Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T19:00:53.360Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Encultured minds, not error reduction minds

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 May 2020

Robert Mirski
Affiliation:
The John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, 20-950Lublin, Poland. [email protected] https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Robert_Mirski
Mark H. Bickhard
Affiliation:
Lehigh University, Bethlehem, PA 18015. [email protected] https://www.lehigh.edu/~mhb0/mhb0.html
David Eck
Affiliation:
Cañada College, Redwood City, CA94061. [email protected] https://canadacollege.academia.edu/DavidEck
Arkadiusz Gut
Affiliation:
The Nicolaus Copernicus University in Toruń, 87-100Toruń, Poland. [email protected] https://umk.academia.edu/ArkadiuszGut

Abstract

There are serious theoretical problems with the free-energy principle model, which are shown in the current article. We discuss the proposed model's inability to account for culturally emergent normativities, and point out the foundational issues that we claim this inability stems from.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bickhard, M. H. (1992) How does the environment affect the person? In Children's development within social context, eds. Winegar, L. T. & Valsiner, J., pp. 6392. L. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (2007) Language as an interaction system. New Ideas in Psychology 25(2):171–87. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2007.02.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (2008) Social ontology as convention. Topoi 27(1–2):139–49. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-008-9036-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (2009) The interactivist model. Synthese 166(3):547–91. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-008-9375-x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (2015) Toward a model of functional brain processes II: Central nervous system functional macro-architecture. Axiomathes 25(4):377407. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10516-015-9276-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (2016) The anticipatory brain: Two approaches. In Fundamental issues of artificial intelligence, ed. Müller, V. C., pp. 259–81. Springer International Publishing.Google Scholar
Campbell, R. J. (2015) The metaphysics of emergence. Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A. (2013a) Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36(3):181204. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12000477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eck, D. (2015) The Encultured Mind: From Cognitive Science to Social Epistemology (Ph.D. thesis). University of South Florida.Google Scholar
Eck, D. & Levine, A. (2017) Prioritizing otherness: The line between vacuous individuality and hollow collectivism. In Studies in the philosophy of sociality: Volume 9. Sociality and normativity for robots: Philosophical inquiries into human-robot interactions, eds. Hakli, R. & Seibt, J., pp. 6787. Springer.Google Scholar
Friston, K. J., Samothrakis, S. & Montague, R. (2012) Active inference and agency: Optimal control without cost functions. Biological Cybernetics 106(8–9):523–41. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00422-012-0512-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiebel, S. J., Daunizeau, J. & Friston, K. J. (2008) A hierarchy of time-scales and the brain. PLoS Computational Biology 4(11):e1000209. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Litwin, P. & Miłkowski, M. (submitted) Unification by fiat: arrested development of predictive processing. Cognitive Science.Google Scholar
Martyushev, L. M. (2018) Living systems do not minimize free energy: Comment on “Answering Schrödinger's question: A free-energy formulation” by Maxwell James Dèsormeau Ramstead et al. Physics of Life Reviews 24:4041. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plrev.2017.11.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mirski, R. & Gut, A. (2018) Action-based versus cognitivist perspectives on socio-cognitive development: Culture, language and social experience within the two paradigms. Synthese 28(2):96. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-018-01976-y.Google Scholar
Roesch, E. B., Nasuto, S. J. & Bishop, J. M. (2012) Emotion and anticipation in an enactive framework for cognition (response to Andy Clark). Frontiers in Psychology 3:398. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar