Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-29T01:43:25.608Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The protective effect of neighbourhood social cohesion on adolescent mental health following stressful life events

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 June 2019

Mila Kingsbury
Affiliation:
School of Epidemiology and Public Health, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Canada
Zahra Clayborne
Affiliation:
School of Epidemiology and Public Health, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Canada
Ian Colman*
Affiliation:
School of Epidemiology and Public Health, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Canada
James B. Kirkbride
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, UCL, London, England
*
Author for correspondence: Ian Colman, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Exposure to stressful life events is an established risk factor for the development of adolescent mental disorder. Growing evidence also suggests that neighbourhood social environments, including strong social cohesion, could have a protective effect on mental health. However, little is known about how neighbourhood social cohesion may buffer against the effects of stressful life events on adolescent mental health. Our aim was to assess whether neighbourhood social cohesion modifies the association between stressful life events and adolescent mental health outcomes.

Methods

Data were drawn from a nationally-representative prospective sample of Canadian adolescents, including 5183 adolescents aged 12/13 years at T1 and 14/15 years at T2. Caregivers reported neighbourhood social cohesion at T1, and exposure to stressful life events between T1 and T2. Symptoms of mental health and behaviour problems were self-reported by adolescents at T1 and T2. Multivariable logistic regression was used to determine whether the relationship between stressful life events and outcomes was modified by neighbourhood social cohesion.

Results

Associations between stressful life events and adolescent outcomes were statistically significantly lower in neighbourhoods with greater social cohesion for: depression/anxiety (high cohesion OR = 0.98 v. low cohesion OR = 3.11), suicidal ideation (ORhigh = 1.30 v. ORlow = 5.25), aggression/conduct disorder (ORhigh = 1.09 v. ORlow = 4.27), and property offence (ORhigh = 1.21 v. ORlow = 4.21).

Conclusions

Greater neighbourhood social cohesion appeared to buffer the effects of stressful life events on several domains of adolescent mental health. This potentially presents a target for public health intervention to improve adolescent mental health and behavioural outcomes.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2019

Stressful life events [SLEs] in childhood and adolescence are well-established risk factors for the development of later psychiatric problems (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Davis and Kendler1997). Exposure to both acute and chronic stressors early in life, ranging from parental separation to exposure to violence or abuse, can have adverse, long-term impacts on mental health. For example, SLEs, including maltreatment, have been linked to increased depressive and anxiety symptoms (Michl et al., Reference Michl, McLaughlin, Shepherd and Nolen-Hoeksema2013), as well as antisocial behaviour (Lansford et al., Reference Lansford, Dodge, Pettit, Bates, Crozier and Kaplow2002), conduct disorder (Jaffee et al., Reference Jaffee, Caspi, Moffitt, Dodge, Rutter, Taylor and Tully2005), hyperactivity (De Sanctis et al., Reference De Sanctis, Nomura, Newcorn and Halperin2012), and suicidal ideation (Afifi et al., Reference Afifi, Enns, Cox, Asmundson, Stein and Sareen2008). These early mental health problems can persist into adulthood (Naicker et al., Reference Naicker, Galambos, Zeng, Senthilselvan and Colman2013), carrying additional risks of experiencing substance use problems, lower educational attainment, difficulty maintaining stable employment, and difficulty developing healthy and meaningful interpersonal relationships (Colman et al., Reference Colman, Murray, Abbott, Maughan, Kuh, Croudace and Jones2009). Given their potential long-term impacts on mental health and psychosocial development, early life SLEs present a major public health issue for which we need to identify potential factors which may improve the long-term outlook for children exposed to these early-life stressors.

Social cohesion, defined as the level of connectedness between individuals living in close geographical proximity (Sampson, Reference Sampson2003), may present such a factor, and has been linked with better physical and mental health (Araya et al., Reference Araya, Dunstan, Playle, Thomas, Palmer and Lewis2006; Echeverría et al., Reference Echeverría, Diez-Roux, Shea, Borrell and Jackson2008). One mechanism through which this may operate is via the strength of community ties, which may create environments where health-promoting behaviours are reinforced, and negative behaviours (e.g. vandalism, drinking in public spaces) are discouraged (Kawachi and Berkman, Reference Kawachi and Berkman2001; Echeverría et al., Reference Echeverría, Diez-Roux, Shea, Borrell and Jackson2008). A growing number of studies demonstrate the impact of social cohesion on a number of youth mental health outcomes (De Silva et al., Reference De Silva, McKenzie, Harpham and Huttly2005; Donnelly et al., Reference Donnelly, McLanahan, Brooks-Gunn, Garfinkel, Wagner, Jacobsen, Gold and Gaydosh2016). In particular, inverse relationships have been demonstrated between social cohesion and child and adolescent depression (De Silva et al., Reference De Silva, McKenzie, Harpham and Huttly2005; Echeverría et al., Reference Echeverría, Diez-Roux, Shea, Borrell and Jackson2008; Kingsbury et al., Reference Kingsbury, Kirkbride, McMartin, Wickham, Weeks and Colman2015; Donnelly et al., Reference Donnelly, McLanahan, Brooks-Gunn, Garfinkel, Wagner, Jacobsen, Gold and Gaydosh2016; Solmi et al., Reference Solmi, Colman, Weeks, Lewis and Kirkbride2017), anxiety (De Silva et al., Reference De Silva, McKenzie, Harpham and Huttly2005; Kingsbury et al., Reference Kingsbury, Kirkbride, McMartin, Wickham, Weeks and Colman2015; Donnelly et al., Reference Donnelly, McLanahan, Brooks-Gunn, Garfinkel, Wagner, Jacobsen, Gold and Gaydosh2016), and externalizing behaviours (Curtis et al., Reference Curtis, Dooley and Phipps2004; De Silva et al., Reference De Silva, McKenzie, Harpham and Huttly2005; Jaffee et al., Reference Jaffee, Caspi, Moffitt, Polo-Tomás and Taylor2007).

