No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Vocalizations are ideal identity signals
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 03 April 2025
Abstract
If human societies are understood as identity groups, then our psychology should include design for the production and detection of credible identity signals. We argue that vocalizations are ideal identity signals because the human auditory system is sensitive to subtle acoustic features; vocal signals are efficient; and speech and song are highly complex, enabling the embedding therein of identity signals.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Bryant, G. A., Fessler, D. M., Fusaroli, R., Clint, E., Aarøe, L., Apicella, C. L., … Zhou, Y. (2016). Detecting affiliation in colaughter across 24 societies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(17), 4682–4687.Google ScholarPubMed
Bryant, G. A., Fessler, D. M., Fusaroli, R., Clint, E., Amir, D., Chávez, B., … Zhou, Y. (2018). The perception of spontaneous and volitional laughter across 21 societies. Psychological Science, 29(9), 1515–1525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carson, M. T., & Hung, H. C. (2021). Let's catch octopus for dinner: Ancient inventions of octopus lures in the Mariana Islands of the remote tropical pacific. World Archaeology, 53(4), 599–614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catchpole, C. K., & Slater, P. J. B. (1995). Bird song: Biological themes and variations. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Chwe, M. S. Y. (2013). Rational ritual: Culture, coordination, and common knowledge. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Crockford, C., Herbinger, I., Vigilant, L., & Boesch, C. (2004). Wild chimpanzees produce group-specific calls: A case for vocal learning? Ethology, 110(3), 221–243.Google Scholar
Davies, N. B. (2011). Cuckoo adaptations: Trickery and tuning. Journal of Zoology, 284(1), 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Desai, N. P., Fedurek, P., Slocombe, K. E., & Wilson, M. L. (2022). Chimpanzee pant-hoots encode individual information more reliably than group differences. American Journal of Primatology, 84(11), e23430.Google ScholarPubMed
Didla, G. S. (2020). A review of voice disguise in a forensic phonetic context. International Journal of English Literature and Social Sciences, 5(3), 721–725.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, N., & Levinson, S. C. (2009). The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(5), 429–448.Google ScholarPubMed
Fitch, W. T. (2011). Unity and diversity in human language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1563), 381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitch, W. T. (2018). The biology and evolution of speech: A comparative analysis. Annual Review of Linguistics, 4, 265–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giles, H., & Billings, A. C. (2004). Assessing language attitudes: Speaker evaluation studies. In Davies, A., & Elder, C. (Eds.), The handbook of applied linguistics, 187–209. Blackwell Publishing Ltd.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hilton, C. B., Crowley-de Thierry, L., Yan, R., Martin, A., & Mehr, S. A. (2023). Children infer the behavioral contexts of unfamiliar foreign songs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 152(3), 839.Google ScholarPubMed
Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E., & Spelke, E. S. (2007). The native language of social cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(30), 12577–12580.Google ScholarPubMed
Lomax, A. (1968). Folk song style and culture. American Association for the Advancement of Science.Google Scholar
McElreath, R., Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. (2003). Shared norms and the evolution of ethnic markers. Current Anthropology, 44(1), 122–130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mclver, J. D., & Stonedahl, G. (1993). Myrmecomorphy: Morphological and behavioral mimicry of ants. Annual Review of Entomology, 38(1), 351–377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mehr, S. A., & Krasnow, M. M. (2017). Parent-offspring conflict and the evolution of infant-directed song. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(5), 674–684.Google Scholar
Mehr, S. A., & Spelke, E. S. (2018). Shared musical knowledge in 11-month-old infants. Developmental Science, 21(2), 1–7.Google ScholarPubMed
Mehr, S. A., Song, L. A., & Spelke, E. S. (2016). For 5-month-old infants, melodies are social. Psychological Science, 27(4), 486–501.Google ScholarPubMed
Mehr, S. A., Singh, M., York, H., Glowacki, L., & Krasnow, M. M. (2018). Form and function in human song. Current Biology, 28(3), 356–368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mehr, S. A., Singh, M., Knox, D., Ketter, D. M., Pickens-Jones, D., Atwood, S., … Glowacki, L. (2019). Universality and diversity in human song. Science, 366(6468), 957–970.Google ScholarPubMed
Mehr, S. A., Krasnow, M. M., Bryant, G. A., & Hagen, E. H. (2021) Origins of music in credible signaling. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 44(59), 1–22.Google Scholar
Moser, C., Buckner, W., Sarian, M., & Winking, J. (2023). Aggressive mimicry and the evolution of the human cognitive niche. Human Nature, 34(3), 456–475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oller, D. K., Ramsay, G., Bene, E., Long, H. L., & Griebel, U. (2021). Protophones, the precursors to speech, dominate the human infant vocal landscape. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 376(1836), 20200255.Google ScholarPubMed
Przepiorka, W., & Diekmann, A. (2021). Parochial cooperation and the emergence of signalling norms. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 376(1838), 20200294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sims, J. P., Pirtle, W. L., & Johnson-Arnold, I. (2020). Doing hair, doing race: The influence of hairstyle on racial perception across the US. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 43(12), 2099–2119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh, M., & Mehr, S. A. (2023). Universality, domain-specificity and development of psychological responses to music. Nature Reviews Psychology, 2(6), 333–346.Google ScholarPubMed
Soltis, J. (2004). The developmental mechanisms and the signal functions of early infant crying. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(4), 477–490.Google Scholar
Titze, I. R. (2021). Regulation of laryngeal resistance and maximum power transfer with semi-occluded airway vocalization. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 149(6), 4106–4118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7(1), 49–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, C., Liang, W., Antonov, A. T., Cai, Y., Stokke, B. G., Fossøy, F., … Røskaft, E. (2012). Diversity of parasitic cuckoos and their hosts in China, Chinese Birds, 3 (1), 9–32.Google Scholar
Yurdum, L., Singh, M., Glowacki, L., Vardy, T., Atkinson, Q. D., Hilton, C. B., … Mehr, S. A. (2023). Universal interpretations of vocal music. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 120(37), e2218593120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zatorre, R. J., Belin, P., & Penhune, V. B. (2002). Structure and function of auditory cortex: Music and speech. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(1), 37–46.Google ScholarPubMed
Target article
What is a society? Building an interdisciplinary perspective and why that's important
Related commentaries (24)
A nation by any other name: A failure to focus on function
Belonging to a community of moral values as a key criterion of society
Beyond biology: A sociological stance on what is society
Collective memories and understandings of human societies
Definitions and cultural dynamics in understanding “societies”
Do boundaries matter so much for societies?
Group identity without social interactions?
How an interdisciplinary study of societies can develop a comprehensive understanding of the function of deceptive behavior
Identity groups, perceived group continuity, and schism
Identity is probably too complicated to serve as a useful criterion for defining society
Multi-species societies
Philosophy or science of societies?
Psychological mechanisms for individual recognition- and anonymous-societies in humans and other animals
Revisiting the spaces of societies and the cooperation that sustains them
Societal inferences from the physical world
Societies have functions for individuals and collectives
Societies of the open ocean without territories
Societies, identities, and macrodemes
Society: An anthropological perspective
The family as the primary social group
Understanding the jaggedness in social complexity is more important
Vocalizations are ideal identity signals
What is a society in the case of multilevel societies?
Why societies are important and grow so large: Tribes, nations, and teams
Author response
A society as a clearly membered, enduring, territory-holding group