, J and Ballard, CG (2017) Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies Fourth consensus report of the DLB Consortium. Neurology 89, 88100.Google Scholar
Meppelink, AM, De Jong, BM, Renken, R, Leenders, KL, Cornelissen, FW and Van Laar, T (2009) Impaired visual processing preceding image recognition in Parkinson's disease patients with visual hallucinations. Brain 132, 29802993.Google Scholar
Mosimann, UP, Rowan, EN, Partington, CE, Collerton, D, Littlewood, E, O'Brien, JT, Burn, DJ and Mckeith, IG (2006) Characteristics of visual hallucinations in Parkinson disease dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 14, 153160.Google Scholar
Ohayon, MM (2000) Prevalence of hallucinations and their pathological associations in the general population. Psychiatry Research 97, 153164.Google Scholar
Onofrj, M, Thomas, A and Bonanni, L (2007) New approaches to understanding hallucinations in Parkinson's disease: phenomenology and possible origins. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics 7, 17311750.Google Scholar
Pacchetti, C, Manni, R, Zangaglia, R, Mancini, F, Marchioni, E, Tassorelli, C, Terzaghi, M, Ossola, M, Martignoni, E and Moglia, A (2005) Relationship between hallucinations, delusions, and rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder in Parkinson's disease. Movement Disorders 20, 14391448.Google Scholar
Paleacu, D, Schechtman, E and Inzelberg, R (2005) Association between family history of dementia and hallucinations in Parkinson disease. Neurology 64, 17121715.Google Scholar
Papapetropoulos, S, Katzen, H, Schrag, A, Singer, C, Scanlon, BK, Nation, D, Guevara, A and Levin, B (2008) A questionnaire-based (UM-PDHQ) study of hallucinations in Parkinson's disease. BMC Neurology 8, 21.Google Scholar
Perez, PA, Garcia-Antelo, MJ and Rubio-Nazabal, E (2017) ‘Doctor, I hear music’: a brief review about musical hallucinations. The Open Neurology Journal 11, 11.Google Scholar
Piggott, MA, Ballard, CG, Rowan, E, Holmes, C, Mckeith, IG, Jaros, E, Perry, RH and Perry, EK (2007) Selective loss of dopamine D2 receptors in temporal cortex in dementia with Lewy bodies, association with cognitive decline. Synapse 61, 903911.Google Scholar
Porteous, H and Ross, D (1956) Mental symptoms in parkinsonism following benzhexol hydrochloride therapy. British Medical Journal 2, 138.Google Scholar
Rizzo, G, Arcuti, S, Copetti, M, Alessandria, M, Savica, R, Fontana, A, Liguori, R and Logroscino, G (2017) Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 89, 358366.Google Scholar
Robson, E, Tweedy, C, Manzanza, N, Taylor, J-P, Atkinson, P, Randall, F, Reeve, A, Clowry, GJ and Lebeau, FE (2018) Impaired fast network oscillations and mitochondrial dysfunction in a mouse model of alpha-synucleinopathy (A30P). Neuroscience 377, 161173.Google Scholar
Sanchez-Castaneda, C, Rene, R, Ramirez-Ruiz, B, Campdelacreu, J, Gascon, J, Falcon, C, Calopa, M, Jauma, S, Juncadella, M and Junque, C (2010) Frontal and associative visual areas related to visual hallucinations in dementia with Lewy bodies and Parkinson's disease with dementia. Movement Disorders 25, 615622.Google Scholar
Sedley, W, Friston, KJ, Gander, PE, Kumar, S and Griffiths, TD (2016) An integrative tinnitus model based on sensory precision. Trends in Neurosciences 39, 799812.Google Scholar
Shea, YF, Ha, J and Chu, LW (2015) Comparisons of clinical symptoms in biomarker-confirmed Alzheimer's disease, dementia with Lewy bodies, and frontotemporal dementia patients in a local memory clinic. Psychogeriatrics 15, 235241.Google Scholar
Shine, JM, Halliday, GM, Naismith, SL and Lewis, SJ (2011) Visual misperceptions and hallucinations in Parkinson's disease: dysfunction of attentional control networks? Movement Disorders 26, 21542159.Google Scholar
Shinn-Cunningham, BG (2008) Object-based auditory and visual attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12, 182186.Google Scholar
Suárez-González, A, Serrano-Pozo, A, Arroyo-Anlló, EM, Franco-Macías, E, Polo, J, García-Solís, D and Gil-Néciga, E (2014) Utility of neuropsychiatric tools in the differential diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer's disease: quantitative and qualitative findings. International Psychogeriatrics 26, 453461.Google Scholar
Svetel, M, Smiljković, T, Pekmezović, T and Kostić, V (2012) Hallucinations in Parkinson's disease: cross-sectional study. Acta Neurologica Belgica 112, 3337.Google Scholar
Urwyler, P, Nef, T, Killen, A, Collerton, D, Thomas, A, Burn, D, Mckeith, I and Mosimann, UP (2014) Visual complaints and visual hallucinations in Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders 20, 318322.Google Scholar
Vann Jones, S and O'Brien, J (2014) The prevalence and incidence of dementia with Lewy bodies: a systematic review of population and clinical studies. Psychological Medicine 44, 673683.Google Scholar
Wand, AP (2012) Transient citalopram-induced auditory hallucinations in a patient with Parkinson's disease and depression. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 46, 178178.Google Scholar
Wang, H, Brozoski, TJ and Caspary, DM (2011) Inhibitory neurotransmission in animal models of tinnitus: maladaptive plasticity. Hearing Research 279, 111117.Google Scholar
Waters, F, Blom, J, Jardri, R, Hugdahl, K and Sommer, I (2018) Auditory hallucinations, not necessarily a hallmark of psychotic disorder. Psychological Medicine 48, 529536.Google Scholar
Weintraub, D, Chiang, C, Kim, HM, Wilkinson, J, Marras, C, Stanislawski, B, Mamikonyan, E and Kales, HC (2016) Association of antipsychotic use with mortality risk in patients with Parkinson disease. JAMA Neurology 73, 535541.Google Scholar
Williams, DR, Warren, JD and Lees, AJ (2008) Using the presence of visual hallucinations to differentiate Parkinson's disease from atypical parkinsonism. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 79, 652655.Google Scholar
Yýlmaz, S, Karalý, E, Tokmak, A, Güçlü, E, Koçer, A and Öztürk, Ö (2009) Auditory evaluation in Parkinsonian patients. European Archives of Oto-Rhino-Laryngology 266, 669671.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. PRISMA flow diagram demonstrating study selection process.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Forest plot showing random-effects model estimate of AH overall pooled prevalence of 11.9% (±7.9 to 17.7) in Lewy body disease (LBD = DLB, PDD and PDND combined). I2 = between-study heterogeneity; POP = study population; PREV = prevalence; LCL = lower confidence level; UCL = upper confidence level; WGHT = weight under random effects model.

Figure 2

Fig. 3. Forest plot showing random-effects model estimates of AH prevalence of 30.8% (±23.4 to 39.3) in DLB and 8.9% (±5.3 to 14.5) in PD.

Figure 3

Fig. 4. Forest plot showing random-effects model estimate of VH prevalence of 35.9% (±26.2 to 47.0) in LBD.

Figure 4

Fig. 5. Forest plot showing random-effects model estimates of VH prevalence of 61.8% (±49.1 to 73.0) in DLB and 28.2% (±19.1 to 39.5) in PD.

Supplementary material: File

Eversfield and Orton supplementary material

Eversfield and Orton supplementary material
Download Eversfield and Orton  supplementary material(File)
File 121.6 KB