Emerging research has also demonstrated that residing in highly cohesive neighbourhoods may strengthen a child's ability to positively cope with adversity (Silk et al., Reference Silk, Sessa, Morris, Steinberg and Avenevoli2004; Jaffee et al., Reference Jaffee, Caspi, Moffitt, Polo-Tomás and Taylor2007). For example, among children exposed to maltreatment, those who report stronger social ties with adults in their community, including their parents, extended family members, and schoolteachers, tend to report better overall adjustment compared with children who report weaker social ties with community members (Cicchetti and Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch1997; Jaffee et al., Reference Jaffee, Caspi, Moffitt, Polo-Tomás and Taylor2007). Children with stronger community ties also score more highly on measures of resiliency, and do not present with the elevated levels of antisocial behaviour typically seen among children exposed to maltreatment (Jaffee et al., Reference Jaffee, Caspi, Moffitt, Polo-Tomás and Taylor2007). This raises the possibility that for children and adolescents exposed to SLEs, living in a cohesive community may buffer the adverse, long-term impacts of these stressors. To our knowledge, however, no population-based, longitudinal study has tested whether social cohesion moderates the relationship between childhood adversity and subsequent common mental and behavioural disorders. We therefore sought to investigate the role of neighbourhood social cohesion as a potential modifier of the associations between exposure to stressful life events in early adolescence and symptoms of mental and behavioural disorders two years later, using well-characterized longitudinal data from a large, prospective cohort. We hypothesized that neighbourhood social cohesion would buffer the effect of stressful life events on negative outcomes in adolescence.

Methods

Data source

Data for the present study were drawn from cycles 5 (2001–2002), 6 (2003–2004), 7 (2005–2006), and 8 (2007–2008) of the National Longitudinal Survey of Children and Youth (NLSCY). The NLSCY is a longitudinal study of Canadian children and adolescents designed to track multiple aspects of youth health and development. Stratified sampling resulted in a sample that is considered representative of children living in private homes in Canada's 10 provinces (excluding families in the Armed Forces, those living in institutions, on reserves, and in the northern Territories). Cohort members were followed prospectively, with assessments from multiple informants every two years. Statistics Canada obtained written informed consent from parents of survey respondents and regulates access of survey data through National research data centres. The present sample was based on 5913 respondents who were aged 12/13 (T1) in cycles 5, 6, or 7, and for whom data were available 2 years later at ages 14/15 (T2), during cycles 6, 7, or 8.

Measures

Mental and behavioural disorders

Adolescent psychiatric symptoms were self-reported at T1 and T2 using behaviour scales adapted from questionnaires used in the Montreal Longitudinal Study and the Ontario Child Health Study (Boyle et al., Reference Boyle, Offord, Hofmann, Catlin, Byles, Cadman, Crawford, Links, Rae-Grant and Szatmari1987). The scales were designed to identify children who would be most likely to meet DSM-III-TR criteria for a psychiatric diagnosis. Paper questionnaires were completed privately by adolescents, and returned to interviewers in a sealed envelope. For the present study, the following subscales were of interest: anxiety/depression (7 items, e.g. ‘I am not as happy as other people my age’; ‘ I am too fearful or nervous’), physical aggression/conduct disorder (6 items, e.g. ‘I get into many fights’), property offence (6 items, e.g. ‘I vandalize’), and hyperactivity/inattention (7 items, e.g. ‘I am easily distracted’; ‘I am impulsive, I act without thinking’). Adolescents responded on a 3-point scale (‘never or not true’; ‘sometimes or somewhat true’; ‘often or very true’). Subscale scores, pro-rated for item-level missingness, were provided by Statistics Canada. For each outcome, subscale scores were dichotomized at the top decile to indicate psychopathology of potential clinical relevance, consistent with previous studies (McMartin et al., Reference McMartin, Kingsbury, Dykxhoorn and Colman2014; Kingsbury et al., Reference Kingsbury, Kirkbride, McMartin, Wickham, Weeks and Colman2015).

