Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T21:21:25.543Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Munda languages are father tongues, but Japanese and Korean are not

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 May 2020

Gyaneshwer Chaubey
Affiliation:
Department of Zoology, Benaras Hindu University, Varanasi, Uttar Pradesh221005, India
George van Driem*
Affiliation:
Linguistics Institute, University of Bern, Länggassstrasse 49, CH 3012Bern, Switzerland
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Over two decades ago, it was observed that the linguistic affinity of the language spoken by a particular population tended to correlate with the predominant paternal, i.e. Y-chromosomal, lineage found in that population. Such correlations were found to be ubiquitous but not universal, and the striking exceptions to such conspicuous patterns of correlation between linguistic and genetic phylogeography elicit particular interest and beg for clarification. Within the Austroasiatic language family, the Munda languages are a clear-cut case of father tongues, whereas Japanese and Korean are manifestly not. In this study, the cases of Munda and Japanese are juxtaposed. A holistic understanding of these contrasting cases of ethnolinguistic prehistory with respect to the father tongue correlation will first necessitate a brief exposition of the phylogeography of the Y chromosomal lineage O. Then triangulation discloses some contours and particulars of both long lost episodes of ethnolinguistic prehistory.

Type
Review
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Media summary: The origins of the Japanese and the origins of the Munda peoples of India provide contrasting cases of ethnolinguistic prehistory.

The uneasy relationship between language and the Y chromosome

The observation that the linguistic affinity of the language spoken by a particular population tends often to correlate with the predominant Y-chromosomal lineage found in that population was first pointed out by a Swiss–Italian team of geneticists (Poloni et al. Reference Poloni, Semino, Passarino, Santachiara-Benerecetti, Dupanloup, Langaney and Excoffier1997, Reference Poloni, Ray, Schneider, Langaney, Dessalles and Ghadakpour2000). As Lendering (Reference Lendering2010: 252) observed in his history of Alexander the Great, ‘As so often happened in the wars of antiquity, the widows married the murderers of their spouses’. Historical accounts record that the campaigns led by Genghis Khan, Tamerlane and other conquerors availed themselves of the same tactic, and Kivisild (Reference Kivisild2014) has wryly qualified warfare as a Y chromosome-linked pathology.

Although the motif of male genocide has repeated itself throughout history, conquering incursive groups might very well have been more clement in many particular instances, yet may nonetheless have benefitted from preferential access to the womenfolk and a more prolific siring of progeny by dint of élite dominance. As we have noted before, the preponderance of such correlations allows us to deduce that a mother teaching her children their father's tongue must have been a prevalent and recurrent pattern in linguistic prehistory. As a consequence of this social and demographic mechanism, paternally inherited polymorphisms often serve as tracers for linguistic dispersals in the past (van Driem Reference van Driem2007), and a particular Asian subset of such patterns of correlation forms the topic of the present paper.

Furthermore, the shallower time depth of the linguistically reconstructible past and the putative age of recognised language families match the time depth attributed to the most recent common ancestor of many geographically widespread paternal lineages (Zerjal et al. Reference Zerjal, Xue, Bertorelle, Spencer Wells, Bao, Zhu and Tyler-Smith2003; Balaresque et al. Reference Balaresque, Poulet, Cussat-Blanc, Gerard, Quintana-Murci, Heyer and Jobling2015). However, a few other forces are hypothesised to influence this topology equally (Karmin et al. Reference Karmin, Saag, Vicente, Wilson Sayres, Järve, Talas and Kivisild2015). In sharp contrast with our mitochondrial past and also in comparison with the rest of the genome, Y chromosomal phylogeography is relatively recent, having undergone a global bottleneck towards the end of the last ice age, when a subset of the then existing paternal clades began eradicating and out-competing other clades (Karmin et al. Reference Karmin, Saag, Vicente, Wilson Sayres, Järve, Talas and Kivisild2015; Silva et al. Reference Silva, Oliveira, Vieira, Brandão, Rito, Pereira and Soares2017). Effective palaeolithic founder populations of major modern paternal subclades were small. In terms of Y-chromosomal lineages, entire new populations arose from small surviving subsets that had passed through such bottlenecks. Similarly, many of today's language families and linguistic phyla appear to be the result of prehistoric bottlenecks.

When languages and genes happen to exhibit a correlation, such a relationship ought not to be confused with identity, and a chromosomal marker should not be simplistically equated with populations speaking languages of a particular linguistic phylum. Rather, markers on the Y chromosome serve as proxies or tracers for the movements of paternal ancestors. Although ubiquitous, the father tongue correlation is not universal. Exceptions to the father tongue correlation, such as Hungary and Baltistan (van Driem Reference van Driem2007), are not unique cases. Rather, the meticulous study of correlations of genetic polymorphisms with the geographical distribution of language families and their constituent subclades as well as the discrepancies between genetic and linguistic phylogeography, nowadays enriched with the information rendered available through whole genome studies and ancient DNA findings, are providing us with an increasingly differentiated view of the past with ever greater detail.

The East Asian linguistic phylum

The East Asian linguistic phylum was proposed by Starosta in Périgueux in 2001, one year before his death. His thinking was published posthumously, shown in Figure 1 (Starosta Reference Starosta, Sagart, Blench and Sanchez-Mazas2005; van Driem Reference van Driem, Sagart, Blench and Sanchez-Mazas2005). In proposing to unite the Kradai, Austronesian, Trans-Himalayan, Hmong-Mien and Austroasiatic language families into a single East Asian linguistic phylum, Starosta had numerous precursors. Conrady (Reference Conrady1916, Reference Conrady1922) and Wulff (Reference Wulff1934, Reference Wulff1942) proposed a linguistic phylum consisting of Austroasiatic, Austronesian, Kradai and Trans-Himalayan, whilst Benedict (Reference Benedict1942), Blust (Reference Blust and Goodenough1996) and Peiros (Reference Peiros1998) proposed an Austric superfamily comprising Austroasiatic, Austronesian, Kradai and possibly Hmong-Mien. The more modest proposal to unite just two of these five East Asian language families, viz. Kradai and Austronesian, was first advanced by Schlegel (Reference Schlegel1901, Reference Schlegel1902) and then seconded by Benedict (Reference Benedict1942). However, the first sound historical comparative evidence for the Austro-Tai family was adduced by Ostapirat (Reference Ostapirat, Sagart, Blench and Sanchez-Mazas2005, Reference Ostapirat2013).

Figure 1. The enhanced 2012 Benares recension of Starosta's East Asian linguistic phylum (Starosta Reference Starosta, Sagart, Blench and Sanchez-Mazas2005; van Driem Reference van Driem, Jamir and Hazarika2014).

For his grander East Asian linguistic phylum, Starosta adduced the putative shared morphological vestiges of an agentive prefix *<m->, patient suffix *<-n>, instrumental prefix <s-> and perfective prefix *<n->. A discussion of the merits of this evidence strikes us as being of little utility, since we consider the antiquity of the proposed linguistic phylum to lie at the ‘linguistic event horizon’ or maximal time depth reconstructible through methodologically sound historical linguistic comparison. Beyond this epistemological boundary hypotheses regarding long-distance linguistic relationship are reduced to sheer speculation. Rather, Starosta (Reference Starosta, Sagart, Blench and Sanchez-Mazas2005: 194) modestly proposed that the ‘potential utility’ of his hypothesis lay ‘in helping to focus scholars’ efforts on particular specific questions, resulting in the replacement of parts of this hypothesis with better supported arguments’. Following Ostapirat, Starosta's East Asian linguistic phylum may be construed to comprise four recognised language families: Austro-Tai, Trans-Himalayan, Hmong-Mien and Austroasiatic.

The Story of O

Populations today speaking languages of the Trans-Himalayan, Hmong-Mien, Austroasiatic and Austro-Tai language families happen to be characterised by a preponderance of the paternal lineage O. In fact, each of the four language families are characterised by a particular subclade of O, suggesting both a paternal spread of these language families and a time depth for the putative East Asian language family coeval with the antiquity of the paternal haplogroup O itself. There is good reason to believe that the geographical locus of the ancestral haplogroup NO (M214) lay in the eastern Himalayan region, where the two paternal lineages N and O split up (Karmin et al. Reference Karmin, Saag, Vicente, Wilson Sayres, Järve, Talas and Kivisild2015; Ilumäe et al. Reference Ilumäe, Reidla, Chukhryaeva, Järve, Post, Karmin and Rootsi2016; McColl et al. Reference McColl, Racimo, Vinner, Demeter, Gakuhari, Víctor Moreno-Mayar and Willerslev2018).

We previously identified the clade N (M231) with the paternal spread of Fortescue's Uralo-Siberian linguistic phylum (van Driem Reference van Driem, Jamir and Hazarika2014). The bearers of haplogroup N set out for East Asia just after the Last Glacial Maximum, braving ice and tundra, and in a grand counterclockwise sweep, gradually migrated across northern Eurasia as far west as Lappland (Rootsi et al. Reference Rootsi, Zhivotovsky, Baldovič, Kayser, Kutuev, Khusainova and Villems2007; Derenko et al. Reference Derenko, Malyarchuk, Denisova, Wozniak, Grzybowski, Dambueva and Zakharov2007; Mirabal et al. Reference Mirabal, Reguiero, Cadenas, Cavalli-Sforza, Underhill, Verbenko and Herrera2009; Ilumäe et al. Reference Ilumäe, Reidla, Chukhryaeva, Järve, Post, Karmin and Rootsi2016), whilst the ancestral form *N may originally have been situated in northern Burma, Yúnnán and Sìchuān. Recent genetic studies have provided evidence that corroborates our earlier linguistic conjecture that the westward spread of this linguistic phylum across northern Eurasia must have involved the linguistic assimilation of earlier populations already residing in Siberia (Tambets et al. Reference Tambets, Yunusbayev, Hudjashov, Ilumäe, Rootsi, Honkola and Metspalu2018; Lamnidis et al. Reference Lamnidis, Majander, Jeong, Salmela, Wessman and Vyacheslav Moiseyev2018; Günther et al. Reference Günther, Malmström, Svensson, Omrak, Kılınç and Jakobsson2018; Saag et al. Reference Saag, Laneman, Varul, Malve, Valk, Razzak and Tambets2019).

