Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T11:47:34.925Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Self-reported food intake decreases over recording period in the National Diet and Nutrition Survey

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 April 2020

Stephen Whybrow*
Affiliation:
Life Course and Population Health, The Rowett Institute, University of Aberdeen, Aberdeen AB25 2ZD, UK
Graham W. Horgan
Affiliation:
Biomathematics & Statistics Scotland, Aberdeen AB25 2ZD, UK
Jennie I. Macdiarmid
Affiliation:
Life Course and Population Health, The Rowett Institute, University of Aberdeen, Aberdeen AB25 2ZD, UK
*
*Corresponding author: Stephen Whybrow, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

From 2008, the UK’s National Diet and Nutrition Survey (NDNS) changed the method of dietary data collection from a 7-d weighed diary to a 4-d unweighed diary, partly to reduce participant burden. This study aimed to test whether self-reported energy intake changed significantly over the 4-d recording period of the NDNS rolling programme. Analyses used data from the NDNS years 1 (2008/2009) to 8 (2015/2016) inclusive, from participants aged 13 years and older. Dietary records from participants who reported unusual amounts of food and drink consumed on one or more days were excluded, leaving 6932 participants. Mean daily energy intake was 7107 kJ (1698 kcal), and there was a significant decrease of 164 kJ (39 kcal) between days 1 and 4 (P < 0·001). There was no significant interaction of sex or low-energy reporter status (estimated from the ratio of reported energy intake:BMR) with the change in reported energy intake. The decrease in reported energy intake on day 4 compared with day 1 was greater (P < 0·019) for adults with higher BMI (>30 kg/m2) than it was for leaner adults. Reported energy intake decreased over the 4-d recording period of the NDNS rolling programme suggesting that participants change their diet more, or report less completely, with successive days of recording their diet. The size of the effect was relatively minor, however.

Type
Full Papers
Copyright
© The Authors 2020

The burden on participants of completing food diaries has long been recognised. In one of the earliest dietary studies to use the weighed intake method, conducted 100 years ago, Moss commented that ‘It is extremely difficult to induce even the best men to undertake the required task for seven to ten days’(Reference Moss1). Despite the advantages of 7-d dietary records capturing a complete cycle of human behaviour(Reference McCarthy2), shorter recording periods are frequently used because of the lesser commitment needed from study participants, and because it is assumed that recording completeness diminishes as the recording period progresses(Reference Young and Trulson3). Indeed, there is some evidence that calculated energy intake from self-reported food intakes decreases over a 7-d recording period. In an earlier examination of the effect of recording period on low-energy reporting in the National Diet and Nutrition Survey (NDNS), mean reported energy intake decreased by 49 kJ (12 kcal) (se 22 kJ) per d between day 2 and 7 (P = 0·026) after accounting for the effect of day of the week on reported energy intake(Reference Whybrow, Horgan and Stubbs4). Until 2003, the NDNS used a 7-d weighed intake diary method, with recording starting on different days of the week. From 2008, the NDNS became a rolling programme of annual surveys using 4-d estimated food diaries, which were considered much less burdensome for participants(Reference Ashwell, Barlow and Gibson5). In year 1 of the rolling programme (2008/2009), participants started recording their food intake on Thursdays, Fridays or Saturdays, resulting in an unbalanced representation of days of the week and no recording on Wednesdays. This was addressed in the following years of the programme with the first recording day being balanced over all days of the week. Data collection for the rolling programme differs significantly from the earlier NDNS and is less work for participants to complete, and this may reduce the effect of decreasing reported energy intake over the recording period.

The present study aimed to compare the effects of recording period on reported energy intake in the NDNS rolling programme data, and specifically to test whether reported energy intake decreased as recording period progressed, and whether any effect was different for males v. females, or between low-energy reporters and presumed valid reporters.