Adolescent suicidal behaviour was assessed at T2 on the basis of two questions. First, adolescents were asked whether they had seriously considered suicide in the past 12 months, with suicidal ideation defined as answering ‘yes’ to this question. Second, adolescents who screened positive for suicidal ideation were additionally asked how many times they had attempted suicide in the past year. For the present study, suicide attempt was defined as one or more attempts in the past year.

Stressful life events [SLEs]

Adolescent exposure to SLEs in the past 2 years was reported by the person most knowledgeable about the child at T2 (hereafter referred to as ‘primary caregiver’; approximately 90% were biological mothers). Respondents were asked whether the participant had experienced an event that caused the participant ‘a great amount of worry or unhappiness’ in the past 2 years (i.e. since baseline). Those who answered ‘yes’ were then asked about 13 specific life events (e.g. parental death, parental divorce/separation, abuse or fear of abuse). For the present study, we defined exposure to SLEs in the previous two years as a binary variable (any v. none).

Neighbourhood social cohesion

Primary caregivers reported on neighbourhood social cohesion at T1. The social cohesion score was based on 5 statements, rated on a 4-point scale from ‘strongly agree’ to ‘strongly disagree’ (‘people around here are willing to help their neighbours’; ‘there are adults in the neighbourhood that children can look up to’; ‘when I'm away from home, I know that my neighbours will keep their eyes open for possible trouble’; ‘you can count on adults in this neighbourhood to watch out that children are safe and don't get in trouble’; ‘if there's a problem around here, the neighbours get together to deal with it’). Scores for these items were summed to create a total score for social cohesion, ranging from 0–15. For the present study, social cohesion was dichotomized at the first quartile (i.e. comparing those in low-cohesion neighbourhoods to all others).

Covariates

Adolescent sex was reported by the primary caregiver at T1.

Caregivers reported on adolescent ethnicity (white/non-white) at T1. When possible, data from earlier cycles was carried forward by Statistics Canada to replace missing data on this variable.

Depressive symptoms in the primary caregiver were assessed at T1 using the Depression Rating Scale, a shortened 12-item version of the CES-D (Radloff, Reference Radloff1977). For the present study, caregiver depression was operationalized as a score in the top 10% on the depression scale.

Family poverty was assessed using the ratio of income to the corresponding low-income cut-off (LICO). LICO is defined as the income below which a family would have difficulty making ends meet, and is based on family size and geographic area (Statistics Canada, 2009). For the present analyses, this ratio was dichotomized at 1 (i.e. comparing families with incomes below and above the low-income cut-off).

Family composition was reported by the primary caregiver at T1. For the present analysis, we dichotomized this variable to consider adolescents living with two biological parents v. those living in other family structures (e.g. step-parent families, single-parent families, foster families).

Primary caregivers reported on their levels of social support using the 8-item social support scale. Caregivers rated their agreement with each item (e.g. ‘There are people I can count on in an emergency’) on a 4-point scale, from ‘strongly agree’ to ‘strongly disagree’. For these analyses, social support was dichotomized at the bottom quartile.

Finally, perceived neighbourhood safety was assessed in the NLSCY using a 3-item scale. Caregivers rated their agreement with each statement (‘It is safe to walk alone in this neighbourhood after dark’; ‘It is safe for children to play outside during the day’; ‘There are safe parks, playgrounds, and play spaces in this neighbourhood’) on a 4-point scale, from ‘strongly agree’ to ‘strongly disagree’. Scores on neighbourhood safety were trichotomized to reflect low (bottom 25%), average (middle 50%), and high (top 25%) safety.

Analysis

Separate multivariable logistic regression models were estimated for each mental health outcome. First, we established whether SLE exposure was associated with each outcome by fitting a model including baseline mental illness symptoms, stressful life events, neighbourhood social cohesion, ethnicity, sex, caregiver depression and family poverty. p < 0.05 was considered to be statistically significant. To test the modifying effect of neighbourhood social cohesion on the association between SLE exposure and each outcome, we fitted an interaction term between SLE exposure and social cohesion, and tested whether this improved model fit via Score χ2 tests. In the presence of effect modification, we reported stratified effects of SLEs on each outcome, in low and higher social cohesion neighbourhoods, as defined above. Normalized survey weights based on derived weights generated by Statistics Canada were used to take into account the complex survey design. Cases with missing data on the exposure (SLEs) or effect modifier (neighbourhood cohesion) were listwise deleted. All analyses were conducted using SAS software (version 9.3, SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA).