The dissemination of Y chromosomal haplogroup O (M175) throughout East Asia from the eastern Himalayan region, as temperature and humidity increased after the Last Glacial Maximum, has been recounted in detail before (van Driem Reference van Driem, Jamir and Hazarika2014) and will be recapitulated here in updated form. The entirely non-random correlation of subclades of this particular paternal lineage with populations speaking languages of the Trans-Himalayan, Austro-Tai, Hmong-Mien and Austroasiatic families enables us to infer the following sequence of events. Before the end of the Last Glacial Maximum, the paternal lineage O (M175) split into the subclades O2 (M122) and O1 (F265, M1354), as shown in Figure 2. The two subclades can be putatively assigned to two geographical loci, with the haplogroup O1 (F265, M1354) moving eastward into East Asia south of the Yangtze, whilst bearers of the O2 (M122) haplogroup settled in the eastern Himalayan region. Subsequently, over the course of time, as temperature and humidity increased after the last glacial maximum, haplogroup O split further into the paternal lineages that serve as tracers for the spread of Trans-Himalayan, Hmong-Mien, Austroasiatic and Austro-Tai.

Figure 2. After the Last Glacial Maximum, the Y chromosomal haplogroup O (M175) split into the subclades O1 (F265, M1354) and O2 (M122).

The O1 (F265, M1354) lineage south of the Yangtze split into the subclades O1b (M268) and O1a (M119), with the latter moving eastward to the Fújiàn hill tracts and across the strait to settle on Formosa, which so became the Urheimat of the Austronesians. The founding dispersal of the Austro-Tai language family can be traced through a correlation of the current geographical range of Austro-Tai languages with the chronology and spread of the molecular proxies defining the paternal haplogroups O1b (M268) and O1a (M119). Just as has long been thought (Skeat and Blagden Reference Skeat and Blagden1906), Asian negritos reflect an older layer of peopling (Chaubey and Endicott Reference Chaubey and Endicott2013; Jinam et al. Reference Jinam, Phipps, Aghakhanian, Majumder, Datar, Stoneking and Saitou2017), and the peoples of insular Southeast Asia represent a patchwork quilt resulting from layers of peopling, with the Austronesian expansion from Formosa overlaying earlier strata of peopling (Mörseburg et al. Reference Mörseburg, Pagani, Ricaut, Yngvadottir, Harney, Castillo and Kivisild2016).

Recently, an exploration of Southeast Asian prehistory (Lipson et al. Reference Lipson, Cheronet, Mallick, Rohland, Oxenham, Pietrusewsky and Reich2018) was quickly superseded by a more detailed study (McColl et al. Reference McColl, Racimo, Vinner, Demeter, Gakuhari, Víctor Moreno-Mayar and Willerslev2018). The findings of both studies are commensurate with the model of a linguistic dispersal emanating from Formosa through insular Southeast Asia to the Southeast Asian mainland, Madagascar and Oceania, for which the geographical spread of the paternal lineage O1a (M119) serves as a molecular tracer. In this context, the Papuan ancestry in the Southwest Pacific appears to reflect a layer of peopling more recent than the initial population. The bearers of the Papuan ancestral component arrived either in belated emulation of the original Austronesian seafarers or as part of later waves of mixed migration (Skoglund et al. Reference Skoglund, Posth, Sirak, Spriggs, Valentin, Bedford and Reich2016).

Subsequent to the split-up of the paternal lineage O1 (F265, M1354) into the subclades O1b (M268) and O1a (M119), the paternal subclade O1b (M268) gave rise to the filial subclades O1b2 (M176) and O1b1a1a (M95). The bearers of haplogroup O1b1a1a (M95) became the progenitors of the Austroasiatics (van Driem Reference van Driem2007). The Austroasiatics spread throughout the Salween drainage and thence to southern Yúnnán, northern Thailand and western Laos. In time, the Austroasiatics would spread as far as the Mekong delta, the Malay peninsula and the Nicobars, and their paternal lineage would also spread deep into insular Southeast Asia. However, the prominent paternal lineage O1b2 (M176), previously referred to as ‘para-Austroasiatic’, does not appear to be correlated with any extant linguistic group today (Figure 3).

Figure 3. Paternal lineages branching into new subclades. Each event involved a linguistic bottleneck leading to language families that today are reconstructible as distinct linguistic phyla. The O1 (F265, M1354) lineage gave rise to the O1a (M119) subclade, which moved eastward to the Fújiàn hill tracts and across the strait to Formosa, which so became the Urheimat of the Austronesians. Bearers of the O1b (M268) paternal lineage domesticated Asian rice. Bearers of O2a2a1a2 (M7) became the Proto-Hmong-Mien. In the Eastern Himalaya, the bearers of haplogroup O2a2b1 (M134) expanded and became the Trans-Himalayans. Haplogroup O1b1a1a (M95) is the Proto-Austroasiatic paternal lineage. The para-Austroasiatic fraternal clade O1b2 (M176) spread eastward, sowing seed along the way.

The spread of haplogroup O1 (F265, M1354) reflects the paternal founding dispersals of both Austro-Tai and Austroasiatic as well as the geographical spread of a para-Austroasiatic paternal subclade that evidently left no modern linguistic descendants. Our data from the Himalayan region and the data from populations elsewhere in Asia indicate that the geographical range and the chronology of spread of haplogroup O2a2b1 (M134) trace the founding dispersal of the Trans-Himalayan language family, whereas the paternal lineage O2a2a1a2 (M7) serves as a molecular proxy for the founding and spread of Hmong-Mien.

About 12,000 years ago, at the dawn of the Holocene, in the southeastern Himalayas and eastern slopes of the Tibetan Plateau, haplogroup O2 (M122) gave rise to the ancestral Trans-Himalayan paternal lineage O2a2b1 (M134) and the ‘Yangtzean’ or Hmong-Mien paternal lineage O2a2a1a2 (M7). The bearers of the polymorphism O2a2b1 (M134) at first remained in the Eastern Himalaya, which today continues to represent the centre of phylogenetic and linguistic diversity of the Trans-Himalayan language family based on the geographical distribution of primary linguistic subgroups. Subsequently, after bearers of the O2a2a1a2 (M7) lineage migrated eastward to settle in areas south of the Yangtze, they were followed by early Trans-Himalayan language communities that spread from northeastern India into southeastern Tibet and northern Burma.

Austroasiatic seafarers set sail for the Subcontinent

The unity of Austroasiatic as a linguistic family has always been in evidence ever since Mason (Reference Mason1854), even though this linguistic finding flew in the face of the pronounced phenotypical disparity readily observed between Austroasiatic language communities. Schmidt (Reference Schmidt1906) proposed the idea of an Austroasiatic ‘race’, but Blagden (Reference Blagden1909) rejected both Schmidt's Austric theory and assailed his notion of an Austroasiatic ‘racial stock’, stressing instead the demonstrable linguistic unity of the Austroasiatic language family.

Whether or not there is a thin strain of common blood running through these very diverse races is a point that does not and cannot affect the classification of their languages. Personally I rather regret that the attempt has been made to establish even a qualified racial unity such as this amongst populations which differ physically amongst themselves as much as chalk does from cheese. Not only is it in my judgement premature inasmuch as the data available are quite inadequate to support the conclusion, but it is liable to do harm by casting doubt on the validity of the purely linguistic inferences, where the evidence is far more perfect. (Blagden Reference Blagden1909: 172)

Schmidt's adversary in Vienna, Robert von-Heine-Geldern (Reference von Heine-Geldern1920), likewise dismissed his Austroasiatic ‘race’ as untenable, and hastened to point out that the phenotypical diversity between populations speaking Austroasiatic languages contrasted with the conspicuous lack of quantifiable phenotypical differences that could be observed ‘zwischen den Austroasiaten, Tibeto-Birmanen und Siamo-Chinesen Birmas, Assams und der Chittagong Hill Tracts’. The history of science has now partly vindicated both Schmidt and his rivals, for, whilst there is no such thing as ‘race’, let alone any such thing as an Austroasiatic ‘race’, the once mooted ‘thin strain of common blood running through these very diverse races’ has actually been found in the shape of molecular genetic evidence indicating that, secondarily, male Austroasiatics introduced both their language and their paternal lineage, O1b1a1a (M95), to the indigenous peoples of the Choṭā Nāgpur.

We first showed (Chaubey et al. Reference Chaubey, Metspalu, Choi, Mägi, Romero, Rootsi and Kivisild2011) that the Munda branch of Austroasiatic had arisen as the result of a sexually biased linguistic intrusion into the Indian subcontinent from Southeast Asia, and our findings have been corroborated by subsequent studies (Arunkumar et al. Reference Arunkumar, Tatarinova, Duty, Rollo, Syama and Arun2015; Metspalu et al. Reference Metspalu, Mondal and Chaubey2018; Tätte et al. Reference Tätte, Pagani, Pathak, Kõks, Duy, Ho and Metspalu2019). As a consequence of the comparatively younger date and the highly pronounced gender asymmetry of this linguistic intrusion, it appears that the deepest division within the Khasi-Aslian trunk of Austroasiatic, i.e. the split between Khasi-Pakanic and Mon-Khmer, would be indicative of the geographical location of the Austroasiatic homeland, rather than the split between Munda and Khasi-Aslian. Therefore, the point of dispersal for Khasi-Aslian would appear to have lain in the area between South Asia proper and mainland Southeast Asia proper.