Methods

Data from the NDNS rolling programme years 1 (2008/2009) to 8 (2015/2016)(Reference Bates, Cox and Nicholson6,Reference Bates, Lennox and Prentice7) were used to test for any effects of day of recording of dietary intake on reported energy intake. Participants in the NDNS rolling programme complete an unweighed food diary, recording household measures, or weights of foods from labels, for four consecutive days. Participants are a representative sample from the UK and were aged from 1 to 96 years.

Parents or carers completed the food diaries of children aged 12 years and younger(Reference Bates, Lennox and Prentice7), and these were not included in this study.

Food diary data were examined to identify any that were likely to have been unusual. Data from some participants were excluded where participants had only 3 d’s dietary data (n 160) or had recorded for non-consecutive days (n 5). When completing the diet record, participants note if they ate or drank more or less than usual, and the reason why this was so. Participants were excluded from these analyses where they reported unusual intakes because of illness or medical reasons (n 699), observing Ramadan (n 3), conscious effort such as being on a weight-loss diet (n 197) or reported an unusual intake but gave no reason (n 175). Of the remaining 6932 participants, 4060 reported an unusual intake on at least 1 of the 4 d, but the reason given was considered part of the normal day-to-day variability in food and drink consumption, such as ‘at friends’, ‘working’, ‘with family’ and ‘weekend’. These data were not excluded, and 4-d diet records from 6932 participants were analysed.

BMI was calculated from height and weight measurements taken by NDNS interviewers using standard protocols. Adults (≥18 years old) were classified into six groups by BMI(8).

BMR was estimated using the equations of Henry(Reference Henry9), using sex, age and body weight and, where a measurement was performed during the NDNS interview (n 6877), height.

Low-energy reporters were identified where the mean reported energy intake from the 4-d food record was less than 1·06 × BMR(Reference Goldberg, Black and Jebb10).

Statistics

Daily energy intake data were analysed by fitting a linear mixed model with fixed effect terms for the recording day (days 1–4), day of the week (Monday–Sunday), sex (male or female), age group and low-energy reporter status as a categorical variable (low-energy reporters or assumed valid reporters) and random effect terms for variation between and within individuals. Two-way interactions between fixed effects were also included. Significance of fixed effects was assessed by F tests with estimated denominator df. Change in reported energy intake between day 1 and 4 across groups by BMI category was compared using ANOVA, and least squares distance post hoc tests. Analyses were conducted using Genstat 17th edition.

Results

Reported energy intake differed by day of the week (P < 0·001) and by recording day (P < 0·001; Table 1). Reported energy intake decreased significantly with progressive day of recording after accounting for the day-of-the-week effect, with the mean difference in adjusted energy intake between day 1 and 4 being −164 kJ (−39 kcal).

Table 1. Mean unadjusted and adjusted reported energy intakes by day of week and by recording day

* Adjusted daily energy intake has accounted for effects of sex, age group, low-energy reporter status as a categorical variable, and recording day (for the day of week means) or day of the week (for the recording day means).

Mean daily reported energy intake was 7107 kJ (1698 kcal), equal to 1·18 × BMR, and 2509 (36·2 %) respondents were identified as low-energy reporters. Mean reported energy intake of the low-energy reporters was, obviously, lower than that of the presumed valid reporters (5759 kJ (1376 kcal) and 8703 kJ (2080 kcal), respectively, P < 0·001), and there was no significant interaction between low-energy reporter status and the difference in reported energy intake over the recording period. The mean difference in adjusted energy intake between recording days 1 and 4 was −116 kJ (–28 kcal) and −124 kJ (−30 kcal) (P = 0·144), equal to −3·7 and −2·4 % (P = 0·182) of mean daily energy intake, for low-energy reporters and assumed valid reporters, respectively.

There was no significant interaction between sex and recording day on reported energy intake between males and females (P = 0·702).

The interaction between age group and recording day on reported energy intake was statistically significant (P < 0·001). The youngest age group (13–20 years) had the biggest decrease, while the older adults (>61 years) generally increased energy intake over the recording period. Table 2 shows the mean unadjusted reported energy intake values by age group and sex.