Results

Adolescents who had experienced SLEs in the past 2 years were more likely to be white, have a depressed primary caregiver, have a family income below the corresponding low-income cut-off, and were less likely to be living with two biological parents (Table 1).

Table 1. Sample characteristics by exposure to stressful life events (SLEs) (weighted percentages)a

a Raw frequencies in each cell are not given, in accordance with Statistics Canada guidelines

Predicting adolescent mental health

The available sample size with complete data varied depending on the outcome investigated, from 3629 for hyperactivity to 3776 for suicidality (Fig. 1). Those missing data on T1 variables (cohesion, baseline mental health symptoms) were more likely to be male, non-white, living with a depressed caregiver, living in poverty, in low safety neighbourhoods, have a caregiver with low social support, and less likely to be living with 2 biological parents (online Supplementary eTable S1). Those who dropped out between T1 and T2 were more likely to be male, white, and to live in neighbourhoods characterized by low cohesion and low safety (online Supplementary eTable S2).

Fig. 1. Sample size flow diagram.

Depression/anxiety

In our main model, living in a cohesive neighbourhood was protective against adolescent depression/anxiety (OR 0.62, 95% CI 0.43–0.90; Table 2). There was a significant interaction between SLEs and neighbourhood cohesion (χ2 = 14.98, p < 0.001; Table 3). In low-cohesion neighbourhoods, SLEs were significantly and positively associated with adolescent depression/anxiety (OR 3.11, 95% CI 1.64–5.90), but no effect was observed in higher cohesion neighbourhoods (OR 0.99, 95% CI 0.71–1.37).

Table 2. Results of logistic regression predicting adolescent mental health outcomes

OR, Odds ratio; 95% CI, 95% confidence intervals.

Note: Statistically significant effects are shown in bold.

Table 3. Effects of stressful life events on adolescent mental health outcomes, stratified by level of neighbourhood cohesiona

95% CI, 95% confidence interval.

Note: significant χ2s and odds ratios (ORs) are presented in bold.

a Adjusted for child sex, ethnicity, caregiver depression, and family poverty.

Suicidal ideation

Adolescents who had experienced SLEs in the past two years were more likely to report suicidal ideation than those who had not (OR 1.54, 95% CI 1.14–2.07). However, this main effect was superseded by a significant interaction between SLEs and neighbourhood cohesion (χ2 = 7.52, p = .006); thus, the effect of SLEs on adolescent suicidal ideation was substantially greater in low social cohesion neighbourhoods (OR 5.25, 95% CI 2.23–1.32) than higher cohesion neighbourhoods (OR 1.30, 95% CI 0.94–1.81, Table 3).

Suicide attempt

Similarly to our findings for suicidal ideation, adolescents who had experienced SLEs in the past two years were also more likely to have attempted suicide than those who had not (OR 1.74, 95% CI 1.16–2.59). The interaction between SLEs and neighbourhood social cohesion was not statistically significant (χ2 = 2.79, p = 0.094; Table 3); however, inspection of the stratified results suggested that SLEs had a significant effect on suicide attempt among adolescents in low cohesion neighbourhoods (OR 3.02, 95% CI 1.24–7.37) but not higher cohesion neighbourhoods (OR 1.44, 95% CI 0.90–2.29).

Aggression/conduct disorder

Adolescents who had experienced SLEs had higher odds of elevated aggressive/conduct symptoms (OR 1.44, 95% CI 1.08–1.91). The interaction between SLEs and neighbourhood cohesion was also statistically significant (χ2 = 10.90, p < 0.001), and as for most other outcomes, the effect of SLEs was stronger in low-cohesion neighbourhoods (OR 4.49, 95% CI 2.23–8.19), but absent in higher cohesion neighbourhoods (Table 3).

Property offence

Similarly, the experience of SLEs was associated with increased risk of property offence (OR 1.50, 95% CI 1.14–1.86), with strong evidence of an interaction between SLEs and neighbourhood cohesion (χ2 = 8.68, p = .003) suggesting that this association was significantly stronger in low social cohesion neighbourhoods (OR 4.21, 95% CI 2.28–7.76) than in higher social cohesion neighbourhoods (OR 1.21, 95% CI 0.92–1.60, Table 3).

Hyperactivity

Neither stressful life events, neighbourhood social cohesion, nor their interaction significantly predicted adolescent hyperactivity (Tables 2 and 3).

Sensitivity analysis

We conducted a post hoc sensitivity analysis exploring the effects of exposure to SLEs across three levels of neighbourhood social cohesion (bottom 25%; middle 50%; top 25%). Results (online Supplementary eTable S3) suggested that the moderating effects of social cohesion were largely constrained to the lowest quartile: SLEs were significantly associated with mental health and behavioural outcomes at the lowest levels of social cohesion, but not in moderate or high cohesion neighbourhoods.