Rau and Sidwell (Reference Rau and Sidwell2019) have advanced the daring Munda maritime hypothesis, proposing that the male linguistic ancestors of the Munda migrated by sea to the Orissan coast from the Tenasserim or the Isthmus of Kra or even from the South China Sea littoral of mainland Southeast Asia and thence through the Straits of Malacca to the Indian subcontinent. Their hypothesis is inspired by Chaubey et al. (Reference Chaubey, Metspalu, Choi, Mägi, Romero, Rootsi and Kivisild2011), but their linguistic data are gossamer and limited to a few lexemes pertaining to rice, millet agriculture and livestock. A new genetic study (Tätte et al. Reference Tätte, Pagani, Pathak, Kõks, Duy, Ho and Metspalu2019) has lent support to this bold hypothesis in that the Munda show the highest sharing of identity-by-descent segments with Austroasiatic tribal groups on the Malay peninsula. The presence at Orissan coastal sites of knobbed and rouletted ware, which formed part of the maritime trade between South and Southeast Asia, is likewise suggestive (Tripati et al. Reference Tripati, Prabhu, Patnaik, Acharya, Mishra, Behera and Joshi2019). Based on these findings, we would suggest that it might prove fruitful to compare Munda more particularly with Proto-Nico-Monic.

However, a new paper (Singh et al. Reference Singh, Vishwakarma, Gazi Sultana, Pilvar, Karmin, Rootsi and Chaubeyforthcoming), which specifically examines Austroasiatic populations of the Subcontinent, has identified three founding paternal lineages common to both Khasi and Munda speaking populations. Yet the number of Khasi individuals analysed was small, and more Khasi individuals will need to be analysed. Figure 4 therefore depicts two versions of the male-biased Austroasiatic linguistic intrusion that established Munda languages in India, the northern trajectory originally proposed by Chaubey et al. (Reference Chaubey, Metspalu, Choi, Mägi, Romero, Rootsi and Kivisild2011) and the new southern Munda maritime exodus, with the Nicobar archipelago shown lying squarely in the path of the seafarers’ course. Both arrows of migration depict Munda languages as father tongues, whether brought to India by men from the Meghālaya or spawned by Austroasiatic seamen.

Figure 4. Two versions are depicted of the male-biased migration which introduced Austroasiatic language and four O1b1a1a1 (M95) paternal lineages to indigenous populations of the Choṭā Nāgpur, the overland trek from the Meghālaya originally proposed by Chaubey et al. (Reference Chaubey, Metspalu, Choi, Mägi, Romero, Rootsi and Kivisild2011) and the Munda maritime migration proposed by Rau and Sidwell (Reference Rau and Sidwell2019).

The population genetics of Asian rice has suggested three distinct domestication events involving the ahu, indica and japonica rice cultivars (Londo et al. Reference Londo, Chiang, Hung, Chiang and Schaal2006; McNally et al. Reference McNally, Child, Bohnert, Davidson, Zhao, Ulat and Leach2009; Civáň et al. Reference Civáň, Craig, Cox and Brown2015). Based on linguistic palaeontological evidence (van Driem Reference van Driem, Robbeets and Savelyev2017), the linguistic ancestors of the Austroasiatics and the Hmong-Mien or ‘Yangtzeans’ were identified as the people behind two of these three domestications. The hypothesis was argued that the ‘para-Austroasiatic’ bearers of Y-chromosomal haplogroup O1b2 (M176), might have been the actors behind a third domestication. A large rice population genetic (Wang et al. Reference Wang, Yan, Qin, Lu and Ding2013) has meanwhile lent additional support to our reconstruction involving several distinct rice domestications. Bearers of the para-Austroasiatic paternal lineage advanced as far as the Korean peninsula and became a major contributor to the Japanese genome, representing the probable paternal lineage of the Yayoi people, who introduced rice agriculture to the Japanese archipelago, as early as the second millennium BC, during the final phase of the Jōmon period.

The story of O continues

Intimate interaction between ancient Austroasiatics and the ancestral Hmong-Mien not only involved the sharing of knowledge about rice agriculture, but also left a genetic trace in the high frequency of haplogroup O1b1a1a (M95) in modern Hmong-Mien populations and of haplogroup O2a2a1a2 (M7) in Austroasiatic populations. Further support for our 2017 rice domestication model has been provided by the most recent study of mainland Southeast Asia, where early Mesolithic male and female demographic expansions are reported to have taken place 10,000 years ago, whereas subsequent population prehistory shows contrasting male and female genetic histories. A major male-specific expansion involving Y-chromosomal haplogroup O1b1a1a (M95) took place in the Neolithic period between 5000 and 4000 years ago, reflected in modern Austroasiatic language communities. A second major male-specific expansion transpired during the Bronze and Iron Age between 2500 and 2000 years ago, involving Y-chromosomal haplogroup O2a (M324), seen in modern Kradai language communities in this region (Kutanan et al. Reference Kutanan, Kampuansai, Changmai, Flegontov, Schröder, Macholdt and Stoneking2018a, Reference Kutanan, Kampuansai, Brunelli, Ghirotto, Pittayaporn, Ruangchai and Stonekingb, Reference Kutanan, Kampuansai, Srikummool, Brunelli, Ghirotto, Arias and Stoneking2019).

Meanwhile, bearers of Y chromosomal haplogroup O2a2b1 (M134) in the eastern Himalayan region expanded eastward throughout Sìchuān and Yúnnán, north and northwest across the Tibetan plateau as well as westward across the Himalayas and southward into the Indo-Burmese borderlands. On the Brahmaputra plain, Trans-Himalayans encountered the Austroasiatics, who had preceded them. The relative frequencies of the Y chromosomal haplogroup O1b1a1a (M95) in Bodo-Koch language communities (Sahoo et al. Reference Sahoo, Singh, Himabindu, Banerjee, Sitalaximi, Gaikwad and Kashyap2006; Reddy et al. Reference Reddy, Langstieh, Kumar, Nagaraja, Reddy, Aruna and Singh2007) suggest that these Trans-Himalayan populations of the Indian subcontinent included and assimilated male Austroasiatic speakers in the past.

Finally, the Trans-Himalayan paternal lineage O2a2b1 (M134) spread northeast to the North China plain. The complex history of Sinitic populations featured successive constellations of dynastic empires governed from geographically ever shifting capitals, whereby subjugated and neighbouring populations as well as immigrants were absorbed. Consequently, Hàn Chinese populations comprise an amalgam of East Asian paternal lineages. Even in modern Hàn Chinese populations, however, the molecular marker associated with the spread of a Trans-Himalayan father tongue from the eastern Himalayan region, i.e. haplogroup O2a2b1 (M134) and its subclade O2a2b1a1 (M117), occurs in a much higher frequency than any other O haplogroup subclade, and approximately twice as frequently as the next most frequent fraternal subclade O2a1c (002611) (Yan et al. Reference Yan, Wang, Li, Li and Jin2011; Wang et al. Reference Wang, Yan, Qin, Lu and Ding2013; Yao et al. Reference Yao, Tang, Yao, Yeh, Zhang, Xie and Wang2017b).

In autosomal terms, the Hàn ethnicity arose through incessant gene flow within successive dynastic empires, with their geographically ever shifting centres (Chiang et al. Reference Chiang, Mangul, Robles and Sankararaman2018). Hamada (Reference Hamada2006) has shown how local and private concerns with regard to ethnic identity have been projected onto the past and thus distorted the interpretation of anthropological, archaeological and linguistic data in the Japanese context. The same may continue to be said today of laymen and even scholars projecting modern Hàn ethnic identity onto the past despite the admonitions of the eminent Chinese archaeologist Kwang-chih Chang (Reference Chang1986) to avoid the anachronisms that arise from applying the label ‘Chinese’ to archaeological cultural assemblages or peoples of the distant past.

The centre of Trans-Himalayan linguistic diversity lies in the eastern Himalayan region, more particularly on the southern flanks of the great Himalayan divide, where most of the languages of the family and over three-quarters of all currently recognised primary linguistic subgroups of the family are found. The aberrant nature of some Sinitic lexicons has long indicated to the minds of many an historical linguist that Sinitic must have arisen through creolisation when an ancient Trans-Himalayan speaking population first moved to the already populated North China plain (Poppe Reference Poppe1965; Benedict Reference Benedict1972; Hashimoto Reference Hashimoto1976a, Reference Hashimoto1976b, Reference Hashimoto1980, Reference Hashimoto, McCoy and Light1986; Ballard Reference Ballard1979; Norman Reference Norman1982; Comrie Reference Comrie, Djamouri, Meisterernst and Sybesma2008; DeLancey Reference DeLancey and Jing-Schmidt2011). Genetic data have newly begun to lend support to this linguistic hypothesis (He et al. Reference He, Chen, Wu, Luo, Gao, Wang and Jia2019).

Findings of ancient DNA studies in this regard form the topic of one of our forthcoming studies. Much later, at the far shallower time depth of the Qín dynasty in the third century BC, this ethnicity spread southward from the Yellow River basin into southern China (Wen et al. Reference Wen, Hui, Daru, Xiufeng, Feng, Yungang and Li2004), where this martial and male-biased historical spread during the cultural sinification of the region south of the Yangtze involved both the spread of language and the introduction of paternal lineages, as historically documented in the Chinese chronicles.

