Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T19:46:13.946Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Efficacy of bio- and neurofeedback for depression: a meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 November 2021

J. Fernández-Alvarez*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Milan, Italy Department of Basic Psychology, Clinic and Psychobiology, Universitat Jaume I, Castellón, Spain
M. Grassi
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Hermanas Hospitalarias, Villa San Benedetto Menni Hospital, FoRiPsi, Albese con Cassano, Como, Italy Department of Biomedical Sciences, Humanitas University, Rozzano, Milan, Italy
D. Colombo
Affiliation:
Department of Basic Psychology, Clinic and Psychobiology, Universitat Jaume I, Castellón, Spain
C. Botella
Affiliation:
Ciber Fisiopatología Obesidad y Nutrición, CB06/03 Instituto Salud Carlos III, Madrid, Spain
P. Cipresso
Affiliation:
Applied Technology for Neuro-Psychology Lab, IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Milan, Italy Department of Psychology, University of Turin, Turin, Italy
G. Perna
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Hermanas Hospitalarias, Villa San Benedetto Menni Hospital, FoRiPsi, Albese con Cassano, Como, Italy Department of Biomedical Sciences, Humanitas University, Rozzano, Milan, Italy Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Miller School of Medicine, University of Miami, Miami, FL, USA Research Institute of Mental Health and Neuroscience and Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, Faculty of Health, Medicine and Life Sciences, University of Maastricht, Maastricht, the Netherlands
G. Riva
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Milan, Italy Applied Technology for Neuro-Psychology Lab, IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Milan, Italy
*
Author for correspondence: J. Fernández-Alvarez, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

For many years, biofeedback and neurofeedback have been implemented in the treatment of depression. However, the effectiveness of these techniques on depressive symptomatology is still controversial. Hence, we conducted a meta-analysis of studies extracted from PubMed, Scopus, Web of Science and Embase.

Methods

Two different strings were considered for each of the two objectives of the study: A first group comprising studies patients with major depressive disorder (MDD) and a second group including studies targeting depressive symptomatology reduction in other mental or medical conditions.

Results

In the first group of studies including patients with MDD, the within-group analyses yielded an effect size of Hedges' g = 0.717, while the between-group analysis an effect size of Hedges' g = 1.050. Moderator analyses indicate that treatment efficacy is only significant when accounting for experimental design, in favor of randomized controlled trials (RCTs) in comparison to non RCTs, whereas the type of neurofeedback, trial design, year of publication, number of sessions, age, sex and quality of study did not influence treatment efficacy. In the second group of studies, a small but significant effect between groups was found (Hedges' g = 0.303) in favor of bio- and neurofeedback against control groups. Moderator analyses revealed that treatment efficacy was not moderated by any of the sociodemographic and clinical variables.

Conclusions

Heart rate variability (HRV) biofeedback and neurofeedback are associated with a reduction in self-reported depression. Despite the fact that the field has still a large room for improvement in terms of research quality, the results presented in this study suggests that both modalities may become relevant complementary strategies for the treatment of MDD and depressive symptomatology in the coming years.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Major depressive disorder (MDD) represents a worldwide leading cause of disability, with more than 300 million people affected (WHO, 2017). Not only does it entail a major impact on people's quality of life and social functioning (Angermeyer, Holzinger, Matschinger, & Stengler-Wenzke, Reference Angermeyer, Holzinger, Matschinger and Stengler-Wenzke2002; Gili et al., Reference Gili, García Toro, Armengol, Garcia-campayo, Castro and Roca2013; IsHak et al., Reference IsHak, Balayan, Bresee, Greenberg, Fakhry, Christensen and Rapaport2013; Zuelke et al., Reference Zuelke, Luck, Schroeter, Witte, Hinz, Engel and Riedel-heller2018), but also MDD is strongly related with a vast array of other mental disorders, mainly anxiety disorders (Watson, Reference Watson2009), as well as a great number of medical conditions, including chronic physical illnesses (Kang et al., Reference Kang, Kim, Bae, Kim, Shin, Yoon and Kim2015) and neurological diseases (Raskind, Reference Raskind2008).

Although psychotherapy (Cuijpers, Cristea, Karyotaki, Reijnders, & Huibers, Reference Cuijpers, Cristea, Karyotaki, Reijnders and Huibers2016), psychopharmacology (Cipriani et al., Reference Cipriani, Furukawa, Salanti, Chaimani, Atkinson, Ogawa and Geddes2018) and the combination of the previous two (Craighead & Dunlop, Reference Craighead and Dunlop2014) have shown to be efficacious, there is still much room for improvement. Around 40–50% of patients do not respond to treatment (Cuijpers et al., Reference Cuijpers, Karyotaki, Weitz, Andersson, Hollon and Van Straten2014) and a third of those who do respond present relapses (Beshai, Dobson, Bockting, & Quigley, Reference Beshai, Dobson, Bockting and Quigley2011; Burcusa & Iacono, Reference Burcusa and Iacono2007). Besides, it is estimated that 75% of depressed people remain untreated (WHO, 2017). Hence, it is of utmost importance to develop new modalities of treatment that can help to overcome the aforementioned obstacles (Kazdin & Blase, Reference Kazdin and Blase2011).

In this sense, biofeedback is considered one of the existing mind-body interventions that may foster the bridging of physiological and psychological interventions. Biofeedback techniques entail a signal (e.g. video, audio display or tactile) connected to a physiological process that enables the person to be aware of normally unconscious physiological activity (Browne, Reference Browne and Friedman2015). In this sense, individuals are provided with explicit information of a certain psychophysiological process in order to foster its regulation.

Biofeedback has principally been used in the medical realm, although there is also a long-standing tradition of research on biofeedback techniques for mental disorders (Lehrer & Gevirtz, Reference Lehrer and Gevirtz2014; Sacchet & Gotlib, Reference Sacchet and Gotlib2016). In particular, post-traumatic stress disorder and substance use disorder are among the most researched conditions (Schoenberg & David, Reference Schoenberg and David2014). Different physiological processes have been implemented for biofeedback procedures, including both the central and autonomous nervous systems. Electromyography biofeedback (EMGB), skin conductance biofeedback or heart rate variability biofeedback (HRVB) are some of the most used peripheral responses, while electroencephalographic (EEG) and functional magnetic resonance imaging neurofeedback (fMRI-NF) are two of the most common techniques using neural activity (Sacchet & Gotlib, Reference Sacchet and Gotlib2016).

Ample evidence demonstrated that different psychophysiological processes are impaired in patients with MDD. With regard to the neurocircuitry, functional impairments have been identified in prefrontal, limbic, striatal, thalamic and basal forebrain structures (Price & Drevets, Reference Price and Drevets2010). Of particular importance for neurofeedback, there is consistent evidence from EEG research demonstrating that depressive individuals present higher left-hemispheric alpha activity, including hypoactivation in the left prefrontal area. In this regard, an improvement of the depressive symptomatology has been observed after a neurofeedback-based training of this asymmetry (Linden, Reference Linden2014). Likewise, neuroimaging and brain structural research indicate that people with depression present several abnormalities. For instance, the amygdala has been identified as an important target in neurofeedback interventions for depression due to its role in emotional processing and responding, interacting with different cortical and subcortical areas and having shown to be a key marker of the onset and recovery of MDD (Young et al., Reference Young, Zotev, Phillips, Misaki, Drevets and Bodurka2018).