Discussion

The association between SLEs and four out of six major mental health or behavioural outcomes in young adolescents was stronger amongst those living in low social cohesion neighbourhoods than higher social cohesion neighbourhoods measured two years earlier in this longitudinal cohort study. A trend to this effect was found for a fifth outcome, suicide attempts, although no discernable effects were apparent for our final outcome, hyperactivity. Associations between exposure to SLEs and psychiatric symptoms were attenuated to the null for adolescents living in neighbourhoods with higher levels of social cohesion. These results could not be explained by differences in income, sex, ethnicity, family structure, social support, neighbourhood safety, mental health at baseline, or depression in the primary caregiver. The consistency of our results suggest that neighbourhood social cohesion may effectively buffer children and adolescents from the otherwise potentially deleterious effects that SLEs can have on future mental health and behavioural problems. If causal, our findings strongly suggest that efforts to improve neighbourhood social cohesion, specifically here, amongst teenagers, will have positive effects on future mental health.

Neighbourhood social cohesion and mental health

Social cohesion was, on its own, associated with only one of the adolescent mental health outcomes assessed – symptoms of depression and anxiety. Across the majority of outcomes, higher social cohesion appeared to buffer the effects of exposure to SLEs. Among adolescents residing in neighbourhoods characterized by low social cohesion, the recent experience of SLEs was associated with increased risk of depression/anxiety, suicide ideation and attempt, aggression/conduct disorder, and property offence. In higher cohesion neighbourhoods, the effects of SLEs on these psychiatric symptoms were attenuated to the null. Exposure to SLEs in childhood and adolescence has been consistently linked to later psychiatric illness (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Davis and Kendler1997; Anda et al., Reference Anda, Felitti, Bremner, Walker, Whitfield, Perry, Dube and Giles2006; Schilling et al., Reference Schilling, Aseltine and Gore2007), including internalizing(Chapman et al., Reference Chapman, Whitfield, Felitti, Dube, Edwards and Anda2004; Schilling et al., Reference Schilling, Aseltine and Gore2007) and externalizing problems (Schilling et al., Reference Schilling, Aseltine and Gore2007; Baglivio et al., Reference Baglivio, Epps, Swartz, Sayedul Huq, Sheer and Hardt2014), and suicidal behaviour (Isohookana et al., Reference Isohookana, Riala, Hakko and Räsänen2013). Few studies have previously investigated whether neighbourhood social cohesion buffers such stressors, although one study showed that higher perceived neighbourhood cohesion attenuated the effects of maternal hostility on child externalizing behaviours, including symptoms of conduct disorder and property offences (Silk et al., Reference Silk, Sessa, Morris, Steinberg and Avenevoli2004). Two further studies from the same sample have shown that greater neighbourhood social cohesion moderates the effects of childhood poly-victimization on early and late adolescent psychotic symptoms (Crush et al., Reference Crush, Arseneault, Jaffee, Danese and Fisher2018a, Reference Crush, Arseneault, Moffitt, Danese, Caspi, Jaffee, Matthews and Fisher2018b). Our results suggest similar mechanisms may be at play with regards to neighbourhood social cohesion.

At the individual level, social support has long been hypothesized to buffer against the effects of stress (Cohen and Wills, Reference Cohen and Wills1985) on mental health. Evidence suggests this may operate in at least two ways (Kawachi and Berkman, Reference Kawachi and Berkman2001). First, the perceived availability of social support can lead to more benign cognitive appraisals of stressors as they are encountered, and second, the experience of social support during a time of stress can lead to a dampening of the behavioural and even physiological responses to stressors (Kawachi and Berkman, Reference Kawachi and Berkman2001). Similar mechanisms may apply to adolescents living in socially cohesive neighbourhoods following exposure to an SLE. Notably, our results were not explained by caregiver social support at the individual level, suggesting that social processes operating at the wider neighbourhood environment may be at play. It is also possible that those living in more socially cohesive neighbourhoods benefit from social learning via increased exposure to multiple adult role models, or more generally from positive emotional and instrumental support between neighbours (Silk et al., Reference Silk, Sessa, Morris, Steinberg and Avenevoli2004). Our findings suggest further longitudinal research is warranted to tease out potential pathways between SLEs, social cohesion and adolescent mental health.