The peopling of Japan

Our original reconstruction of the peopling of Japan (van Driem Reference van Driem, Jamir and Hazarika2014), based on earlier genetic research (Kivisild et al. Reference Kivisild, Tolk, Parik, Wang, Papiha, Bandelt and Villems2002; Tajima et al. Reference Tajima, Hayami, Tokunaga, Juji, Matsuo, Marzuki and Horai2004; Tanaka et al. Reference Tanaka, Cabrera, González, Larruga, Takeyasu, Fuku and Shimodaira2004; Hammer et al. Reference Hammer, Karafet, Park, Omoto, Harihara, Stoneking and Horai2006; Jin et al. Reference Jin, Tyler-Smith and Kim2009; Karafet et al. Reference Karafet, Zegura, Hammer, Peregrine, Peiros and Feldman2009), has been borne out – and can now also be fleshed out – by additional work on modern Japanese DNA (Mabuchi et al. Reference Mabuchi, Susukida, Kido and Oya2007; Nonaka et al. Reference Nonaka, Minaguchi and Takezaki2007; Yamaguchi-Kabata et al. Reference Yamaguchi-Kabata, Nakazono, Takahashi, Saito, Hosono, Kubo and Kamatani2008; Nohira et al. Reference Nohira, Maruyama and Minaguchi2008; Pen and Zhang Reference Peng and Zhang2012; Poznik et al. Reference Poznik, Xue, Mendez, Willems, Massaia, Wilson Sayres and Tyler-Smith2016) and on ancient Japanese and East Asian DNA (Shinoda Reference Shinoda2004; Xue et al. Reference Xue, Zerjal, Bao, Zhu, Shu, Xu and Tyler-Smith2006; Shinoda and Doi Reference Shinoda and Doi2008; Adachi et al. Reference Adachi, Shinoda, Umetsu and Matsumura2009, Reference Adachi, Shinoda, Umetsu, Kitano, Matsumura, Fujiyama and Tanaka2011, Reference Adachi, Sawada, Yoneda, Kobayashi and Itoh2013; Igawa et al. Reference Igawa, Manabe, Oyamada, Kitagawa, Kato, Ikematsu and Rokutanda2009; Kim et al. Reference Kim, Kim, Shin, Jin, Kwak, Han and Kim2011; Jinam et al. Reference Jinam, Nishida, Hirai, Kawamura, Oota, Umetsu and Saitou2012, Reference Jinam, Kanzawa-Kiriyama and Saitou2015; Kanzawa-Kiriyama et al. Reference Kanzawa-Kiriyama, Saso, Suwa and Saitou2013; Trejaut et al. Reference Trejaut, Poloni, Yen, Lai, Loo, Lee and Lin2014; Nakagome et al. Reference Nakagome, Sato, Ishida, Hanihara, Yamaguchi and Kimura2015; Yao et al. Reference Yao, Wang, Wang, Tao, Shang, Wen and Li2017a, Reference Yao, Tang, Bian, Wu, Chen and Wangc; Saitou & Jinam Reference Saitou and Jinam2017; Adachi et al. Reference Adachi, Kakuda, Takahashi, Kanzawa-Kiriyama and Shinoda2018; Watanabe et al. Reference Watanabe, Naka, Khor, Sawai, Hitomi, Tokunaga and Ohashi2019; Gakuhari et al. Reference Gakuhari, Nakagome, Rasmussen, Allentoft, Sato, Korneliussen and Oota2019). The synoptic reconstruction presented here and in Figure 5 embodies a number of hypotheses, which may be corroborated or refuted by future ancient DNA findings, or perhaps just require minor reformulation.

Figure 5. In successive waves, the paternal lineages D1a2 (M55), C1a1 (M8), C2 (M217) and O1b2 (M176) migrated from the East Asian mainland to the Japanese archipelago at the dawn of the Palaeolithic, the Incipient Jōmon, the Early Jōmon and the Yayoi period respectively.

Palaeolithic hunter–foragers bearing the paternal lineage D1a2 (M55) and speaking a language ancestral to Ainu settled Japan about 38,000 years ago, bringing with them the oldest Palaeolithic tools now found in the archipelago. This paternal lineage is retained throughout Japan and particularly survives in a high frequency on the Ryūkyū islands and in a very high frequency of over 80% amongst the Ainu of Hokkaidō, whom ethnographic accounts have described as hirsute and phenotypically distinct from other Japanese. Palaeolithic settlements on the main Japanese islands appear over 34,000 years ago (Mizoguchi Reference Mizoguchi2002, Reference Mizoguchi2014). In the Ryūkyū archipelago, palaeolithic settlements on the Amami and Okinawa island groups are likewise attested from 34,000 years ago. On the Sakishima islands, palaeolithic cultural assemblages in the Miyako and Yaeyama island groups are attested from 27,000 years ago. In the Amami and Okinawa island groups, archaeological strata reflecting the Early Shellmound period begin at about 9000 years ago, whereas in the southern portion of the Ryūkyū archipelago the archaeological strata identified as reflecting the enigmatic Shimotabaru period begin about 4900 years ago (Pearson Reference Pearson2013; Akamine Reference Akamine2017).

Philip von Siebold (Reference von Siebold and Leupe1858) argued that the ancestors of the Ainu had originated in the Amur basin at a time that preceded the advent of Japonic speakers, whose subsequent arrival to the archipelago compelled the Ainu to migrate northward (von Siebold Reference von Siebold and Leupe1858: 380). The toponymical studies of Chamberlain (Reference Chamberlain1887) and Batchelor (Reference Batchelor1925) showed that most old place names on Hokkaidō and numerous place names in northern and central Honshū were Ainu toponyms, with those ending in -betsu or -be [<Ainu pet ‘river’] and in -nai [<Ainu nai ‘stream’] being amongst the most conspicuous.

Subsequently, bearers of the paternal lineage C1a1 (M8) arrived in the archipelago and introduced the Incipient Jōmon culture, typified by early ceramic cultures such as the Ōdai Yamamoto i site. This paternal lineage is borne by 10% of modern Japanese men. At the dawn of the Early Jōmon period, bearers of the paternal lineage C2 (M217) arrived in Japan speaking the ancient Japonic language, which ultimately gave rise to modern Japanese and the Ryūkyūan dialects. This paternal lineage is borne by 6% of modern Japanese men.

Kæmpfer (Reference Kæmpfer1729: 63–65) identified the Amur river basin as the Altaic homeland, whence the linguistic ancestors of the Japanese had migrated to the archipelago via the Korean peninsula, an idea also espoused by latter-day linguists, e.g. van Driem (Reference van Driem2001) and Robbeets (Reference Robbeets, Robbeets and Bisang2014). The dual nature of Japanese population structure was advanced by Miller (Reference Miller1971), who proposed that the resident Jōmon population spoke an Altaic language ancestral to modern Japanese, and this Altaic tongue underwent Austronesian influence when the islanders absorbed the bearers of the incursive Yayoi culture. The Altaic linguistic phylum comprises Japonic, Korean, Tungusic, Mongolic and Turkic, but Robbeets (Reference Robbeets, Robbeets and Bisang2014) reserves the label ‘Altaic’ for a putative clade which she believes comprises Tungusic, Mongolic and Turkic, and introduced the new label ‘Trans-Eurasian’ for the linguistic phylum traditionally called Altaic (Blažek et al. Reference Blažek, Schwarz and Srba2019).

About 3000 years ago, the bearers of the O1b2 (M176) paternal lineage came to Japan from the Korean peninsula, introducing rice cultivation and appearing archaeologically as the Yayoi. In their wake, bearers of other O haplogroup subclades prevalent on adjacent portions of the East Asian mainland also migrated to the archipelago. In time, the Yayoi sedentary agricultural lifestyle prevailed, and Yayoi paternal lineages came to predominate and today account for over half of all Japanese paternal lineages, with the highest frequencies in Kyūshū. Yet the gracile Yayoi newcomers with their farming subsistence strategy, notwithstanding their superior bronze and iron metallurgy, were evidently motivated or compelled to assimilate linguistically to the robust Japonic speakers already on the island.

The Japonic-speaking Early Jōmon people must have been drawn in to avail themselves of the pickings of Yayoi agricultural yields, and the Yayoi may have prospered and succeeded in multiplying their paternal lineages precisely because they managed to accommodate the Jōmon linguistically and in material ways. In addition to rice, the Yayoi introduced other crops of continental inspiration to the Japanese archipelago such as millet, wheat and melons. Their ancestors had certainly encountered these crops on their way northward to the Korean peninsula, the earliest attested domestic millet dating from before 6000 BC at Xīnglōnggōu near Chìfēng, where a Neolithic culture without sickles has been described (Zhào Reference Zhào2005).

The original ‘para-Austroasiatic’ tongue of the Yayoi was lost except perhaps for loan words denoting agricultural terms (cf. Benedict Reference Benedict1990). Notwithstanding a possible Austronesian presence in the Sakishima islands from Formosa at the end of the third millennium BC (Hudson Reference Hudson, Piper, Matsumura and Bulbeck2017), any alleged ‘Austronesian’ influence on Japonic (Polivanov Reference Polivanov1918, Reference Polivanov1924; van Hinloopen Labberton Reference van Hinloopen Labberton1924, Reference van Hinloopen Labberton1925; Whymant, Reference Whymant1926; Benedict Reference Benedict1990) would have had to antedate the arrival of the Yayoi in Japan, deriving from the Lóngshān interaction sphere connecting the Dàwènkǒu culture of Shāndōng with Formosa and other coastal cultures, e.g. Qīngliángǎng in northern Jiāngsū, Mǎjiābāng in the Yangtze delta.

The legacy of lost father tongues and the spread of agriculture

We previously took issue with the hypothesis that the founding dispersals of language families coincided with the spread of agriculture in a volume edited by Bellwood and Renfrew, two staunch proponents of this very hypothesis (van Driem Reference van Driem, Bellwood and Renfrew2002). Quite to the contrary, the spread of Indo-European furnishes the most obvious counter-example. Likewise, Sumerian, Elamite, Akkadian, Hurrian, Hattic and other languages which have left no surviving linguistic descendants were the tongues spoken by early agricultural civilisations, which therefore bear witness to the permeability of linguistic boundaries for the dissemination of agriculture.

The Neolithic and Bronze Age of Asia Minor and Mesopotamia are characterised by a very long period of incursive population movements into, rather than out of, Anatolia and the Fertile Crescent, driven or lured by the relative affluence of urban centres supported by agricultural surplus. Not just Indo-Europeans such as Hittites and Mitanni were drawn in by the good life. Gutaeans, Amorites, Kassites and other peoples likewise came to settle in the Fertile Crescent and Anatolia. Toponymical evidence and details about the cults of certain deities have been used to argue that even the Sumerians originally migrated from an earlier northern homeland to lower Mesopotamia, where they adopted agriculture from a resident population, at which time the Sumerians borrowed agricultural terms such as agar ‘field’, apin ‘seeder plough’ and apsin ‘furrow’ from a substrate language (Landsberger Reference Landsberger1943, Reference Landsberger1944, Reference Landsberger1945, Reference Landsberger1974; cf. Rubio Reference Rubio1999).

Similarly, it is likely that the Trans-Himalayans who introduced their pre-Sinitic language to the Yellow River basin first came as migrants in search of the good life to the affluent agriculture societies on the North China plain. It is therefore not inconceivable that the Yellow River basin, where Setaria and Panicum began to be domesticated about nine millennia ago, could have been the original Altaic homeland. This primary homeland may have been abandoned after an ancient Trans-Himalayan population introduced themselves and their Proto-Sinitic language to the early inhabitants of the North China plain, after which the secondary homeland of Trans-Eurasian may have moved north towards the Amur river basin.

The Father Tongue correlation can likewise not explain everything. If the paternal lineage C2 (M217) is correlated with Altaic linguistic affinity, as appears to be the case for Turkic, Mongolic and Tungusic, then Japanese is no Father Tongue, and neither is Korean. This Y-chromosomal haplogroup accounts for 11% of Korean paternal lineages, and the frequency of the lineage is even more reduced in Japan. Yet this molecular marker may still be a tracer for the introduction of Altaic language to the archipelago, where the paternal lineage has persisted, albeit in a frequency of just 6%.

On the other hand, the Y-chromosomal haplogroup D1a2 (M55) appears to be correlated with the ancient linguistic phylum of which Ainu is the surviving remnant. Therefore, Ainu is a father tongue, and the ancient paternal lineage D1a2 (M55) also remains robustly present elsewhere in Japan. If the bearers of the paternal lineage O2b introduced Yayoi culture and wet rice cultivation to the Japanese archipelago, then agriculture and superior metallurgy appear to have contributed to the fecundity of this paternal lineage, a veritable agricultural spread but without language.

Overly simple approaches that turn up correlations where direct causation is lacking or unlikely have come to enjoy perennial popularity (e.g. Nettle Reference Nettle1998; Gorenflo et al. Reference Gorenflo, Romaine, Mittermeier and Walker-Painemilla2012; Axelsen and Manrubia Reference Axelsen and Manrubia2014; Greenhill et al. Reference Greenhill, Hua, Welsh, Schneemann and Bromham2018; Hua et al. Reference Hua, Greenhill, Cardillo, Schneemann and Bromham2019). The Farmer Language Dispersal model is no doubt superior to such approaches in seeking to understand the dispersal of language families in terms of the spread of a subsistence strategy that has changed the face of our planet. However, enthused scholars oblivious to the faulty nature of the reasoning underlying the Farmer Language Dispersal model are inclined to seek corroboration and reinterpret evidence in ways that ‘fit’ that model. Similarly, the false assumption that any widely observed phenomenon, such as the Father Tongue correlation must therefore be universal, a presumption which we have repeatedly disavowed in print, would likewise prevent us from discerning the contours of a more complex picture of the past and render us unable to find the right fit for the plentiful pieces of the puzzle of prehistory.

References

Adachi, N., Shinoda, K., Umetsu, K., & Matsumura, H. (2009). Mitochondrial DNA analysis of Jomon skeletons from the Funadomari site, Hokkaido, and its implication for the origins of Native American. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 138(3), 255265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adachi, N., Shinoda, K., Umetsu, K., Kitano, T., Matsumura, H., Fujiyama, R., … Tanaka, M. (2011). Mitochondrial DNA analysis of Hokkaido Jomon skeletons, remnants of archaic maternal lineages at the southwestern edge of former Beringia. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 146(3), 346360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adachi, N., Sawada, J., Yoneda, M., Kobayashi, K., & Itoh, S. (2013). Mitochondrial DNA analysis of the human skeleton of the initial Jomon phase excavated at the Yugura cave site, Nagano, Japan. Anthropological Science, 121(2), 137143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adachi, N., Kakuda, T., Takahashi, R., Kanzawa-Kiriyama, H., & Shinoda, K. (2018). Ethnic derivation of the Ainu inferred from ancient mitochondrial DNA data. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 165(1), 139148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Akamine, M. (2017). The Ryūkyū Kingdom, Cornerstone of East Asia. Honolulu, HI: University of Hawai‘i Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arunkumar, G. P., Tatarinova, T. V., Duty, J., Rollo, D., Syama, A., Arun, V. S., … the Genographic Consortium. (2015). Genome-wide signatures of male-mediated migration shaping the Indian gene pool. Journal of Human Genetics, 60(9), 493499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Axelsen, J. B., & Manrubia, S. (2014). River density and landscape roughness are universal determinants of linguistic diversity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 281, 20141179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Balaresque, P., Poulet, N., Cussat-Blanc, S., Gerard, P., Quintana-Murci, L., Heyer, E., & Jobling, M. A. (2015). Y-chromosome descent clusters and male differential reproductive success: young lineage expansions dominate Asian pastoral nomadic populations. European Journal of Human Genetics, 23, 14131422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ballard, W. L. (1979). Chinese – a bastard at the Sino-Tibetan family reunion, paper presented at the 12th International Conference on Sino-Tibetan Languages and Linguistics, Paris.Google Scholar
Batchelor, J. (1925). The Pit-dwellers of Hokkaido and Ainu Place-names Considered. Sapporo: John Batchelor.Google Scholar
Benedict, P. K. (1942). Thai, Kadai, and Indonesia: a new alignment in southeastern Asia. American Anthropologist, 44, 576601.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedict, P. K. (1972). Sino-Tibetan: a Conspectus. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedict, P. K. (1990). Japanese/Austro-Tai (Linguistica Extranea Studia 20). Ann Arbor, MI: Karoma.Google Scholar
Blagden, C. O. (1909). From central India to Polynesia: a new linguistic synthesis. Journal of the Straits Branch of the Royal Asiatic Society, 53, 163173.Google Scholar
Blažek, V., Schwarz, M., & Srba, O. (2019). Altaic Languages: History of Research, Survey, Classification and a Sketch of Comparative Grammar. Brno: Masaryk University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blust, R. (1996). Beyond the Austronesian homeland: the Austric hypothesis and its implications for archaeology. In Goodenough, W. H. (Ed.), Prehistoric Settlement of the Pacific (pp. 117160). Philadelphia, PA: American Philosophical Society.Google Scholar
Chamberlain, B. H. (1887). The Language, Mythology, and Geographical Nomenclature of Japan viewed in the light of Aino Studies (Memoirs of the Literature College, Imperial University of Japan, No. I). Tōkyō: Imperial University.Google Scholar
Chang, K-C. (1986). The Archaeology of Ancient China (4th edn) New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Chaubey, G., & Endicott, P. (2013). The Andaman Islanders in a regional genetic context: reexamining the evidence for an early peopling of the archipelago from South Asia. Human Biology, 25(1), 7.Google Scholar
Chaubey, G., Metspalu, M., Choi, Y., Mägi, R., Romero, I. G., Rootsi, S., … Kivisild, T. (2011). Population genetic structure in Indian Austroasiatic speakers: the role of landscape barriers and sex-specific admixture. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 28(2), 10131024.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiang, C. W. K., Mangul, S., Robles, C., & Sankararaman, S. (2018) A comprehensive map of genetic variation in the world’s largest ethnic group-Han Chinese. Molecular Biology and Evolution 35(11), 27362750.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Civáň, P., Craig, H., Cox, C. J., & Brown, T. A. (2015). Three geographically separate domestications of Asian rice. Nature Plants, 1, 15164. doi:10.1038/nplants.2015.164CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Comrie, B. (2008). The areal typology of Chinese: Between North and Southeast Asia. In Djamouri, R., Meisterernst, B., & Sybesma, R. (Eds), Chinese Linguistics in Leipzig: Chinese Linguistics in Europe 2 (pp. 121). Paris: Centre de Recherches Linguistiques sur l'Asie Orientale.Google Scholar
Conrady, A. (1916). Eine merkwürdige Beziehung zwischen den austrischen und den indochinesischen Sprachen. In Aufsätze zur Kultur- und Sprachgeschichte vornehmlich des Orients: Ernst Kuhn zum 70. Geburtstage am 7. Februar 1916 gewidmet von Freunden und Schülern (pp. 475504). München: Verlag von M. & H. Marcus.Google Scholar
Conrady, A. (1922). Neue austrisch-indochinesische Parallelen. In Asia Major: Hirth Anniversary Volume, (pp. 2366). London: Robsthan and Company.Google Scholar
DeLancey, S. (2011). On the origins of Sinitic. In Jing-Schmidt, Zhuo (Ed.), Proceedings of the 23 rdNorth American Conference on Chinese Linguistics (Vol. 1, pp. 51–64). Eugene, OR: University of Oregon.Google Scholar
Derenko, M., Malyarchuk, B., Denisova, G., Wozniak, M., Grzybowski, T., Dambueva, I., & Zakharov, I. (2007). Y-chromosome haplogroup N dispersal from south Siberia to Europe. Journal of Human Genetics, 52, 763770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gakuhari, T., Nakagome, S., Rasmussen, S., Allentoft, M., Sato, T., Korneliussen, T., … Oota, H. (2019). Jomon genome sheds light on East Asian population history, à paraître.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gorenflo, L. J., Romaine, S., Mittermeier, R. A., & Walker-Painemilla, K. (2012). Co-occurrence of linguistic and biological diversity in biodiversity hotspots and high biodiversity wilderness areas. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(21), 80328037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenhill, S. J., Hua, X., Welsh, C. F., Schneemann, H., & Bromham, L. (2018). Population size and the rate of language evolution: a test across Indo-European, Austronesian and Bantu languages. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 576.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Günther, T., Malmström, H., Svensson, E. M., Omrak, A., Sánchez-Quinto1, F., Kılınç, G. M., … Jakobsson, M. (2018). Population genomics of Mesolithic Scandinavia: investigating early postglacial migration routes and high-latitude adaptation. Public Library of Science, Biology, 16(1), e2003703.Google ScholarPubMed
Hamada, S. (2006). Anthropology in Social Context: The Influence of Nationalism on the Discussion of the Ainu. Thesis, Master of Arts in Anthropology, Portland State University.Google Scholar
Hammer, M. F., Karafet, T. M., Park, H., Omoto, K., Harihara, S., Stoneking, M., & Horai, S. (2006). Dual origins of the Japanese: common ground for hunter–gather and farmer Y chromosomes. Journal of Human Genetics, 51, 4758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hashimoto, M. (1976a). Language diffusion on the Asian continent. Computational Analyses of Asian and African Languages, 3, 4964.Google Scholar
Hashimoto, M. (1976b). The agrarian and the pastoral diffusion of language. In Genetic Relationship, Diffusion and Typological Similarities of East and Southeast Asian Languages (pp. 114). Tokyo: Japan Society for the Promotion of Sciences.Google Scholar
Hashimoto, M. (1980). Typogeography of phonotactics and suprasegmentals in the languages of the East Asian continent. Computational Analyses of Asian and African Languages, 13, 153164.Google Scholar
Hashimoto, M. (1986). The altaicization of Northern Chinese. In McCoy, J. & Light, T. (Eds), Contributions to Sino-Tibetan Studies (pp. 7697). Leiden: E.J. Brill.Google Scholar
He, G., Chen, P., Wu, J., Luo, L., Gao, H., Wang, M., … Jia, F. (2019). Population genetic analysis of modern and ancient DNA variations yields new insights into the formation, genetic structure and phylogenetic relationship of northern Hàn Chinese. Frontiers in Genetics. doi: 10.3389/fgene.2019.01045Google Scholar
Hua, X., Greenhill, S. J., Cardillo, M., Schneemann, H., & Bromham, L. (2019). The ecological drivers of variation in global language diversity. Nature Communications, 10, 2047.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hudson, M. J. (2017). The Ryukyu islands and the northern frontier of prehistoric Austronesian settlement. In Piper, P. J., Matsumura, H., & Bulbeck, D. (Eds), New Perspectives in Southeast Asian and Pacific Prehistory (pp. 189199). Canberra: Australian National University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Igawa, K., Manabe, Y., Oyamada, J., Kitagawa, Y., Kato, K., Ikematsu, K., … Rokutanda, A. (2009). Mitochondrial DNA analysis of Yayoi period human skeletal remains from the Doigahama site. Journal of Human Genetics, 54, 581588.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ilumäe, A.-M., Reidla, M., Chukhryaeva, M., Järve, M., Post, H., Karmin, M., … Rootsi, S. (2016). Human Y chromosome haplogroup N: a non-trivial time-resolved phylogeography that cuts across language families. American Journal of Human Genetics, 99, 163173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jin, H.-J., Tyler-Smith, C., & Kim, Wook (2009). The peopling of Korea revealed by analyses of mitochondrial DNA and Y-chromosomal markers. Public Library of Science PLoS One 4(1), e4210.Google ScholarPubMed
Jinam, T. A., Nishida, N., Hirai, M., Kawamura, S., Oota, H., Umetsu, K., … Saitou, N. (2012). The history of human populations in the Japanese archipelago inferred from genome-wide SNP data with a special reference to the Ainu and the Ryukyuan populations. Journal of Human Genetics, 57, 787795.Google ScholarPubMed
Jinam, T. A., Kanzawa-Kiriyama, H., & Saitou, N. (2015). Human genetic diversity in the Japanese archipelago: dual structure and beyond. Genes and Genetic Systems, 90, 147152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jinam, T. A., Phipps, M. E., Aghakhanian, F., Majumder, P. P., Datar, F., Stoneking, M., … Saitou, N. (2017). Discerning the origins of the negritos, first Sundaland people: deep divergence and archaic admixture. Genome Biology and Evolution, 9(8), 20132022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kæmpfer, E. (1729). De Beschryving van Japan, behelsende een Verhaal van den Ouden en Tegenwoordigen Staat en Regeering van dat Ryk, van deszelfs Tempels, Paleysen, Kasteelen en andere Gebouwen; van deszelfs Metalen, Mineralen, Boomen, Planten, Dieren, Vogelen en Visschen, van de Tydrekening, en Opvolging van de Geestelyke en Wereldlyke Keyzers, van de Oorspronkelyke Afstamming, Godsdiensten, Gewoonten en Handwerkselen der Inboorlingen, en van hunnen Koophandel met de Nederlanders en de Chineesen, benevens eene Beschryving van het Koningryk Siam. Amsterdam: Balthasar Lakeman.Google Scholar
Kanzawa-Kiriyama, H., Saso, A., Suwa, G., & Saitou, N. (2013). Ancient mitochondrial DNA sequences of Jomon teeth samples from Sanganji, Tohoku district, Japan. Anthropological Science, 121(2), 89103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karafet, T. M., Zegura, S. L., & Hammer, M. F. (2009). Y-chromosome Japanese roots. In Peregrine, P. N., Peiros, I., & Feldman, M. (Eds), Ancient Human Migrations: A Multidisciplinary Approach (pp. 137148). Salt Lake City: University of Utah Press.Google Scholar
Karmin, M., Saag, L., Vicente, M., Wilson Sayres, M. A., Järve, M., Talas, U. G., … Kivisild, T. (2015). A recent bottleneck of Y chromosome diversity coincides with a global change in culture. Genome Research, 25(4), 459466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, S.-H., Kim, K.-C., Shin, D.-J., Jin, H.-J., Kwak, K.-D., Han, M.-S., … Kim, W. (2011). High frequencies of Y-chromosome haplogroup O2b-SRY465 lineages in Korea: a genetic perspective on the peopling of Korea. Investigative Genetics, 2011(2), 10.Google Scholar
Kivisild, T. (2014). Mapping Chromosome diversity in Eurasia, paper presented at the conference Migrations and Transfers in Prehistory Asian and Oceanic Ethnolinguistic Phylogeography, University of Bern, 28–30 July 2014.Google Scholar
Kivisild, T., Tolk, H.-V., Parik, J., Wang, Y., Papiha, S. S., Bandelt, H.-J., & Villems, R. (2002). The emerging limbs and twigs of the East Asian mtDNA tree. Molecular Biology and Evolution 19(10), 17371751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kutanan, W., Kampuansai, J., Changmai, P., Flegontov, P., Schröder, R., Macholdt, E., … Stoneking, M. (2018a). Contrasting maternal and paternal genetic variation of hunter–gatherer groups in Thailand. Nature Scientific Reports, 8, 1536.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kutanan, W., Kampuansai, J., Brunelli, A., Ghirotto, S., Pittayaporn, P., Ruangchai, S., … Stoneking, M. (2018b). New insights from Thailand into the maternal genetic history of mainland Southeast Asia. European Journal of Human Genetics, 26, 898911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kutanan, W., Kampuansai, J., Srikummool, M., Brunelli, A., Ghirotto, S., Arias, L., … Stoneking, M. (2019). Contrasting paternal and maternal genetic histories of Thai and Lao populations. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 36(7), 14901506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamnidis, T. C., Majander, K., Jeong, C., Salmela, E., Wessman, A., Vyacheslav Moiseyev, V. (2018). Ancient Fennoscandian genomes reveal origin and spread of Siberian ancestry in Europe. Nature Communications, 9, 5018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landsberger, B. (1943). Die Sumerer. Ankara Üniversitesi Dil ve Tarih-Coğrafya Fakültesi Dergisi, 1(5), 97102.Google Scholar
Landsberger, B. (1944). Die Anfaenge der Zivilisation in Mesopotamien. Ankara Üniversitesi Dil ve Tarih-Coğrafya Fakültesi Dergisi, 2(3), 431437.Google Scholar
Landsberger, B. (1945). Die geistigen Leistungen der Sumerer. Ankara Üniversitesi Dil ve Tarih-Coğrafya Fakültesi Dergisi, 3(2), 150158.Google Scholar
Landsberger, B. (1974). Three Essays on the Sumerians. Los Angeles, CA: Undena.Google Scholar
Lendering, J. (2010). Alexander de Grote: De ondergang van het Perzische rijk. Amsterdam: Athenaeum, Polak & van Gennep.Google Scholar
Lipson, M., Cheronet, O., Mallick, S., Rohland, N., Oxenham, M., Pietrusewsky, M., … Reich, D. (2018). Ancient genomes document multiple waves of migration in Southeast Asian prehistory. Science, 361, 9295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Londo, J. P., Chiang, Y., Hung, K.-H., Chiang, T.-Y., & Schaal, B. A. (2006). Phylogeography of Asian wild rice, Oryza rufipogon, reveals multiple independent domestications of cultivated rice, Oryza sativa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(25), 95789583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mabuchi, T., Susukida, R., Kido, A., & Oya, M. (2007). Typing the 1.1 kb control region of human mitochondrial DNA in Japanese individuals. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 52(2), 355363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mason, F. (1854). The Talaing language. Journal of the American Oriental Society, IV, 277289.Google Scholar
McColl, H., Racimo, F., Vinner, L., Demeter, F., Gakuhari, T., Víctor Moreno-Mayar, J., … Willerslev, E. (2018). The prehistoric peopling of Southeast Asia. Science, 361, 8892.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNally, K. L., Child, K. L., Bohnert, R., Davidson, R. M., Zhao, K., Ulat, V. J., … Leach, J. E. (2009). Genome-wide SNP variation reveals relationships among landraces and modern varieties of rice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(30), 1227312278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metspalu, M., Mondal, M., & Chaubey, G. (2018). The genetic makings of South Asia. Current Opinion in Genetics and Development: Genetics of Human Origins, 53, 128133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, R. A. (1971). Japanese and the Other Altaic Languages. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Mirabal, S., Reguiero, M., Cadenas, A. M., Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., Underhill, P. A., Verbenko, D. A., … Herrera, R. J. (2009). Y chromosome distribution within the geo-linguistic landscape of northwestern Russia. European Journal of Human Genetics,, 2009, 114.Google Scholar
Mizoguchi, K. (2002). An Archaeological History of Japan: 30,000 BC to AD 700. Phildelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Mizoguchi, K. (2014). The Archaeology of Japan from the Earliest Rice Farming Villages to the Rise of the State. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mörseburg, A., Pagani, L., Ricaut, F.-X., Yngvadottir, B., Harney, E., Castillo, C., … Kivisild, T. (2016). Multi-layered population structure in Island Southeast Asians. European Journal of Human Genetics, 24, 16051611.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakagome, S., Sato, T., Ishida, H., Hanihara, T., Yamaguchi, T., Kimura, R., … the Asian DNA Repository Consortium (2015). Model-based verification of hypotheses on the origin of modern Japanese revisited by Bayesian inference based on genome-wide SNP data. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 32(6), 15331543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nettle, D. (1998). Explaining global patterns of language diversity. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 17, 354374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nohira, C., Maruyama, S., & Minaguchi, K. (2008). Phylogenetic classification of Japanese mtDNA assisted by complete mitochondrial DNA sequences. International Journal of Legal Medicine, 124, 712.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nonaka, I., Minaguchi, K., & Takezaki, N. (2007). Y-chromosomal binary haplogroups in the Japanese population and their relationship to 16 Y-STR polymorphisms. Annals of Human Genetics, 71(4), 480495.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norman, J. (1982). Four notes on Chinese-Altaic linguistic contacts. Tsing Hua Journal of Chinese Studies, 1982(1–2), 243247.Google Scholar
Ostapirat, W. (2005). Kra-Dai and Austronesian: Notes on phonological correspondences and vocabulary distribution. In Sagart, L., Blench, R., & Sanchez-Mazas, A. (Eds), The Peopling of East Asia: Putting Together Archaeology, Linguistics and Genetics (pp. 107131). London: Routledge Curzon.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ostapirat, W. (2013). Austro-Tai revisited, paper presented at the 23rd Annual Meeting of the Southeast Asian Linguistic Society, Chulalongkorn University, 29 May.Google Scholar
Pearson, R. (2013). Ancient Ryūkyū, An Archaeological Study of Island Communities. Honolulu, HI: University of Hawai‘i Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peiros, I. (1998). Comparative Linguistics in Southeast Asia. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.Google Scholar
Peng, M-S., & Zhang, Y-P. (2012). Inferring the population expansions in peopling of Japan. Public Library of Science One, 6(6), e21509.Google Scholar
Polivanov, E. D. (1918). Odna iz japono-malajskix parallelej. Izvestija Rossijskoj Akademii Nauk, vième Série XII(18), 22832284.Google Scholar
Polivanov, E. D. (1924). K rabote o muzykal'noj akcentuacii v japonskom jazyke (v svjazi s malajskimi). Bjulleten’ 1-go Sredne-Aziatskogo gosudarstvennogo universiteta, 4, 101108.Google Scholar
Poloni, E. S., Semino, O., Passarino, G., Santachiara-Benerecetti, A. S., Dupanloup, I., Langaney, A., & Excoffier, L. (1997). Human genetic affinities for Y chromosome P49a,f/TaqI haploptypes show strong correspondence with linguistics. American Journal of Human Genetics, 61, 10151035. (Cf. the erratum published in 1998 in the American Journal of Human Genetics 62, 1267.)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poloni, E. S., Ray, N., Schneider, S., & Langaney, A. (2000). Languages and genes: modes of transmission observed through the analysis of male-specific and female-specific genes. In Dessalles, J.-L., & Ghadakpour, L. (Eds), Proceedings: Evolution of Language, 3rd International Conference 3–6 April 2000 (pp. 185186). Paris: École Nationale Supérieure des Télécommunications.Google Scholar
Poppe, N. (1965). Introduction to Altaic Linguistics. Wiesbaden: Otto Harrassowitz.Google Scholar
Poznik, G. D., Xue, Y., Mendez, F. L., Willems, T. F., Massaia, A., Wilson Sayres, M. A., … Tyler-Smith, C. (2016). Punctuated bursts in human male demography inferred from 1,244 worldwide Y-chromosome sequences. Nature Genetics, 48(6), 593599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rau, F., & Sidwell, P. (2019). The Munda maritime hypothesis. Journal of the Southeast Asian Linguistics Society, 12(2), 3557.Google Scholar
Reddy, B. M., Langstieh, B. T., Kumar, V., Nagaraja, T., Reddy, A. N. S., Aruna, M., … Singh, L. (2007). Austro-Asiatic tribes of Northeast India provide hitherto missing genetic link between South and Southeast Asia. Public Library of Science One, 2(11), e1141.Google ScholarPubMed
Robbeets, M. (2014). The Japanese inflectional paradigm in a Trans-Eurasian perspective. In Robbeets, M. & Bisang, W. (Eds), Paradigm Change in the Trans-Eurasian Languages and Beyond. Amsterdam: Benjamins, pp. 197232.Google Scholar
Rootsi, S., Zhivotovsky, L. A., Baldovič, M., Kayser, M., Kutuev, I. A., Khusainova, R., … Villems, R. (2007). A counter-clockwise northern route of the Y-chromosome haplogroup N from Southeast Asia towards Europe. European Journal of Human Genetics, 15, 204211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubio, G. (1999). On the alleged pre-Sumerian substratum. Journal of Cuneiform Studies, 51, 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saag, L., Laneman, M., Varul, L., Malve, M., Valk, H., Razzak, M. A., … Tambets, K. (2019). The arrival of Siberian ancestry connecting the eastern Baltic to Uralic speakers further east. Current Biology, 29, 17011711.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sahoo, S., Singh, A., Himabindu, G., Banerjee, J., Sitalaximi, T., Gaikwad, S., … Kashyap, V. K. (2006). A prehistory of Indian Y chromosomes: Evaluating demic diffusion scenarios. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(4), 843848.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saitou, N., & Jinam, T. A. (2017). Language diversity of the Japanese archipelago and its relationship with human DNA diversity. Man in India, 97(1), 205228.Google Scholar
Schlegel, G. (1901). Review: Elements of Siamese Grammar by O. Frankfurter, Ph.D., Bangkok: Printed at the American Presbyterian Mission Press, Leipzig, Karl W. Hiersemann, 1900. T‘oung Pao, (Série ii) II, 7687.Google Scholar
Schlegel, G. (1902). Siamese Studies, (T‘oung Pao, New Series ii, Volume ii, Supplement). Leiden.Google Scholar
Schmidt, W. (1906). Die Mon-Khmer Völker, ein Bindeglied zwischen Völkern Zentral-Asiens und Austronesiens. Archiv für Anthropologie, Neue Folge V, 59109.Google Scholar
Shinoda, K. (2004). Ancient DNA analysis of skeletal samples recovered from the Kuma–Nishioda Yayoi site. Bulletin of the National Science Museum, Tokyo, Series D 30, 18.Google Scholar
Shinoda, K., & Doi, N. (2008). Mitochondrial DNA analysis of human skeletal remains obtained from the old tomb of Suubaru: genetic characteristics of the westernmost island Japan. Bulletin of the National Museum of Natural Science, Series D 34, 1118.Google Scholar
Silva, M., Oliveira, M., Vieira, D., Brandão, A., Rito, T., Pereira, J. B., … Soares, P. (2017). A genetic chronology for the Indian subcontinent points to heavily sex-biased dispersals. BioMed Central Evolutionary Biology, 17, 88.Google ScholarPubMed
Singh, P. P., Vishwakarma, S., Gazi Sultana, N. N., Pilvar, A., Karmin, M., Rootsi, S., … Chaubey, G. (forthcoming). Counting the paternal founders of Mundari (Austroasiatic) speakers associated with the language dispersal in South Asia. European Journal of Human Genetics.Google Scholar
Skeat, W. W., & Blagden, C. O. (1906). Pagan Races of the Malay Peninsula, (2 volumes). London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Skoglund, P., Posth, C., Sirak, K., Spriggs, M., Valentin, F., Bedford, S., … Reich, D. (2016). Genomic insights into the peopling of the Southwest Pacific. Nature, 538, 510513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Starosta, S. (2005). [posthumous] Proto-East-Asian and the origin and dispersal of languages of East and Southeast Asia and the Pacific. In Sagart, L., Blench, R., & Sanchez-Mazas, A. (Eds), The Peopling of East Asia: Putting Together Archaeology, Lingtuistics and Genetics (pp. 182197). London: Routledge Curzon. [Editorial misrepresentations rectified in van Driem (2005).]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajima, A., Hayami, M., Tokunaga, K., Juji, T., Matsuo, M., Marzuki, S., … Horai, S. (2004). Genetic origins of the Ainu inferred from combined DNA analyses of maternal and paternal lineages. Journal of Human Genetics, 49, 187193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tambets, K., Yunusbayev, B., Hudjashov, G., Ilumäe, A.-M., Rootsi, S., Honkola, T., … Metspalu, M. (2018). Genes reveal traces of common recent demographic history for most of the Uralic-speaking populations. Genome Biology, 19, 139. doi.org/10.1186/s13059-018-1522-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tanaka, M., Cabrera, V. M., González, A. M., Larruga, J. M., Takeyasu, T., Fuku, N., … Shimodaira, H. (2004). Mitochondrial genome variation in eastern Asia and the peopling of Japan. Genome Research, 10A, 18321850.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tätte, K., Pagani, L., Pathak, A. K., Kõks, S., Duy, B. H., Ho, X. D., … Metspalu, M. (2019). The genetic legacy of continental scale admixture in Indian Austroasiatic speakers. Nature Scientific Reports, 9, 3818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trejaut, J. A., Poloni, E. S., Yen, J.-C., Lai, Y.-H., Loo, J.-H., Lee, C.-L., … Lin, M. (2014). Taiwan Y-chromosomal DNA variation and its relationship with Island Southeast Asia. BioMed Central Genetics, 15, 77.Google ScholarPubMed
Tripati, S., Prabhu, G., Patnaik, S. K., Acharya, S. K., Mishra, U. K., Behera, R. P., … Joshi, S. V. (2019). A preliminary study of knobbed ware from Odisha. Man and Environment, LXIV(1), 8289.Google Scholar
van Driem, G. (2001). Languages of the Himalayas: An Ethnolinguistic Handbook of the Greater Himalayan Region, containing an Introduction to the Symbiotic Theory of Language, (2 volumes). Leiden: Brill.Google Scholar
van Driem, G. (2002). Tibeto-Burman phylogeny and prehistory: Languages, material culture and genes. In Bellwood, P. & Renfrew, C. (Eds), Examining the Farming/Language Dispersal Hypothesis, pp. 233249. Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.Google Scholar
van Driem, G. (2005). Tibeto-Burman vs. Indo-Chinese: implications for population geneticists, archaeologists and prehistorians. In Sagart, L., Blench, R., & Sanchez-Mazas, A. (Eds), The Peopling of East Asia: Putting Together the Archaeology, Linguistics and Genetics (pp. 81106). London: Routledge Curzon.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Driem, G. (2007). The diversity of the Tibeto-Burman language family and the linguistic ancestry of Chinese. Bulletin of Chinese Linguistics, 1(2), 211270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Driem, G. (2014). A prehistoric thoroughfare between the Ganges and the Himalayas. In Jamir, T. & Hazarika, M. (Eds), 50 Years after Daojali-Hading: Emerging Perspectives in the Archaeology of Northeast India (pp. 6098). New Delhi: Research India Press.Google Scholar
van Driem, G. (2017). The domestications and the domesticators of Asian rice. In Robbeets, M. & Savelyev, A. (Eds), Language Dispersal Beyond Farming, (pp. 183214). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Hinloopen Labberton, D. (1925). The Oceanic languages and the Nipponese as branches of the Nippon–Malay–Polynesian family of speech [lecture read to the Asiatic Society of Japan at Tokyo on June 17th, 1925]. Transactions of the Asiatic Society of Japan, 2nd Series, II, 77115.Google Scholar
van Hinloopen Labberton, D. (1924). Preliminary results of researches into the original relationship between the Nipponese and the Malay–Polynesian languages. Journal of the Polynesian Society, XXXIII(4), 244280.Google Scholar
von Heine-Geldern, R. (1920). Gibt es eine austroasiatische Rasse? Archiv für Anthropologie, XLVI(1/2), 7999.Google Scholar
von Siebold, P. F. B. (1858). Aardrijks- en Volkenkundige Toelichtingen tot de Ontdekkingen van Maerten Gerritsz. Vries, met het fluitschip Castricum A° 1643, in ’t Oosten en ’t Noorden van Japan, dienende tot Zeemansgids langs de Oostkunst van Japan naar de eilanden Jezo, Krafto en de Kurilen. In Leupe, P. A. (Ed.), Reize van Maarten Gerritsz. Vries in 1643 naar het Noorden en Oosten van Japan volgens het Journaal gehouden door C.J. Coen, op het schip Castricum, naar het handschrift uitgegeven en met belangrijke bijlagen vermeerderd met de daarbij behoorende kaart en eenige facsimilés, en geographische en ethnographische aanteekeningen, tevens dienende tot een zeemansgids naar Jezo, Krafto en de Kurilen, en stukken over de taal en voortbrengselen der Aino-Landen (pp. 263440). Amsterdam: Koninklijk Instituut voor Taal-, Land- en Volkenkunde van Nederlandsch Indië.Google Scholar
Wang, C.-C., Yan, S., Qin, Z.-D., Lu, Y., & Ding, Q.-L. (2013). Late Neolithic expansion of ancient Chinese revealed by Y chromosome haplogroup O3a1c-002611. Journal of Systematics and Evolution, 51(3), 280286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watanabe, Y., Naka, I., Khor, S.-S., Sawai, H., Hitomi, Y., Tokunaga, K., & Ohashi, J. (2019). Analysis of whole Y-chromosome sequences reveals the Japanese population history in the Jomon period. Nature Scientific Reports, 9, 8556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wen, B., Hui, L., Daru, L., Xiufeng, S., Feng, Z., Yungang, H., … Li, J. (2004). Genetic evidence supports demic diffusion of Han culture. Nature, 431, 302305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whymant, A. N. J. (1926). The Oceanic theory of the origin of the Japanese language and people. Transactions of the Asiatic Society of Japan, 2nd Series III, 1581.Google Scholar
Wulff, K. (1934). Chinesisch und Tai: Sprachvergleichende Untersuchungen. Copenhagen: Levin & Munksgaard.Google Scholar
Wulff, K. (1942). [posthumous] Über das Verhältnis des Malay-Polynesischen zum Indochinesischen. Copenhagen: Munksgaard.Google Scholar
Xue, Y., Zerjal, T., Bao, W., Zhu, S., Shu, Q., Xu, J., … Tyler-Smith, C. (2006). Male demography in East Asia: a north–south contrast in human population expansion times. Genetics, 172(4), 24312439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yamaguchi-Kabata, Y., Nakazono, K., Takahashi, A., Saito, S., Hosono, N., Kubo, M., … Kamatani, N. (2008). Japanese population structure, based on SNP genotypes from 7003 individuals compared to other ethnic groups: Effects on population-based association studies. American Journal of Human Genetics, 83, 445456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yan, S., & Wang, C.-C., Li, H., Li, S.-L., Jin, L., & the Genographic Consortium (2011). An updated tree of Y-chromosome haplogroup O and revised phylogenetic positions of mutations P164 and PK4. European Journal of Human Genetics, 19(9), 10131015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yao, H.-B., Wang, C.-C., Wang, J., Tao, X., Shang, L., Wen, S.-Q., … Li, H. (2017a). Genetic structure of Tibetan populations in Gansu revealed by forensic STR loci. Nature Scientific Reports, 7, 41195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yao, H.-B., Tang, S., Yao, X., Yeh, H.-Y., Zhang, W., Xie, Z., … Wang, C.-C. (2017b). The genetic admixture in Tibetan–Yi Corridor. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 164, 522532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yao, X., Tang, S., Bian, B., Wu, X., Chen, G., & Wang, C.-C. (2017c). Improved phylogenetic resolution for Y-chromosome haplogroup O2a1c-002611. Nature Scientific Reports, 7, 1146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zerjal, T., Xue, Y., Bertorelle, G., Spencer Wells, R., Bao, W., Zhu, S., … Tyler-Smith, C. (2003). The genetic legacy of the Mongols. American Journal of Human Genetics, 72(3), 717721.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhào, Z.-J. (2005). Zhíwù káogǔxué jíqí xīn jìnzhǎn. Káogǔ, 2005 4249.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. The enhanced 2012 Benares recension of Starosta's East Asian linguistic phylum (Starosta 2005; van Driem 2014).