Likewise, cardiac activity has proven to greatly contribute to the general physiological dysregulation of depressed patients, and not only to be a correlate of the neural dysregulation (Thayer & Mather, Reference Thayer and Mather2018). Heart rate variability (HRV), in particular the high frequency (HF) of the spectral domain, is considered to index cardiac vagal tone and thus to be a relevant marker of MDD. Research in this domain indicates that depression is associated with lower resting HF-HRV and lower LF/HF ratio (Hamilton & Alloy, Reference Hamilton and Alloy2016; Kemp et al., Reference Kemp, Quintana, Gray, Felmingham, Brown and Gatt2010).

Taken together, these results indicate that NF and HRVB constitute two techniques that gather consistent theoretical support to justify a psychophysiological intervention. Indeed, there are a number of qualitative reviews (Hammond, Reference Hammond2005; Linden, Reference Linden2014; Sacchet & Gotlib, Reference Sacchet and Gotlib2016; Young et al., Reference Young, Zotev, Phillips, Misaki, Drevets and Bodurka2018) that have gathered the available studies of neurofeedback and biofeedback for MDD and depressive symptomatology. Nevertheless, to the best of our knowledge, no study has meta-analytically established the extent to which this approach is efficacious for MDD and depressive symptomatology, respectively.

Apart from calculating the overall effect of bio- and neurofeedback interventions for MDD, this study also aims to calculate the effect of all bio- and neurofeedback studies that included depressive symptomatology as a secondary outcome measure in subjects suffering from other conditions than MDD.

Main research questions

  1. (1) What is the pooled evidence for the effectiveness of bio- and neurofeedback for MDD?

  2. (2) What is the pooled evidence for the effectiveness of bio- and neurofeedback for depressive symptoms in both medical and mental/psychiatric conditions other than MDD?

  3. (3) What moderators explain possible sources of heterogeneity among the effect sizes?

Materials and methods

The systematic review has been developed in accordance with Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) (see Supplementary 1) (Moher et al., Reference Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff, Altman, Altman, Antes and Tugwell2009).

Search strategy

First, articles were identified through comprehensive searches of the following databases: PubMed, Scopus, Web of Science and Embase. The last update was in December 2018. References lists of review articles were also considered for potential undetected studies and gray literature has also been examined (for the search string see Appendix 2).

Eligibility criteria

This study follows a two-step level structure, and thus two different eligibility criteria have been considered. To address the first aim, original articles in English reporting data of the efficacy of bio- and neurofeedback in the treatment of MDD were considered. To select studies, the term “clinical depression” utilized in the DSM 5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) was considered, which comprises MDD and dysthymic disorder. All studies that either established a diagnosis of depression using a standardized diagnostic interview (such as the SCID, CIDI, or SCAN) or participants who presented elevated symptoms of depression based on self-report measures were considered for inclusion. )

Studies that included subjects taking psychopharmacology or receiving any other active treatment such as hormone therapy or psychotherapy were excluded.

To address the second aim, studies that measured depressive symptomatology through a psychometrically validated instrument and that presented a condition of bio- or neurofeedback in a randomized controlled trial were considered for inclusion. The objective of including this second layer of studies was to determine the extent to which depressive symptoms are treated through bio- and neurofeedback techniques in other mental disorders and particularly in medical studies. In other words, all studies assessing depressive symptomatology as a secondary outcome measure were comprised.

Unpublished studies, conference papers and proceedings, thesis and articles published in non-peer-reviewed were excluded from the study selection in both searches.

Study selection procedure

One reviewer completed all database searches for both objectives at the same time. All results were exported to EndNote and duplicates were eliminated. After that, two reviewers (JFA and DC) screened independently all titles and abstracts to identify potentially relevant article for any of the two objectives. Two different folders were created, one for each objective. From the total amount of studies that were included for further examination, the two independent reviewers read full texts to determine if the eligibility criteria were fulfilled. Disagreements were resolved through discussion, and if necessary, a third reviewer was consulted.

Quality assessment of studies

Cochrane Collaboration Risk of Bias tool has been used to assess sources of bias in randomized controlled trials (RCTs). Our considered criteria entail lack of allocation/concealment, lack of blinding, incomplete accounting of outcome or patient events, and selective outcome reporting.

Effect size calculation and coding of studies

We estimated the effect size of both the difference in change between the groups as well as the pre-post change within the biofeedback groups by using Hedges' g, a variation of Cohen's d which takes into account for biases associated with small sample sizes (Hedges & Olkin, Reference Hedges and Olkin1985). When the group mean, standard deviation (s.d.), variance or standard error of the mean, and a number of subjects were available for each group, these data were preferably used to calculate the effect size. When some of these data were missing, we looked for other data allowing for the effect size computation, such as unstandardized mean differences, t and p values. If multiple measurements for depression were used in the same study, a pooled effect size was calculated in order to include in the meta-analyses only a single effect size for each study. The pooled estimate of the effect size was calculated as the average of the different effect sizes of each measure. The variance of the pooled estimate was also calculated as the average of the different variances of the effect sizes; as the correlations among the measures are often not reported in the papers, such approach represents the most conservative way to calculate the pooled effect size variance (i.e. the strategy that leads to the largest variances of the estimate of the pool effect size, assuming a perfect correlation among measures). Similarly, a pooled effect size was calculated and included in the meat-analysis if either multiple biofeedback or control groups were used in the study.

In addition, the following study characteristics were coded and included in the analyses as moderators: (1) sex (% of female subjects in the control group); (2) age (mean age in the control group), (3) length of treatment (number of sessions), (4) type of biofeedback intervention (heart rate variability biofeedback or neurofeedback) (5) year of publication, (6) experimental design (randomized-control trial), (7) methodological quality of studies. Coding of moderators 1–3 may be not possible for all studies. In such case, a “not available” was be inserted.

Two of the authors independently performed the computation of effect sizes and any discrepancy was resolved before analysis, with the involvement of a third author in case of persistent disagreement.

Meta-analytic statistics

Each study effect size was weighted by its inverse variance (the sum of the within-study variance and an estimate of the between-studies variance), giving a larger weighting to studies with large sample sizes than those with small sample sizes. Before excluding a study because it was not possible to calculate the effect size due to a lack of enough statistical details reported in the paper, we tried to contact the corresponding author (only for studies published less than 10 years ago) asking for the missing details. Otherwise, the study was excluded from the analyses.

The pooled effect sizes were estimated using random-effects models (Restricted Maximum-Likelihood Estimation), with confidence intervals and statistical test calculated with Knapp–Hartung method (Knapp & Hartung, Reference Knapp and Hartung2003), which hypothesizes potential significant heterogeneity among studies. A significance level of 0.05 was applied.