Alternate interpretations

Results of sensitivity analysis suggested that the moderating effects of social cohesion were most pronounced for children living in neighborhoods with the lowest levels of social cohesion; that is, there may be a threshold of social cohesion above which additional incremental improvements have little effect on resilience. Alternately, these results can be viewed as evidence for a ‘double disadvantage’ effect, whereby the deleterious effects of life stressors on mental health are only evident among adolescents additionally exposed to suboptimal neighbourhood conditions. Beyond social cohesion, other neighbourhood factors have also been reported to moderate the associations between acute SLEs and psychiatric outcomes, but only below certain thresholds; for example, strong associations between SLEs and increased aggression appear to be restricted to children living in the most economically disadvantaged neighbourhoods (Attar et al., Reference Attar, Guerra and Tolan1994). Importantly, our findings were impervious to adjustment for neighbourhood safety, lending credence to the possibility that neighbourhood social cohesion had moderating effects on various mental health outcomes following exposure to SLEs, over and above the influence of neighbourhood structural disadvantage. Whether these results are interpreted as a buffering effect of higher levels of social cohesion, or an amplification of the negative effects of SLEs by low social cohesion, they nonetheless suggest that improving low social cohesion may have beneficial consequences for youth exposed to life stress.

Strengths and limitations

We acknowledge some limitations to the present study. Caregiver report of neighbourhood social cohesion may reflect certain aspects of their personality and behaviour, introducing bias. For example, parents who are actively involved in the community may also be more likely to promote adaptive behaviour in their children. We sought to minimise this by adjusting for caregiver depression and individual-level social support, however, we were unable to control for other aspects of the primary caregiver's mental health and behaviour, including parenting practices. Adolescent psychiatric symptoms were assessed using self-report scales, and as such may not reflect psychiatric diagnoses. Although these scales were designed to correspond to DSM-III criteria, they are not intended as diagnostic instruments. SLEs were assessed via retrospective caregiver report between T1 and T2. While such data are potentially subject to recall bias (Colman et al., Reference Colman, Kingsbury, Garad, Zeng, Naicker, Patten, Jones, Wild and Thompson2016), it has been suggested that self reports can be reliable for relatively rare and important events (i.e. death, divorce) (Schwarz, Reference Schwarz, Stone, Shiffman, Atienza and Nebeling2007). The short timeframe for recall in the present study also increases confidence in the reliability of the reports. For certain events (i.e. abuse, fear of abuse or parental alcohol abuse), caregivers may have withheld information out of fear of recrimination or social desirability. Future studies may need to use a multi-informant approach to assess exposure to stressful life events in a more objective way. Finally, we did not differentiate between the 13 different types of stressors assessed. However, studies examining multiple types of adverse childhood experiences have reported largely non-specific effects on mental health (Schilling et al., Reference Schilling, Aseltine and Gore2007).

These limitations were balanced by notable strengths. Our study leveraged data from a large population-based prospective sample of adolescents. Additionally, the use of prospectively collected data and adjustment for baseline symptoms allowed for clarity in the temporality of relationships between neighbourhood cohesion and mental health.

Public health implications

The consistency of our findings strengthens the possibility that neighbourhood cohesion in early adolescence may mitigate mental health problems for teenagers exposed to stressful life events in childhood. Given adolescence is a key period for the emergence of mental health disorders (Patton et al., Reference Patton, Sawyer, Santelli, Ross, Afifi, Allen, Arora, Azzopardi, Baldwin, Bonell, Kakuma, Kennedy, Mahon, McGovern, Mokdad, Patel, Petroni, Reavley, Taiwo, Waldfogel, Wickremarathne, Barroso, Bhutta, Fatusi, Mattoo, Diers, Fang, Ferguson, Ssewamala and Viner2016), which often predicts worse adulthood physical, mental and social outcomes (Naicker et al., Reference Naicker, Galambos, Zeng, Senthilselvan and Colman2013; Patton et al., Reference Patton, Coffey, Romaniuk, Mackinnon, Carlin, Degenhardt, Olsson and Moran2014), identifying modifiable prevention targets is a central public mental health concern. We suggest that selected intervention strategies to promote social integration amongst youth who have recently experienced SLEs could be warranted, particularly given that over 1 in 5 adolescents in our sample had experienced at least one SLE in the two years before assessment. These could include helping such individuals develop and maintain peer relationships, known to be of central importance to adolescent health and well-being (Patton et al., Reference Patton, Sawyer, Santelli, Ross, Afifi, Allen, Arora, Azzopardi, Baldwin, Bonell, Kakuma, Kennedy, Mahon, McGovern, Mokdad, Patel, Petroni, Reavley, Taiwo, Waldfogel, Wickremarathne, Barroso, Bhutta, Fatusi, Mattoo, Diers, Fang, Ferguson, Ssewamala and Viner2016), or by establishing or enhancing school- or community-based initiatives which promote conditions for greater prosocial behaviours (van den Bos et al., Reference van den Bos, Crone, Meuwese and Güroğlu2018) and social cohesion (Donnelly et al., Reference Donnelly, McLanahan, Brooks-Gunn, Garfinkel, Wagner, Jacobsen, Gold and Gaydosh2016).

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719001235.

Author ORCIDs

Ian Colman, 0000-0001-5924-0277.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the NLSCY participants, whose participation made this study possible. This paper is based on data from Statistics Canada, and the opinions expressed do not represent the views of Statistics Canada.