Table 2. Mean unadjusted reported energy intake (kJ and kcal) and difference in reported energy intake between days 4 and 1 by age group and sex

Finally, there was a statistically significant, but quantitatively negligible, association between body weight status and the change in reported energy intake over the recording period. Participants with lower BMI tended to have a more negative difference in reported energy intakes on day 4 compared with day 1 than did participants with higher BMI. The correlation between BMI and difference in reported energy intake between recording days 1 and 4 was R 2 = 0·001 (P = 0·014).

BMI is not a good measure of body weight status in children and excluding participants <18 years old removed the correlation (R 2 = 0·000, P = 0·870). Grouping adults (≥18 years old) by BMI category, however, suggested that those with higher BMI reduced their energy intake, or reported less energy, over the recording period to a greater extent than did leaner adults (P = 0·019; Table 3).

Table 3. Mean daily energy intake and difference in energy intake between days 1 and 4 by BMI category

a,b,c,d,e,f Mean values within a column with the same superscript letters were significantly different (P < 0·05).

Discussion

This analysis of self-reported food and drink consumption in the NDNS rolling programme suggests that changing to a shorter recording period and an easier (for participants) recording method has not removed the tendency for energy intake to decrease over the recording period seen in 7-d weighed food records. The effect is still present but remains quantitatively minor at about 2 % of mean daily energy intake over 4 d.

Reported energy intake was significantly lower on day 1 than on other days in the 2000/2001 NDNS and decreased by 49 kJ (12 kcal) per d between days 2 and 7(Reference Whybrow, Horgan and Stubbs4). The adjusted energy intake was 350 kJ (84 kcal) higher on day 4 compared with day 1, whereas in the present analysis, it was 164 kJ (39 kcal) lower. Therefore, the magnitude of any effect of recording period (and the change in recording method from weighed to unweighed) on reported energy intake appears to have decreased slightly between the NDNS and NDNS rolling programmes, mainly because of lower intakes on day 1 in the 2000/2001 NDNS. When recording their food intake using food diaries, study participants tend to change their diets. They also misreport the foods that they consume, to varying degrees depending on the recording method(Reference Stubbs, O’Reilly and Whybrow11). If asked, around half will admit to altering their diet for various reasons, when completing a 7-d weighed food record, including being more conscious of what they were eating(Reference Macdiarmid and Blundell12). Quantifying this effect is difficult because of the need to record food intake without the participants’ awareness. In one study that achieved this by using covert weighing of subjects’ food when they were resident in the Rowett’s Human Nutrition Unit under conditions that were as close to free-living as practicable, subjects ate less when recording their diets compared with when they were not recording their diets, to the extent that energy intake decreased by 5 % (the observation(Reference Stubbs, O’Reilly and Whybrow11) or reactivity(Reference Thompson, Subar and Loria13) effect). Reported energy intake was an additional 5·1 % lower than actual energy intake when participants recorded their intakes using a weighed dietary record (the recording effect). The decrease in reported energy intake over the 4-d period of the NDNS was relatively small and comes from an increase in the observation effect, or an increase in the recording effect (or both) over the recording period. This decrease in the completeness of dietary recording over time is an additional error to the combined observation and recording effects that may need to be considered in study design to avoid introducing bias(Reference Whybrow, Horgan and Stubbs4).