Q statistic and I 2 index were used to investigate the heterogeneity of the effect sizes among studies. The significance level of the Q statistic was set at 0.1 to adjust for the limited statistical power of this test (Petitti, Reference Petitti2001). I 2 can be interpreted as the percentage of the total variability in a set of effect sizes that cannot be attributed only to the sampling error within studies. If the Q statistic resulted significant or I 2 suggested heterogeneity, we checked whether the source of this heterogeneity might have been attributed to one single effect size outlying from all others. In this case, we repeated the meta-analytic analyses one-by-one removing each effect size. The effect size was considered outlying when its exclusion from the analyses yielded a resolution of the heterogeneity, and its inclusion during the removal of other effect sizes did not.

Finally, moderators were included in the meta-analysis in order to try explaining possible sources of heterogeneity among the effect sizes. Moderators were considered only if they were available in at least four independent studies. Considering the limited number of studies expected to be included in the current meta-analyses, moderator analysis will be performed separately for each moderator.

Publication bias

Publication bias was assessed by visual inspection of the funnel plots and by the Egger's regression test (one-tailed p of <0.05 was considered to indicate the presence of the bias) (Egger, Davey Smith, Schneider, & Minder, Reference Egger, Davey Smith, Schneider and Minder1997). We also used the trim-and-fill method from Duval and Tweedie (Peters, Sutton, Jones, Abrams, & Rushton, Reference Peters, Sutton, Jones, Abrams and Rushton2007) to determine the nature of potential publication bias and to compute an estimated effect size that accounts for it.

Results

Included studies

As illustrated in the PRISMA flow chart (Fig. 1), a total of 11 786 records have been retrieved from the initial database searches. After removing all duplicated articles, the first screening step (examination of titles and abstracts) identified 7235 references that were of potential interest for our meta-analysis. Such a process was carried out by two reviewers and yielded 18 and 24 references, for each of the respective steps of our study. A total number of 22 papers fulfilled the inclusion criteria and were finally included in the study. 24 papers were excluded because they did not satisfy the inclusion criteria (described in Fig. 1). The whole procedure was independently done by two reviewers (JFA and DC).

Fig. 1. Flowchart of included studies in the meta-analysis.

A flowchart of the general inclusion procedure is reported in Fig. 1. No reply was received from any author we contacted to obtain missing data. Descriptions of all the included studies with relevant variables and study-level characteristics coded for each study are reported in Tables 13, for each step of the study.

Table 1. Between effect sizes of Neuro- and biofeedback for depressive symptomatology in all conditions

A/C, Allocation/concealment; Beck Depression Inventory–II; BF, biofeedback; CG, Control group; EEG, electroencephalogram; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; HAD, Hamilton Depression Rating Scale; HIPS, horizontal segment of intraparietal sulcus; HRV, Heart rate variability; IA, Incomplete accounting of outcome or patient events; LB, Lack of blinding; MADRS, Montgomery-Åsberg Depression; RCT, randomized controlled trial; Nr. of sessions, Number of sessions; SOR, Selective Outcome Reporting.

a Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (17-items).

Table 2. Within effect sizes of Neuro- and biofeedback for depressive symptomatology in all conditions

A/C, Allocation/concealment; BF, biofeedback; CG, Control group; EEG, electroencephalogram; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; IA, Incomplete accounting of outcome or patient events; LB, Lack of blinding; RCT, randomized controlled trial; Nr. of sessions, Number of sessions; SOR, Selective Outcome Reporting.

Table 3. Between effect sizes of Neuro- and biofeedback for depressive symptomatology in all conditions

A/C, Allocation/concealment; ADHD, Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; CES-D, Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale; CG, Control group; GHQ, General Health Questionnaire; HADS, Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; HRV, heart rate variability; IA, Incomplete accounting of outcome or patient events; PTSD, Post Traumatic Stress Disorder; RCT, randomized controlled trial; NF, Neurofeedback; Nr. of sessions, Number of sessions; LB, Lack of blinding; SOR, Selective Outcome Reporting TAU, Treatment as usual; Waiting List.

a One session with therapists and then four weeks to use at home three times a day.

Efficacy of bio- and neurofeedback for MDD (level 1)

Pre-post between-group effect sizes

For the pre-post between-group analysis comparing the bio- and neurofeedback and control groups, the random-effects analyses yielded an overall effect size of Hedges' g = 0.717 (95% CI 0.2121–1.1224, t = 3.357, p = 0.0121) (Fig. 2), indicating a greater efficacy of bio- and neurofeedback compared to control treatments in the treatment of MDD. In total, this meta-analysis was based on eight studies and 176 patients. No evidence of significant heterogeneity was found considering the Q statistics (Q = 8.193, p = 0.316), while I 2 resulted of 29%, indicating a small potential heterogeneity among the effect sizes of the single studies (Higgins, Reference Higgins2003).

Fig. 2. Pre-post between-group effect sizes in level 1.

Moderator analyses and publication bias

The effect of all moderators resulted statistically non-significant (see Table 4). The occurrence of publication bias was not suggested by any of the tests used (Trim and Fill analysis) suggests that no studies needed to fall to the right or left of the mean to make the plot symmetrical, and Egger's test resulted not significant (p = 0.4365) as well as by visual inspection of the funnel plot (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3. Funnel plot between analyses in level 1.

Pre-post within-group effect sizes

For the pre-post within-group analysis of the sole biofeedback treatment, the random effects meta-analysis yielded an overall within-group effect size of Hedges' g = 1.050 (95% CI 0.492–1.608, t = 5.991, p = 0.001) (Fig. 4), which indicates a significant efficacy of bio- and neurofeedback in improving MDD symptomatology (n = 110). No evidence of significant heterogeneity was found considering the Q statistics (Q = 3.353, p = 0.340), and also I 2 (13%) suggests a very limited heterogeneity among the effect sizes of the single studies.

Fig. 4. Pre-post within-group effect sizes in level 1.

Moderator analyses

Among the moderators (see Table 5), only the experimental design resulted in having a significant effect in moderating the overall within-group effect size (F = 126.582, p = 0.008). The within-group effect size of biofeedback treatments resulted significant both in randomized-controlled studies (Hedges' g = 1.391, 95% CI 1.216–1.566, t = 34.164, p < 0.001) and in non-randomized studies (Hedges' g = 0.783, 95% CI 0.630–1.566, t = 22.045, p = 0.002), with the latter resulting significantly greater than the former.

Table 4. Moderators between analysis level 1

Table 5. Moderators within analysis level 1

Table 6. Moderators level 2

Publication bias

Trim and Fill analysis indicated that one study would need to fall to the left of the mean to make the plot symmetrical, while no studies on the other side. This suggests that the overall effect size calculated in the within-group analysis may be inflated by the lack of inclusion in the meta-analysis of some unreported study, as it is also evidenced by visual inspection of the funnel plot (Fig. 5). However, the random-effects meta-analysis performed adjusting for missing studies still yielded a significant overall effect size (Hedges' g = 1.196, 95% CI 0.985–1.407, t = 11.102, p < 0.0001), with only a very small reduction of its previous magnitude. Instead, no evidence of publication bias was suggested by the Egger's test (p = 0.281)

Fig. 5. Funnel plot within analyses in level 1.

Efficacy of biofeedback for depressive symptoms in other conditions (level 2)

Pre-post between-group effect sizes

For the pre-post between-groups analysis comparing the bio- and neurofeedback and control groups, the random-effects analyses yielded a significant overall effect size of Hedges' g = 0.303 (95% CI 0.121–0.484, t = 2.217, p = 0.003) (Fig. 6), indicating a greater efficacy of bio- and neurofeedback compared to control treatments for depressive symptoms (n = 736). No evidence of significant heterogeneity was found considering the Q statistics (Q = 4.350, p = 0.993), and also I 2 (0%) suggests a lack of heterogeneity among the effect sizes of the single studies.

Fig. 6. Pre-post between-group effect sizes in level 2.

Moderator analyses and publication bias

The effect of all moderators resulted statistically non-significant (Table 6) and no occurrence of publication bias was suggested by any of the tests used (Trim and Fill analysis suggests that no studies needed to fall to the right or left of the mean to make the plot symmetrical, and Egger's test resulted not significant with a p = 0.911) as well as by visual inspection of the funnel plot (Fig. 7).

Fig. 7. Funnel plot between analyses in level 2.

Pre-post within-group effect sizes

Pre-post within-group analysis of the sole biofeedback treatments could not be performed because only one study (Kayiran, Dursun, Dursun, Ermutlu, & Karamürsel, Reference Kayiran, Dursun, Dursun, Ermutlu and Karamürsel2010) satisfied all the inclusion criteria for this part of the meta-analysis.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first meta-analysis of bio- and neurofeedback techniques for the treatment of MDD and depressive symptoms. Taken together, these findings suggest that bio- and neurofeedback constitute effective interventions for both individuals with clinical depression and with secondary depressive symptomatology. Besides, the results are in line with the findings of previous qualitative reviews (Hammond, Reference Hammond2005; Linden, Reference Linden2014; Sacchet & Gotlib, Reference Sacchet and Gotlib2016; Young et al., Reference Young, Zotev, Phillips, Misaki, Drevets and Bodurka2018).

Bio- and neurofeedback for MDD

While the within-group analyses yielded an effect size of Hedges' g = 0.717, the between-group analysis revealed an effect size of Hedges' g = 1.050. The moderator analyses indicate that treatment efficacy is only significant when accounting for experimental design, in favor of RCTs in comparison to non-RCTs. The fact that all other moderators were non-significant may indicate that results can be generalized to a range of social and clinical characteristics.

Nevertheless, these results must be taken with caution given that only one RCT has been conducted for MDD using HRVB (Caldwell & Steffen, Reference Caldwell and Steffen2018), and thus making it difficult to conclude that this technique is effective for MDD. Additionally, many of the identified studies targeting clinical depression with HRVB could not be included due to several reasons, such as the absence of a control group or the fact that participants were taking antidepressants. So far, a considerable number of studies have previously indicated that HRVB is effective for depression, frequently referring to the research conducted by Karavidas et al. (Reference Karavidas, Lehrer, Vaschillo, Vaschillo, Marin, Buyske and Hassett2007) or Siepmann, Aykac, Unterdörfer, Petrowski, and Mueck-Weymann (Reference Siepmann, Aykac, Unterdörfer, Petrowski and Mueck-Weymann2008). While it is true that both interventions were effective, none of them had either a control condition, in both cases patients were under psychopharmacological treatment and the samples were underpowered.

Conversely, neurofeedback gathered more evidence. The existing studies for neurofeedback comprised both studies brought forth for EEG (Cheon, Koo, & Choi, Reference Cheon, Koo and Choi2016; Choi et al., Reference Choi, Chi, Chung, Kim, Ahn and Kim2011) and fMRI (Li et al., Reference Li, Zhang, Song, Zhang, Zhang, Xing and Chen2015; Linden et al., Reference Linden, Habes, Johnston, Linden, Tatineni, Subramanian and Goebel2012; Mehler et al., Reference Mehler, Sokunbi, Habes, Barawi, Subramanian, Range and Linden2018; Young et al., Reference Young, Zotev, Phillips, Misaki, Yuan, Drevets and Bodurka2014, Reference Young, Siegle, Zotev, Phillips, Misaki, Yuan and Bodurka2017). Among the EEG studies, the target was to regulate the frontal asymmetric activation increasing the relative left frontal activity and consequently to improve depressive symptomatology. In the case of the fMRI studies, some of them targeted the amygdala (Young et al., Reference Young, Zotev, Phillips, Misaki, Yuan, Drevets and Bodurka2014, Reference Young, Siegle, Zotev, Phillips, Misaki, Yuan and Bodurka2017) and others the insula and lateral prefrontal areas (Mahler et al., Reference Mehler, Sokunbi, Habes, Barawi, Subramanian, Range and Linden2018), all of which are involved in the regulation of emotions (Sebastian & Ahmed Reference Sebastian, Ahmed, R. B., J. C., E. M. and R.2018). From this point of view, emotion regulation is a well-established transdiagnostic factor that explains both the appearance and maintenance of a vast array of affective disorders (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, Reference Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema and Schweizer2010), including MDD and depressive symptoms. Integrating the evidence from the prominent theoretical frameworks, it is, therefore, consistent to expect that regulating the principal physiological substrates associated with emotion regulation might increase the functionality of brain regions involved in several affective disorders, and thus decreasing the associated symptomatology.

Apart from RCTs, there are numerous single cases studies available in the literature, all conducted with EEG biofeedback (Baehr, Rosenfeld & Baehr, Reference Baehr, Rosenfeld and Baehr1998, Reference Baehr, Rosenfeld and Baehr2001; Earnest, Reference Earnest1999; Grin-Yastenko et al., Reference Grin-Yatsenko, Othmer, Ponomarev, Evdokimov, Konoplev and Kropotov2018; Hammond, Reference Hammond2000; Rosenfeld, Reference Rosenfeld2000). Besides, there are a set of studies that have focused on cognitive, affective or physiological variables without measuring depressive symptomatology but are worth mentioning given that they show the improvement of MDD with neurofeedback. Illustrative examples are the works by Escolano et al. (Reference Escolano, Navarro-Gil, Garcia-Campayo, Congedo, De Ridder and Minguez2014) for the regulation of cognitive deficits through the regulation of alpha activity or the study by Hamilton and Alloy (Reference Hamilton and Alloy2016), in which the authors demonstrated the efficacy in reducing the activity in the salience network. Last but not least, the cutting-edge research carried out by the Laureate Institute for Brain Research, who studied the role of fMRI for the increase of amygdala functional connectivity (Young et al., Reference Young, Zotev, Phillips, Misaki, Drevets and Bodurka2018) or correlation between amygdala activity and EEG asymmetry during emotion regulation (Zotev et al., Reference Zotev, Yuan, Misaki, Phillips, Young, Feldner and Bodurka2016).

Taken together, the results of neurofeedback for MDD indicate that it constitutes a promissory therapeutic alternative. Nonetheless, the discussion around neurofeedback is currently a matter of controversy in the scientific community. Specifically, a fierce discussion has arisen with regard to the consistency of results derived from EEG neurofeedback (EEG-nf). While some authors claim that EEG-nf has a too broad therapeutic target and thus it is not possible to disentangle specific from placebo effects (Thibault, Lifshitz & Raz, Reference Thibault, Lifshitz and Raz2016; Thibault & Raz, Reference Thibault and Raz2017, Reference Thibault and Raz2018), others defend a more nuanced position (Micolaud Franchi & Fovet, Reference Micoulaud-Franchi and Fovet2018). In any case, for the specific case of depression, it is clear that more studies are required before establishing a concluding statement given that the existing evidence is scant.