Financial support

This work was supported by a Sir Henry Dale Fellowship for JBK, jointly funded by the Wellcome Trust and the Royal Society (Grant No. 101272/Z/13/Z). This research was undertaken, in part, thanks to funding from the Canada Research Chairs program for IC.

Conflict of interest

None.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Footnotes

*

Authors contributed equally to the project

References

Afifi, TO, Enns, MW, Cox, BJ, Asmundson, GJG, Stein, MB and Sareen, J (2008) Population attributable fractions of psychiatric disorders and suicide ideation and attempts associated with adverse childhood experiences. American Journal of Public Health 98, 946952.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anda, RF, Felitti, VJ, Bremner, JD, Walker, JD, Whitfield, C, Perry, BD, Dube, SR and Giles, WH (2006) The enduring effects of abuse and related adverse experiences in childhood. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 256, 174186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Araya, R, Dunstan, F, Playle, R, Thomas, H, Palmer, S and Lewis, G (2006) Perceptions of social capital and the built environment and mental health. Social Science & Medicine 62, 30723083.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Attar, BK, Guerra, NG and Tolan, PH (1994) Neighborhood disadvantage, stressful life events and adjustments in urban elementary-school children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 23, 391400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baglivio, MT, Epps, N, Swartz, K, Sayedul Huq, M, Sheer, A and Hardt, NS (2014) The prevalence of adverse childhood experiences (ACE) in the lives of juvenile offenders. OJJDP Journal of Juvenile Justice 3, 118.Google Scholar
Boyle, MH, Offord, DR, Hofmann, HG, Catlin, GP, Byles, JA, Cadman, DT, Crawford, JW, Links, PS, Rae-Grant, NI and Szatmari, P (1987) Ontario Child Health Study: I. Methodology. Archives of General Psychiatry 44, 826831.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, DP, Whitfield, CL, Felitti, VJ, Dube, SR, Edwards, VJ and Anda, RF (2004) Adverse childhood experiences and the risk of depressive disorders in adulthood. Journal of Affective Disorders 82, 217225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D and Rogosch, FA (1997) The role of self-organization in the promotion of resilience in maltreated children. Development and Psychopathology 9, 797815.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, S and Wills, TA (1985) Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin 98, 310357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colman, I, Murray, J, Abbott, RA, Maughan, B, Kuh, D, Croudace, TJ and Jones, PB (2009) Outcomes of conduct problems in adolescence: 40 Year follow-up of national cohort. BMJ (Online) 338, 208211.Google ScholarPubMed
Colman, I, Kingsbury, M, Garad, Y, Zeng, Y, Naicker, K, Patten, S, Jones, PB, Wild, TC and Thompson, AH (2016) Consistency in adult reporting of adverse childhood experiences. Psychological Medicine 46, 543549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crush, E, Arseneault, L, Jaffee, SR, Danese, A and Fisher, HL (2018 a) Protective factors for psychotic symptoms among poly-victimized children. Schizophrenia Bulletin 44, 691700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crush, E, Arseneault, L, Moffitt, TE, Danese, A, Caspi, A, Jaffee, SR, Matthews, T and Fisher, HL (2018 b) Protective factors for psychotic experiences amongst adolescents exposed to multiple forms of victimization. Journal of Psychiatric Research 104, 3238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curtis, LJ, Dooley, MD and Phipps, SA (2004) Child well-being and neighbourhood quality: evidence from the Canadian National Longitudinal Survey of Children and Youth. Social Science & Medicine 58, 19171927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Sanctis, VA, Nomura, Y, Newcorn, JH and Halperin, JM (2012) Childhood maltreatment and conduct disorder: Independent predictors of criminal outcomes in ADHD youth. Child Abuse and Neglect 36, 782789.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Silva, MJ, McKenzie, K, Harpham, T and Huttly, SR (2005) Social capital and mental illness: a systematic review. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 59, 619627.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donnelly, L, McLanahan, S, Brooks-Gunn, J, Garfinkel, I, Wagner, BG, Jacobsen, WC, Gold, S and Gaydosh, L (2016) Cohesive neighborhoods where social expectations are shared may have positive impact on adolescent mental health. Health Affairs 35, 20832091.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Echeverría, S, Diez-Roux, AV, Shea, S, Borrell, LN and Jackson, S (2008) Associations of neighborhood problems and neighborhood social cohesion with mental health and health behaviors: the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis. Health & Place 14, 853865.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isohookana, R, Riala, K, Hakko, H and Räsänen, P (2013) Adverse childhood experiences and suicidal behavior of adolescent psychiatric inpatients. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 22, 1322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaffee, SR, Caspi, A, Moffitt, TE, Dodge, KA, Rutter, M, Taylor, A and Tully, LA (2005) Nature X nurture: Genetic vulnerabilities interact with physical maltreatment to promote conduct problems. Development and Psychopathology 17, 6784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaffee, SR, Caspi, A, Moffitt, TE, Polo-Tomás, M and Taylor, A (2007) Individual, family, and neighborhood factors distinguish resilient from non-resilient maltreated children: a cumulative stressors model. Child Abuse and Neglect 31, 231253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kawachi, I and Berkman, LF (2001) Social ties and mental health. Journal of Urban Health-Bulletin of the New York Academy of Medicine 78, 458467.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, RC, Davis, CG and Kendler, KS (1997) Childhood adversity and adult psychiatric disorder in the US National Comorbidity Survey. Psychological Medicine 27, 11011119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kingsbury, M, Kirkbride, JB, McMartin, SE, Wickham, ME, Weeks, M and Colman, I (2015) Trajectories of childhood neighbourhood cohesion and adolescent mental health: Evidence from a national Canadian cohort. Psychological Medicine 45, 32393248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lansford, JE, Dodge, KA, Pettit, GS, Bates, JE, Crozier, J and Kaplow, J (2002) A 12-year prospective study of the long-term effects of early child physical maltreatment on psychological, behavioral, and academic problems in adolescence. Archives of Pediatrics 156, 824830.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McMartin, SE, Kingsbury, M, Dykxhoorn, J and Colman, I (2014) Time trends in symptoms of mental illness in children and adolescents in Canada. Canadian Medical Association Journal 186, E672E678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michl, LC, McLaughlin, KA, Shepherd, K and Nolen-Hoeksema, S (2013) Rumination as a mechanism linking stressful life events to symptoms of depression and anxiety: longitudinal evidence in early adolescents and adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 122, 339352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naicker, K, Galambos, NL, Zeng, Y, Senthilselvan, A and Colman, I (2013) Social, demographic, and health outcomes in the 10 years following adolescent depression. Journal of Adolescent Health 52, 533538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patton, GC, Coffey, C, Romaniuk, H, Mackinnon, A, Carlin, JB, Degenhardt, L, Olsson, CA and Moran, P (2014) The prognosis of common mental disorders in adolescents: a 14-year prospective cohort study. The Lancet 383, 14041411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patton, GC, Sawyer, SM, Santelli, JS, Ross, DA, Afifi, R, Allen, NB, Arora, M, Azzopardi, P, Baldwin, W, Bonell, C, Kakuma, R, Kennedy, E, Mahon, J, McGovern, T, Mokdad, AH, Patel, V, Petroni, S, Reavley, N, Taiwo, K, Waldfogel, J, Wickremarathne, D, Barroso, C, Bhutta, Z, Fatusi, AO, Mattoo, A, Diers, J, Fang, J, Ferguson, J, Ssewamala, F and Viner, RM (2016) Our future: a Lancet commission on adolescent health and wellbeing. The Lancet 387, 24232478.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radloff, LS (1977) The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement 1, 385401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sampson, RJ (2003) The neighborhood context of well-being. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine 46, S53S64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schilling, EA, Aseltine, RH and Gore, S (2007) Adverse childhood experiences and mental health in young adults: a longitudinal survey. BioMed Central BMC Public Health 7, 30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwarz, N (2007) Retrospective and concurrent self-reports: the rationale for real-time data capture. In Stone, AA, Shiffman, S, Atienza, AA and Nebeling, L (eds), The Science of Real-Time Data Capture: Self-Reports in Health Research. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 1126.Google Scholar
Silk, JS, Sessa, FM, Morris, AS, Steinberg, L and Avenevoli, S (2004) Neighborhood cohesion as a buffer against hostile maternal parenting. Journal of Family Psychology 18, 135146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solmi, F, Colman, I, Weeks, M, Lewis, G and Kirkbride, JB (2017) Trajectories of neighborhood cohesion in childhood, and psychotic and depressive symptoms at age 13 and 18 years. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 56, 570577.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Statistics Canada (2009) Microdata User Guide, National Longitudinal Survey of Children and Youth, Cycle 8, September 2008 to July 2009. Available at http://www23.statcan.gc.ca/imdb-bmdi/document/4450_D4_T9_V8-eng.pdf [Accessed May 31, 2018].Google Scholar
van den Bos, W, Crone, EA, Meuwese, R and Güroğlu, B (2018) Social network cohesion in school classes promotes prosocial behavior. PLOS ONE 13, e0194656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample characteristics by exposure to stressful life events (SLEs) (weighted percentages)a

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Sample size flow diagram.

Figure 2

Table 2. Results of logistic regression predicting adolescent mental health outcomes

Figure 3

Table 3. Effects of stressful life events on adolescent mental health outcomes, stratified by level of neighbourhood cohesiona

Supplementary material: File

Kingsbury et al. supplementary material

Kingsbury et al. supplementary material 1

Download Kingsbury et al. supplementary material(File)
File 113.8 KB