Although the size of change in reported energy intake is similar in the 2000/2001 NDNS and combined NDNS rolling programme (2008/2016), reported energy intakes, and energy intakes relative to estimated energy requirements, in the present analysis appear to have fallen between the two. Mean daily reported energy intake was 8368 kJ (2000 kcal), equivalent to 1·29 × BMR (BMR recalculated using the using the equations of Henry(Reference Henry9)) in our previous analysis(Reference Whybrow, Horgan and Stubbs4) compared with 7107 kJ (1698 kcal) (1·18 × BMR) in the present study. However, our previous study did not include participants <19 years old or >64 years old, and this difference in energy intake between the two studies decreased when the present analysis was repeated using the same age range as in the previous study, being a mean of 7705 kJ (1842 kcal) per d (1·19 × BMR). When comparing nutrient intakes of the first year of the NDNS rolling programme with the 2000/2001 NDNS and 1997 NDNS of young people, Whitton et al. (Reference Whitton, Nicholson and Roberts14) found no significant differences in reported energy intakes. However, the authors did not exclude records from participants reporting unusual days because of illness, etc. Additionally, year 1 of the NDNS rolling programme always included both weekend days in the 4-d diet recording, which will have tended to elevate reported energy intakes, a feature that was acknowledged and addressed in subsequent years of the rolling programme. Numerous studies have reported an association between higher BMI and an increased likelihood of low-energy reporting (e.g. Refs. Reference Poslusna, Ruprich and de Vries15 and Reference Wehling and Lusher16). The prevalence of low-energy reporting in studies with similar methodology to the NDNS was 12–16 % for 3-d records, 31 % for 4-d records and 21–37 % for 7-d records. Across the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES), which assesses diet by 24-h recalls, from 2003 to 2012, the prevalence of low-energy reporting was 17 % for underweight adults, 14 % for normal weight, 23 % for overweight and 37 % for obese adults(Reference Murakami and Livingstone17). However, the few studies that have used a covertly measured food intake as the reference, rather than estimated or measured energy expenditure, have produced inconsistent findings, with no effect of BMI on the degree of misreporting(Reference Poppitt, Swann and Black18,Reference Conway, Ingwersen and Moshfegh19) , that obese subjects are more accurate in reporting their food intake than are overweight or lean subjects(Reference Conway, Ingwersen and Vinyard20), or that obese subjects are less accurate(Reference Schebendach, Porter and Wolper21). The difference in the apparent effect of BMI on the degree of misreporting when using estimated energy requirements compared with actual food intake may reflect a difficulty in estimating energy requirements in individuals with higher BMI. BMR is often estimated using well-established linear regression equations(Reference Henry9,Reference Schofield22) , which tend to over-estimate at higher body weights. Overestimating BMR will lower the ratio of reported energy intake:BMR and result in subjects with higher BMI being more likely to be identified as low-energy reporters than are lean subjects. Despite this, the overweight and obese still appear more likely to be classified as low-energy reporters than others after accounting for differences in body composition by estimating BMR from estimated fat-free mass(Reference Gemming and Ni Mhurchu23). Irrespective of whether or not overweight people under-report their food intake more, or change their diet more while recording it, than do lean people, the present study provides some evidence that the obese (BMI > 30 kg/m2) do change their diet, or under-report more, over the recording period. This is consistent with participants becoming more conscious of their diet as they record it. A greater concern with social approval, or social desirability, appears to be related to increased under-reporting of food intake, probably through the biased under-reporting of less healthy foods(Reference Maurer, Taren and Teixeira24). The effect is reported across most common dietary assessment methods, including food diaries, and tends to be greater in females than males. It is possible that social desirability is related to a greater decrease in reporting over the recording period.

Asking overweight people to record their food intake is a method used to help change behaviour and food choice to create a negative energy balance(Reference Elfhag and Rössner25,Reference Hollis, Gullion and Stevens26) . Completing a food diary results in a significant weight loss over 7 d even in participants who are not aiming to lose weight(Reference Milne, McNeill and Zakary27,Reference Whybrow, Mayer and Kirk28) . The results of this study suggest that the effect is similar in lean people also.

The results of this study might be interpreted as evidence that even shorter recording periods, such as 24-h recalls, would reduce the prevalence of low-energy reporting in dietary surveys. The benefit of less inaccurate diet records must be balanced against the need to capture intakes of foods that are infrequently consumed, such as oil-rich fish(Reference de Roos, Sneddon and Sprague29) and nutrients with large day-to-day variations in intake(Reference Palaniappan, Cue and Payette30). Additionally, 7 d is a full cycle of eating behaviour(Reference McCarthy2), and energy and macronutrient intakes vary over the days of the week(Reference Fyfe, Stewart and Murison31,Reference de Castro and Plunkett32) .

It is possible that the observation and recording effects on reported energy intake vary over the recording period. Some participants may have eaten less than usual on each of the 4 d, but with the same reporting accuracy (food records were an accurate description of an atypical diet), or food intake may have not greatly changed but recording accuracy decreased over the recording period (food records were an inaccurate record of a typical diet). Weighing participants before and after completing the diet records could be used to elucidate this, although change in body weight is not a good indicator of change in energy balance over such a short period. The data collection of the NDNS rolling programme does not allow these two effects to be separated.

Using ratios of reported energy intakes:estimated energy requirements to identify probable low-energy reporters is not without its problems, and participants with relatively high energy intakes may have changed their diet and under-reported their food intake to some degree.

The strengths of this study come from the strengths of the NDNS, which has amassed dietary data from a large nationally representative sample of the UK population, and which is broadly representative of the UK population for socio-economic classification. Because diet records were started on different days of the week, the present study was able to separate the effects of day of the week, and recording day, on reported energy intake.

The results of this study show that estimated energy intake from self-reported food intake decreased by a mean of 164 kJ (39 kcal) over the 4-d recording period of the NDNS rolling programme, suggesting that participants change their diet or report less completely over time.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by funding from the Rural and Environment Science and Analytical Services Division programme of the Scottish Government. Rural and Environment Science and Analytical Services Division had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article.

S. W. designed the study, interpreted the results and wrote the article. G. W. H. conducted the analysis. J. I. M. and G. W. H. contributed to the interpretation of the results and writing of the article.