According to the results of the meta-analysis, MDD has been successfully treated either with HRVB or neurofeedback. Specifically, all studies comprised in level 1 achieved positive results in their patients. This is consistent with the extant theories explaining the mechanisms behind the psychophysiological dysfunction in affective disorders. Several theoretical frameworks such as the Polyvagal theory (Porges, Reference Porges2007), the Neurovisceral integration model (Thayer & Lane, Reference Thayer and Lane2009) or the baroreflex theory (Lehrer & Gevirtz, Reference Lehrer and Gevirtz2014) explored the relationship between visceral signals, afferent systems, and brain activity. Albeit there are differences among these models, there is a common understanding regarding a reciprocal determination between parasympathetic activity (in particular HF-HRV) and brain activity (in particular cortical regions like the prefrontal cortex and subcortical regions like the amygdala). In line with this, there is increasing evidence supporting that HRV and certain breathing patterns might have a causal role on the regulatory brain networks involved in emotion regulation (e.g. Thayer & Mather, Reference Thayer and Mather2018).

Bio- and neurofeedback for depressive symptomatology

Regarding this second group of studies, the effect size was small in magnitude, albeit significant, and smaller than the pre-post between-groups effect size found for MDD. However, it can be stated for the first time that bio- and neurofeedback techniques are efficacious for the reduction of depressive symptomatology. Given the fact that many of the included studies presented heterogeneous conditions, different baseline levels of depressive symptoms and types of biofeedback, the conclusions should be taken with caution. Moreover, even if many of the included studies presented heterogeneous conditions, no evidence of significant heterogeneity among the effect sizes of the different studies was found. This suggests a quite stable efficacy of bio- and neurofeedback on depressive symptoms independently from the condition.

Gaps and future challenges in the literature of bio- and neurofeedback for depression

First, it must be clearly stated that only a few of the included studies were rigorously conducted in both groups. The risk of bias was high or unclear in the majority of the studies, which represents an undoubted necessity of enhancing the quality of research in this field.

Besides, regarding the design, some flaws were identified. First, a great part of the studies was underpowered. Given the increasing availability of low cost but reliable psychophysiological devices, bigger samples will be possible to be recruited in the near future. This would represent an important step in order to more clearly determine the extent to which bio- and neurofeedback are effective interventions. In this direction, our results suggest the need for rigorous RCTs. Due to the high costs of conducting RCTs, single-case experimental designs appear also as a good alternative (Bentley, Kleiman, Elliott, Huffman, & Nock, Reference Bentley, Kleiman, Elliott, Huffman and Nock2019; Kazdin, Reference Kazdin2019). Furthermore, future studies should also consider the inclusion of follow-up assessments. Given that depression usually has a high risk of recurrence (Burcusa & Iacono, Reference Burcusa and Iacono2007), the stability of the therapeutic gains in the mid and long term is of paramount importance.

A third important aspect regarding the design of the studies revolves around control groups. Only a few studies presented active conditions as comparators and even fewer studies included both an active and a wait-list condition. Bio- and neurofeedback techniques permit to easily implement sham conditions. This may allow to increase the experimental rigor and thus to more accurately determine the specific contribution of the active aspects in the final outcome.

A fourth aspect to mention in the primary studies, also identified by Goessl, Curtiss, and Hofmann (Reference Goessl, Curtiss and Hofmann2017), is the necessity to better specify the amount of time spent with the professional or practicing bio- or neurofeedback and the therapeutic protocols that were used. The dose-response relationship may provide clues to explain mechanisms of change, something that has been scarcely researched in bio- and neurofeedback yet.

In the present meta-analysis, all included studies were carried out with traditional bio- and neurofeedback methods. That is, presenting the physiological process in a visual manner but without transforming the sensing into a particular actuation output that may be more relevant or engaging for the participants (Kitson, Prpa & Riecke, Reference Kitson, Prpa and Riecke2018). Besides, the field of bio- and neurofeedback has a lot of potentialities if different technologies are integrated into classical procedures. New engineering and design developments may foster multimodal biofeedback systems, taking into account auditory, visual and haptic feedbacks (Bergstrom, Seinfeld, Arroyo-Palacios, Slater, & Sanchez-Vives, Reference Bergstrom, Seinfeld, Arroyo-Palacios, Slater and Sanchez-Vives2014; Jones & Sarter, Reference Jones and Sarter2008). From this point of view and with regard to depression, a recent study has explored how music neurofeedback (EEG) could improve symptomatology in elderly people (Ramirez, Palencia-Lefler, Giraldo, & Vamvakousis, Reference Ramirez, Palencia-Lefler, Giraldo and Vamvakousis2015). Also, the cross-integration of biofeedback, virtual reality, and serious games is emerging (Schoeller et al., Reference Schoeller, Bertrand, Gerry, Jain, Horowitz and Zenasni2019). Some ongoing examples for affective regulation are already available, such as gamified biofeedback in mobile devices for stress management (Dillon, Kelly, Robertson, & Robertson, Reference Dillon, Kelly, Robertson and Robertson2016) or virtual reality-based biofeedback for generalized anxiety disorder (Repetto et al., Reference Repetto, Gaggioli, Pallavicini, Cipresso, Raspelli and Riva2013). Novel advancements are also very relevant, as the combination of biofeedback in a mobile-based application for the synchronization of HRV and electroencephalography (Lin, Reference Lin2018).

Finally, machine-learning techniques (MLT) may help to personalize bio- and neurofeedback. Adapted features, feedback, and mental strategies could allow for more tailored interventions based on the characteristics of the user (Alkoby, Abu-Rmileh, Shriki, & Todder, Reference Alkoby, Abu-Rmileh, Shriki and Todder2018; Perna, Grassi, Caldirola, & Nemeroff, Reference Perna, Grassi, Caldirola and Nemeroff2018). In the case of neurofeedback, the complexity of neural patterns suggests the convenience of adopting statistical strategies that can foster the identification of individual patterns. Specifically for depression, there have been good signs of progress to apply MLT for neuroimaging data (Kambeitz et al., Reference Kambeitz, Cabral, Sacchet, Gotlib, Zahn, Serpa and Koutsouleris2017) and this should be applied for future neurofeedback interventions.

Conclusion

Despite the described limitations, the results of the present study suggest that bio- and neurofeedback constitutes a promising technique for the reduction of depressive symptomatology in many diverse populations, including patients with MDD. Given the technological advancements in biosensors, a great improvement of this kind of technique may be expected in the near future. Furthermore, these interventions could be consistently integrated into psychotherapeutic contexts (Lehrer, Reference Lehrer2018), constituting together a potential alternative to the state-of-the-art developments in the treatment of depression (Hollon, Cohen, Singla, & Andrews, Reference Hollon, Cohen, Singla and Andrews2019).

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721004396.