Figure 1

Figure 2. After the Last Glacial Maximum, the Y chromosomal haplogroup O (M175) split into the subclades O1 (F265, M1354) and O2 (M122).

Figure 2

Figure 3. Paternal lineages branching into new subclades. Each event involved a linguistic bottleneck leading to language families that today are reconstructible as distinct linguistic phyla. The O1 (F265, M1354) lineage gave rise to the O1a (M119) subclade, which moved eastward to the Fújiàn hill tracts and across the strait to Formosa, which so became the Urheimat of the Austronesians. Bearers of the O1b (M268) paternal lineage domesticated Asian rice. Bearers of O2a2a1a2 (M7) became the Proto-Hmong-Mien. In the Eastern Himalaya, the bearers of haplogroup O2a2b1 (M134) expanded and became the Trans-Himalayans. Haplogroup O1b1a1a (M95) is the Proto-Austroasiatic paternal lineage. The para-Austroasiatic fraternal clade O1b2 (M176) spread eastward, sowing seed along the way.

Figure 3

Figure 4. Two versions are depicted of the male-biased migration which introduced Austroasiatic language and four O1b1a1a1 (M95) paternal lineages to indigenous populations of the Choṭā Nāgpur, the overland trek from the Meghālaya originally proposed by Chaubey et al. (2011) and the Munda maritime migration proposed by Rau and Sidwell (2019).

Figure 4

Figure 5. In successive waves, the paternal lineages D1a2 (M55), C1a1 (M8), C2 (M217) and O1b2 (M176) migrated from the East Asian mainland to the Japanese archipelago at the dawn of the Palaeolithic, the Incipient Jōmon, the Early Jōmon and the Yayoi period respectively.