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

References

Moss, NK (1923) Some effects of high air temperature and muscular exertion upon colliers. Proc R Soc B 95, 181200.Google Scholar
McCarthy, S (2014) Weekly patterns, diet quality and energy balance. Physiol Behav 134, 5559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, CM & Trulson, MF (1960) Methodology for dietary studies in epidemiological surveys. II-strengths and weaknesses of existing methods. Am J Public Health 50, 803814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whybrow, S, Horgan, G & Stubbs, RJ (2008) Low-energy reporting and duration of recording period. Eur J Clin Nutr 62, 11481150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashwell, M, Barlow, S, Gibson, S, et al. (2006) National Diet and Nutrition Surveys: the British experience. Public Health Nutr 9, 523530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, B, Cox, L, Nicholson, S, et al. (2016) National Diet and Nutrition Survey Results from Years 5 and 6 (combined) of the Rolling Programme (2012/2013–2013/2014). London: Public Health England.Google Scholar
Bates, B, Lennox, A, Prentice, A, et al. (2014) National Diet and Nutrition Survey results from years 1, 2, 3 and 4 (combined) of the rolling programme (2008/2009–2011/2012). London: Public Health England.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (2000) Obesity: preventing and managing the global epidemic. Report of a WHO consultation. World Health Organ Tech Rep Ser 894, 1253.Google Scholar
Henry, CJK (2005) Basal metabolic rate studies in humans: measurement and development of new equations. Public Health Nutr 8, 11331152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, GR, Black, AE, Jebb, SA, et al. (1991) Critical-evaluation of energy-intake data using fundamental principles of energy physiology. 1. Derivation of cutoff limits to identify under-recording. Eur J Clin Nutr 45, 569581.Google ScholarPubMed
Stubbs, RJ, O’Reilly, LM, Whybrow, S, et al. (2014) Measuring the difference between actual and reported food intakes in the context of energy balance under laboratory conditions. Br J Nutr 111, 20322043.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macdiarmid, JI & Blundell, JE (1997) Dietary under-reporting: what people say about recording their food intake. Eur J Clin Nutr 51, 199200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, FE, Subar, AF, Loria, CM, et al. (2010) Need for technological innovation in dietary assessment. J Am Diet Assoc 110, 4851.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitton, C, Nicholson, SK, Roberts, C, et al. (2011) National Diet and Nutrition Survey: UK food consumption and nutrient intakes from the first year of the rolling programme and comparisons with previous surveys. Br J Nutr 106, 18991914.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poslusna, K, Ruprich, J, de Vries, JH, et al. (2009) Misreporting of energy and micronutrient intake estimated by food records and 24 hour recalls, control and adjustment methods in practice. Br J Nutr 101, Suppl. 2, S73S85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wehling, H & Lusher, J (2017) People with a body mass index ≥30 under-report their dietary intake: a systematic review. J Health Psychol 24, 20422059.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murakami, K & Livingstone, MBE (2015) Prevalence and characteristics of misreporting of energy intake in US adults: NHANES 2003–2012. Br J Nutr 114, 12941303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poppitt, SD, Swann, D, Black, AE, et al. (1998) Assessment of selective under-reporting of food intake by both obese and non-obese women in a metabolic facility. Int J Obes 22, 303311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conway, JM, Ingwersen, LA & Moshfegh, AJ (2004) Accuracy of dietary recall using the USDA five-step multiple-pass method in men: an observational validation study. J Am Diet Assoc 104, 595603.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conway, JM, Ingwersen, LA, Vinyard, BT, et al. (2003) Effectiveness of the US Department of Agriculture 5-step multiple-pass method in assessing food intake in obese and nonobese women. Am J Clin Nutr 77, 11711178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schebendach, JE, Porter, KJ, Wolper, C, et al. (2012) Accuracy of self-reported energy intake in weight-restored patients with anorexia nervosa compared with obese and normal weight individuals. Int J Eating Disord 45, 570574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schofield, WN (1985) Predicting basal metabolic rate, new standards and review of previous work. Hum Nutr Clin Nutr 39, 541.Google ScholarPubMed
Gemming, L & Ni Mhurchu, C (2016) Dietary under-reporting: what foods and which meals are typically under-reported? Eur J Clin Nutr 70, 640641.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maurer, J, Taren, DL, Teixeira, PJ, et al. (2006) The psychosocial and behavioral characteristics related to energy misreporting. Nutr Rev 64, 5366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elfhag, K & Rössner, S (2005) Who succeeds in maintaining weight loss? A conceptual review of factors associated with weight loss maintenance and weight regain. Obes Rev 6, 6785.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollis, JF, Gullion, CM, Stevens, VJ, et al. (2008) Weight loss during the intensive intervention phase of the weight-loss maintenance trial. Am J Prev Med 35, 118126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milne, A, McNeill, G & Zakary, A (1991) Weight change as an indicator of energy imbalance during 7 day weighed food intake studies. Ecol Food Nutr 26, 281289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whybrow, S, Mayer, C, Kirk, TR, et al. (2007) Effects of two-weeks’ mandatory snack consumption on energy intake and energy balance. Obes Res 15, 673685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Roos, B, Sneddon, AA, Sprague, M, et al. (2017) The potential impact of compositional changes in farmed fish on its health-giving properties: is it time to reconsider current dietary recommendations? Public Health Nutr 20, 20422049.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palaniappan, U, Cue, RI, Payette, H, et al. (2003) Implications of day-to-day variability on measurements of usual food and nutrient intakes. J Nutr 133, 232235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fyfe, CL, Stewart, J, Murison, SD, et al. (2010) Evaluating energy intake measurement in free-living subjects: when to record and for how long? Public Health Nutr 13, 172180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Castro, JM & Plunkett, S (2002) A general model of intake regulation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26, 581595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Mean unadjusted and adjusted reported energy intakes by day of week and by recording day

Figure 1

Table 2. Mean unadjusted reported energy intake (kJ and kcal) and difference in reported energy intake between days 4 and 1 by age group and sex

Figure 2

Table 3. Mean daily energy intake and difference in energy intake between days 1 and 4 by BMI category