References

Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 217237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alkoby, O., Abu-Rmileh, A., Shriki, O., & Todder, D. (2018). Can we predict who will respond to neurofeedback? A review of the inefficacy problem and existing predictors for successful EEG neurofeedback learning. Neuroscience, 378, 155164. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.12.050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Angermeyer, M. C., Holzinger, A., Matschinger, H., & Stengler-Wenzke, K. (2002). Depression and quality of life: Results of a follow-up study. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 48(3), 189190. https://doi.org/10.1177/002076402128783235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baehr, E., Rosenfeld, J. P., & Baehr, R. (1998). The clinical use of an alpha asymmetry biofeedback protocol in treatment of depressive disorders: Two case studies. Journal of Neurotherapy, 2, 1227.Google Scholar
Baehr, E., Rosenfeld, J. P., & Baehr, R. (2001). Clinical use of an Alpha Asymmetry Neurofeedback Protocol in the treatment of mood disorders. Journal of Neurotherapy, 4(4), 1118. doi: 10.1300/J184v04n04.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bentley, K. H., Kleiman, E. M., Elliott, G., Huffman, J. C., & Nock, M. K. (2019). Real-time monitoring technology in single-case experimental design research: Opportunities and challenges. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 117, 8796. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2018.11.017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bergstrom, I., Seinfeld, S., Arroyo-Palacios, J., Slater, M., & Sanchez-Vives, M. V. (2014). Using music as a signal for biofeedback. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 93(1), 140149. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.IJPSYCHO.2013.04.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beshai, S., Dobson, K. S., Bockting, C. L. H., & Quigley, L. (2011). Relapse and recurrence prevention in depression: Current research and future prospects. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(8), 13491360. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2011.09.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Browne, T. G. (2015). Biofeedback and neurofeedback. In Friedman, H. S. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of mental health: Second edition (pp. 170177). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-397045-9.00121-X.Google Scholar
Burcusa, S. L., & Iacono, W. G. (2007). Risk for recurrence in depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 27(8), 959985. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CPR.2007.02.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caldwell, Y. T., & Steffen, P. R. (2018). Adding HRV biofeedback to psychotherapy increases heart rate variability and improves the treatment of major depressive disorder. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 131(September 2018), 96101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheon, E. J., Koo, B. H., & Choi, J. H. (2016). The efficacy of neurofeedback in patients with major depressive disorder: An open labeled prospective study. Applied Psychophysiology Biofeedback, 41(1), 103110. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10484-015-9315-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, S. W., Chi, S. E., Chung, S. Y., Kim, J. W., Ahn, C. Y., & Kim, H. T. (2011). Is alpha wave neurofeedback effective with randomized clinical trials in depression? A pilot study. Neuropsychobiology, 63(1), 4351. https://doi.org/10.1159/000322290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choobforoushzadeh, Azadeh, Neshat-Doost, H. T., Molavi, H., & Abedi, M. R. (2015). Effect of neurofeedback training on depression and fatigue in patients with multiple sclerosis. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 40(1), 18. doi: 10.1007/s10484-014-9267-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cipriani, A., Furukawa, T. A., Salanti, G., Chaimani, A., Atkinson, L. Z., Ogawa, Y., … Geddes, J. R. (2018). Comparative efficacy and acceptability of 21 antidepressant drugs for the acute treatment of adults with major depressive disorder: A systematic review and network meta-analysis. The Lancet, 391(10128), 13571366. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(17)32802-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craighead, W. E., & Dunlop, B. W. (2014). Combination psychotherapy and antidepressant medication treatment for depression: For whom, when, and How. Annual Review of Psychology, 65(1), 267300. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131653.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuijpers, P., Cristea, I. A., Karyotaki, E., Reijnders, M., & Huibers, M. J. H. (2016). How effective are cognitive behavior therapies for major depression and anxiety disorders? A meta-analytic update of the evidence. World Psychiatry, 15(3), 245258. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuijpers, P., Karyotaki, E., Weitz, E., Andersson, G., Hollon, S. D., & Van Straten, A. (2014). The effects of psychotherapies for major depression in adults on remission, recovery and improvement: A meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 159, 118126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2014.02.026.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehghani-Arani, F., Rostami, R., & Nadali, H. (2013). Neurofeedback training for opiate addiction: Improvement of mental health and craving. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 38(2), 133141. doi: 10.1007/s10484-013-9218-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dillon, A., Kelly, M., Robertson, I. H., & Robertson, D. A. (2016). Smartphone applications utilizing biofeedback can aid stress reduction. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(JUN), 17. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00832.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Earnest, C.. (1999). Abnormal QEEG patterns associated with dissociation and violence. Journal of Neurotherapy, 3(2), 2835. doi: 10.1300/J184v03n02.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egger, M., Davey Smith, G., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ (Clinical Research Ed.), 315(7109), 629634. https://doi.org/10.1136/BMJ.315.7109.629.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Escolano, C., Navarro-Gil, M., Garcia-Campayo, J., Congedo, M., De Ridder, D., & Minguez, J. (2014). A controlled study on the cognitive effect of alpha neurofeedback training in patients with major depressive disorder. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 112. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gili, M., García Toro, M., Armengol, S., Garcia-campayo, J., Castro, A., & Roca, M. (2013). Major depressive disorder and comorbid anxiety disorder. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 58(12), 679686.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goessl, V. C., Curtiss, J. E., & Hofmann, S. G. (2017). The effect of heart rate variability biofeedback training on stress and anxiety: A meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 47(15), 25782586. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717001003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grin-Yatsenko, V. A., Othmer, S., Ponomarev, V. A., Evdokimov, S. A., Konoplev, Y. Y., & Kropotov, J. D. ( 2018). Infra-low frequency neurofeedback in depression: Three case studies. NeuroRegulation, 5(1), 3042. doi: 10.15540/nr.5.1.30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hallman, D. M., Olsson, E. M. G., von Schéele, B., Melin, L., & Lyskov, E.. (2011). Effects of heart rate variability biofeedback in subjects with stress-related chronic neck pain: a pilot study. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 36(2), 7180. doi: 10.1007/s10484-011-9147-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, J. L., & Alloy, L. B. (2016). Atypical reactivity of heart rate variability to stress and depression across development: Systematic review of the literature and directions for future research. Clinical Psychology Review, 50, 6779. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2016.09.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammond, D. (2005). Neurofeedback with anxiety and affective disorders. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 14(1), 105123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2004.07.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammond, D. C. (2000). Neurofeedback treatment of depression with the Roshi. Journal of Neurotherapy, 4(2), 4556. doi: 10.1300/J184v04n02_06.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Higgins, J. P. T. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ, 327(7414), 557560. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollon, S. D., Cohen, Z. D., Singla, D. R., & Andrews, P. W. (2019). New developments in the treatment of depression. Behavior Therapy, 50, 257269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2019.01.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hsueh, J.-J., Chen, T.-S., Chen, J.-J., & Shaw, F.-Z. (2016). Neurofeedback training of EEG alpha rhythm enhances episodic and working memory. Human Brain Mapping, 37(7), 26622675. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
IsHak, W. W., Balayan, K., Bresee, C., Greenberg, J. M., Fakhry, H., Christensen, S., & Rapaport, M. H. (2013). A descriptive analysis of quality of life using patient-reported measures in major depressive disorder in a naturalistic outpatient setting. Quality of Life Research, 22(3), 585596. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11136-012-0187-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, L. A., & Sarter, N. B. (2008). Tactile displays: Guidance for their design and application. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 50(1), 90111. https://doi.org/10.1518/001872008X250638.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kambeitz, J., Cabral, C., Sacchet, M. D., Gotlib, I. H., Zahn, R., Serpa, M. H., … Koutsouleris, N. (2017). Detecting neuroimaging biomarkers for depression: A meta-analysis of multivariate pattern recognition studies. Biological Psychiatry, 82(5), 330338. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.10.028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kang, H.-J., Kim, S.-Y., Bae, K.-Y., Kim, S.-W., Shin, I.-S., Yoon, J.-S., & Kim, J.-M. (2015). Comorbidity of depression with physical disorders: Research and clinical implications. Chonnam Medical Journal, 51(1), 818. https://doi.org/10.4068/cmj.2015.51.1.8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karavidas, M. K., Lehrer, P. M., Vaschillo, E., Vaschillo, B., Marin, H., Buyske, S., … Hassett, A. (2007). Preliminary results of an open-label study of heart rate variability biofeedback for the treatment of major depression. Applied Psychophysiology Biofeedback, 32(1), 1930. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10484-006-9029-z.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kayiran, S., Dursun, E., Dursun, N., Ermutlu, N., & Karamürsel, S. (2010). Neurofeedback intervention in fibromyalgia syndrome; A randomized, controlled, rater blind clinical trial. Applied Psychophysiology Biofeedback, 35(4), 293302. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10484-010-9135-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kazdin, A. E. (2019). Single-case experimental designs. Evaluating interventions in research and clinical practice. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 117, 317. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2018.11.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kazdin, A. E., & Blase, S. L. (2011). Rebooting psychotherapy research and practice to reduce the burden of mental illness. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(1), 2137. doi: 10.1177/1745691610393527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kemp, A. H., Quintana, D. S., Gray, M. A., Felmingham, K. L., Brown, K., & Gatt, J. M. (2010). Impact of depression and antidepressant treatment on heart rate variability: A review and meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 67(11), 10671074. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kitson, A., Prpa, M., & Riecke, B. E. (2018). Immersive interactive technologies for positive change: A scoping review and design considerations. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 399. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knapp, G., & Hartung, J. (2003). Improved tests for a random effects meta-regression with a single covariate. Statistics in Medicine, 22(17), 26932710. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.1482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lackner, N., Unterrainer, H. F., Skliris, D., Wood, G., Wallner-Liebmann, S. J., Neuper, C., & Gruzelier, J. H. (2016). The effectiveness of visual short-time neurofeedback on brain activity and clinical characteristics in alcohol use disorders. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 47(3), 188195. doi: 10.1177/1550059415605686.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehrer, P. M. (2018). Heart rate variability biofeedback and other psychophysiological procedures as important elements in psychotherapy. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 131, 8995. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.09.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehrer, P. M., & Gevirtz, R. (2014). Heart rate variability biofeedback: How and why does it work? Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 19. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, X., Zhang, T., Song, L. P., Zhang, Y., Zhang, G. G., Xing, C. X., & Chen, H. (2015). Effects of heart rate variability biofeedback therapy on patients with poststroke depression: A case study. Chinese Medical Journal, 128(18), 25422545. https://doi.org/10.4103/0366-6999.164986.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, I. M. (2018). Effects of a cardiorespiratory synchronization training mobile application on heart rate variability and electroencephalography in healthy adults. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 134(April), 168177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.09.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linden, D. E. J., Habes, I., Johnston, S. J., Linden, S., Tatineni, R., Subramanian, L., … Goebel, R. (2012). Real-time self-regulation of emotion networks in patients with depression. PLoS ONE, 7(6), e38115. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linden, D. E. J. (2014). Neurofeedback and networks of depression. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 16(1), 103112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2015.10.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mehler, D. M. A., Sokunbi, M. O., Habes, I., Barawi, K., Subramanian, L., Range, M., … Linden, D. E. J. (2018). Targeting the affective brain—a randomized controlled trial of real-time fMRI neurofeedback in patients with depression. Neuropsychopharmacology, 43(13), 25782585. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0126-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mehler, D. M. A., Sokunbi, M. O., Habes, I., Barawi, K., Subramanian, L., Range, M., … Linden, D. E. J. (2018). Targeting the affective brain—A randomized controlled trial of real-time fMRI neurofeedback in patients with depression. Neuropsychopharmacology, 43(13), 25782585. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-018-0126-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mennella, R., Patron, E., & Palomba, D. (2017). Frontal alpha asymmetry neurofeedback for the reduction of negative affect and anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 92, 3240. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2017.02.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Micoulaud-Franchi, J.-A., & Fovet, T. (2018). A framework for disentangling the hyperbolic truth of neurofeedback: Comment on Thibault and Raz (2017). American Psychologist, 73(7), 933935. doi: 10.1037/amp0000340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D. G., Altman, D., Antes, G., … Tugwell, P. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement (Chinese edition). Journal of Chinese Integrative Medicine, 7(9), 889896. https://doi.org/10.3736/jcim20090918.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patron, E., Messerotti, B. S., Favretto, G., Valfrè, C., Bonfà, C., Gasparotto, R., & Palomba, D. (2013). Biofeedback assisted control of respiratory sinus arrhythmia as a biobehavioral intervention for depressive symptoms in patients after cardiac surgery: A preliminary study. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 38(1), 19. doi: 10.1007/s10484-012-9202-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penzlin, A. I., Siepmann, T., Illigens, B. M.-W., Weidner, K., & Siepmann, M. (2015). Heart rate variability biofeedback in patients with alcohol dependence: A randomized controlled study. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 11, 26192627. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S84798.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perna, G., Grassi, M., Caldirola, D., & Nemeroff, C. B. (2018). The revolution of personalized psychiatry: Will technology make it happen sooner? Psychological Medicine, 48(5), 705713. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717002859.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peters, J. L., Sutton, A. J., Jones, D. R., Abrams, K. R., & Rushton, L. (2007). Performance of the trim and fill method in the presence of publication bias and between-study heterogeneity. Statistics in Medicine, 26(25), 45444562. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.2889.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petitti, D. B. (2001). Approaches to heterogeneity in meta-analysis. Statistics in Medicine, 20(23), 36253633. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.1091.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology, 74(2), 116143. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BIOPSYCHO.2006.06.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, J. L., & Drevets, W. C. (2010). Neurocircuitry of mood disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(1), 192216. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2009.104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramirez, R., Palencia-Lefler, M., Giraldo, S., & Vamvakousis, Z. (2015). Musical neurofeedback for treating depression in elderly people. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 9(OCT), 110. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2015.00354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raskind, M. A. (2008). Diagnosis and treatment of depression comorbid with neurologic disorders. The American Journal of Medicine, 121(11), S28S37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amjmed.2008.09.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ratanasiripong, P., Kaewboonchoo, O., Ratanasiripong, N., Hanklang, S., & Chumchai, P. (2015). Biofeedback intervention for stress, anxiety, and depression among graduate students in public health nursing. Nursing Research and Practice, 15. doi: 10.1155/2015/160746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Repetto, C., Gaggioli, A., Pallavicini, F., Cipresso, P., Raspelli, S., & Riva, G. (2013). Virtual reality and mobile phones in the treatment of generalized anxiety disorders: A phase-2 clinical trial. Personal and Ubiquitous Computing, 17(2), 253260. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00779-011-0467-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenfeld, J. P. (2000). An EEG biofeedback protocol for affective disorders. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 31(1), 712. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/155005940003100106.Google ScholarPubMed
Sacchet, M. D., & Gotlib, I. H. (2016). Recent developments and future directions. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics, 16(9), 10031005. https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schoeller, F., Bertrand, P., Gerry, L. J., Jain, A., Horowitz, A. H., & Zenasni, F. (2019). Combining virtual reality and biofeedback to foster empathic abilities in humans. Frontiers in Psychology, 9(February), 15. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schoenberg, P. L. A., & David, A. S. (2014). Biofeedback for psychiatric disorders: A systematic review. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 39(2), 109135. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10484-014-9246-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schönenberg, M., Wiedemann, E., Schneidt, A., Scheeff, J., Logemann, A., Keune, P. M., & Hautzinger, M. (2017). Neurofeedback, sham neurofeedback, and cognitive-behavioural group therapy in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A triple-blind, randomised, controlled trial. The Lancet Psychiatry, 4(9), 673684. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30291-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sebastian, C. L., & Ahmed, S. P.. (2018). Neurobiology of emotion regulation. In R. B., Anthony, J. C., Adam, E. M., Ruth, & R., Pia (Eds.), The Wiley Blackwell Handbook of Forensic Neuroscience (Vol. 1, pp. 125140). Wiley Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siepmann, M., Aykac, V., Unterdörfer, J., Petrowski, K., & Mueck-Weymann, M. (2008). A pilot study on the effects of heart rate variability biofeedback in patients with depression and in healthy subjects. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 33(4), 195201. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10484-008-9064-z.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, K. S., Gevirtz, R. N., Brown, M., Spira, J., Guarneri, E., & Stoletniy, L. (2009). The effect of biofeedback on function in patients with heart failure. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 34(2), 7191. doi: 10.1007/s10484-009-9077-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thayer, J. F., & Lane, R. D. (2009). Claude Bernard and the heart-brain connection: Further elaboration of a model of neurovisceral integration. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 33(2), 8188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thayer, J. F., & Mather, M. (2018). How heart rate variability affects emotion regulation brain networks. Curr Opin Behav Sci., 19, 98104. doi: 10.1016/J.COBEHA.2017.12.017.Google Scholar
Thibault, R. T., Lifshitz, M., & Raz, A. (2016). The self-regulating brain and neurofeedback: Experimental science and clinical promise. Cortex, 74, 247261. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.10.024.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thibault, R. T., & Raz, A. (2017). The psychology of neurofeedback: Clinical intervention even if applied placebo. American Psychologist, 72(7), 679688. doi: 10.1037/amp0000118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thibault, R. T., & Raz, A. (2018). A consensus framework for neurofeedback research (and the perils of unfounded neuroreductionism): Reply to Micoulaud-Franchi and Fovet (2018). American Psychologist, 73(7), 936937. doi: 10.1037/amp0000366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, D. (2009). Differentiating the mood and anxiety disorders: A quadripartite model. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 5(1), 221247. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.032408.153510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
WHO. (2017). Depression and other common mental disorders: Global health estimates. World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Young, K. D., Siegle, G. J., Zotev, V., Phillips, R., Misaki, M., Yuan, H., … Bodurka, J. (2017). Randomized clinical trial of real-time fMRI amygdala neurofeedback for major depressive disorder: Effects on symptoms and autobiographical memory recall. American Journal of Psychiatry, 174(8), 748755. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.16060637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, K. D., Zotev, V., Phillips, R., Misaki, M., Drevets, W. C., & Bodurka, J. (2018). Amygdala real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging neurofeedback for major depressive disorder: A review. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 72(7), 466481. https://doi.org/10.1111/pcn.12665.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, K. D., Zotev, V., Phillips, R., Misaki, M., Yuan, H., Drevets, W. C., & Bodurka, J. (2014). Real-time fMRI neurofeedback training of amygdala activity in patients with major depressive disorder. PLoS ONE, 9(2). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088785.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yu, L.-C., Lin, I.-M., Fan, S.-Y., Chien, C.-L., & Lin, T.-H. (2018). One-year cardiovascular prognosis of the randomized, controlled, short-term heart rate variability biofeedback among patients with coronary artery disease. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 25(3), 271282. doi: 10.1007/s12529-017-9707-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yuan, H., Young, K. D., Phillips, R., Zotev, V., Misaki, M., & Bodurka, J.. (2014). Resting-state functional connectivity modulation and sustained changes after real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging neurofeedback training in depression. Brain Connectivity, 4(9), 690701. doi: 10.1089/brain.2014.0262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zotev, V., Yuan, H., Misaki, M., Phillips, R., Young, K. D., Feldner, M. T., & Bodurka, J. (2016). Correlation between amygdala BOLD activity and frontal EEG asymmetry during real-time fMRI neurofeedback training in patients with depression. NeuroImage: Clinical, 11, 224238. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.02.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zucker, T. L., Samuelson, K. W., Muench, F., Greenberg, M. A., & Gevirtz, R. N. (2009). The effects of respiratory sinus arrhythmia biofeedback on heart rate variability and posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms: A pilot study. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 34(2), 135143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuelke, A. E., Luck, T., Schroeter, M. L., Witte, A. V., Hinz, A., Engel, C., … Riedel-heller, S. G. (2018). The association between unemployment and depression – results from the. Journal of Affective Disorders, 235(March), 399406. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2018.04.073.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Flowchart of included studies in the meta-analysis.

Figure 1

Table 1. Between effect sizes of Neuro- and biofeedback for depressive symptomatology in all conditions

Figure 2

Table 2. Within effect sizes of Neuro- and biofeedback for depressive symptomatology in all conditions

Figure 3

Table 3. Between effect sizes of Neuro- and biofeedback for depressive symptomatology in all conditions

Figure 4

Fig. 2. Pre-post between-group effect sizes in level 1.

Figure 5

Fig. 3. Funnel plot between analyses in level 1.

Figure 6

Fig. 4. Pre-post within-group effect sizes in level 1.

Figure 7

Table 4. Moderators between analysis level 1

Figure 8

Table 5. Moderators within analysis level 1

Figure 9

Table 6. Moderators level 2

Figure 10

Fig. 5. Funnel plot within analyses in level 1.

Figure 11

Fig. 6. Pre-post between-group effect sizes in level 2.

Figure 12

Fig. 7. Funnel plot between analyses in level 2.

Supplementary material: File

Fernández-Alvarez et al. supplementary material

Fernández-Alvarez et al. supplementary material

Download Fernández-Alvarez et al. supplementary material(File)
File 18.7 KB