Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b95js Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T20:42:41.683Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neural correlates of emotional processing in trauma-related narratives

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 February 2025

Minne Cao
Affiliation:
Affiliated Mental Health Center & Hangzhou Seventh People’s Hospital, Zhejiang University School of Medicine, Hangzhou, Zhejiang, China
Shengnan Zhu
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, Zhejiang University, China
Enze Tang
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, Zhejiang University, China
Chuang Xue
Affiliation:
Affiliated Mental Health Center & Hangzhou Seventh People’s Hospital, Zhejiang University School of Medicine, Hangzhou, Zhejiang, China
Kun Li
Affiliation:
Shandong Daizhuang Hospital, Jining, China
Hua Yu
Affiliation:
Affiliated Mental Health Center & Hangzhou Seventh People’s Hospital, Zhejiang University School of Medicine, Hangzhou, Zhejiang, China
Tao Zhong
Affiliation:
Affiliated Mental Health Center & Hangzhou Seventh People’s Hospital, Zhejiang University School of Medicine, Hangzhou, Zhejiang, China
Tao Li
Affiliation:
Affiliated Mental Health Center & Hangzhou Seventh People’s Hospital, Zhejiang University School of Medicine, Hangzhou, Zhejiang, China Liangzhu Laboratory, MOE Frontier Science Center for Brain Science and Brain–Machine Integration, State Key Laboratory of Brain–Machine Intelligence, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China NHC and CAMS Key Laboratory of Medical Neurobiology, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China
Hui Chen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, Zhejiang University, China
Wei Deng*
Affiliation:
Affiliated Mental Health Center & Hangzhou Seventh People’s Hospital, Zhejiang University School of Medicine, Hangzhou, Zhejiang, China Liangzhu Laboratory, MOE Frontier Science Center for Brain Science and Brain–Machine Integration, State Key Laboratory of Brain–Machine Intelligence, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China
*
Corresponding author: Wei Deng; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a mental health condition caused by the dysregulation or overgeneralization of memories related to traumatic events. Investigating the interplay between explicit narrative and implicit emotional memory contributes to a better understanding of the mechanisms underlying PTSD.

Methods

This case–control study focused on two groups: unmedicated patients with PTSD and a trauma-exposed control (TEC) group who did not develop PTSD. Experiments included real-time measurements of blood oxygenation changes using functional near-infrared spectroscopy during trauma narration and processing of emotional and linguistic data through natural language processing (NLP).

Results

Real-time fNIRS monitoring showed that PTSD patients (mean [SD] Oxy-Hb activation, 0.153 [0.084], 95% CI 0.124 to 0.182) had significantly higher brain activity in the left anterior medial prefrontal cortex (L-amPFC) within 10 s after expressing negative emotional words compared with the control group (0.047 [0.026], 95% CI 0.038 to 0.056; p < 0.001). In the control group, there was a significant time-series correlation between the use of negative emotional memory words and activation of the L-amPFC (latency 3.82 s, slope = 0.0067, peak value = 0.184, difference = 0.273; Spearman’s r = 0.727, p < 0.001). In contrast, the left anterior cingulate prefrontal cortex of PTSD patients remained in a state of high activation (peak value = 0.153, difference = 0.084) with no apparent latency period.

Conclusions

PTSD patients display overactivity in pathways associated with rapid emotional responses and diminished regulation in cognitive processing areas. Interventions targeting these pathways may alleviate symptoms of PTSD.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a psychiatric disorder stemming from the dysregulation or overgeneralization of memories associated with traumatic events, characterized clinically by symptoms of avoidance, intrusive experiences, and heightened alertness linked to fear memories (Harrison et al., Reference Harrison, Cowen, Burns and Fazel2018). Epidemiological studies have shown that not all individuals exposed to similarly intense traumatic events develop PTSD. The lifetime prevalence of PTSD is approximately 7.8% (Wisco et al., Reference Wisco, Marx, Wolf, Miller, Southwick and Pietrzak2014), with a 12-month prevalence ranging between 1.3% and 3.6% (Wisco et al., Reference Wisco, Marx, Miller, Wolf, Mota, Krystal and Pietrzak2016), indicating variability in the processing of fear memories among individuals(Harrison et al., Reference Harrison, Cowen, Burns and Fazel2018). Traumatic incidents elicit strong emotional reactions such as anger, fear, sadness, and shame, which can be reflected through language (Krüger-Gottschalk et al., Reference Krüger-Gottschalk, Ehring, Knaevelsrud, Dyer, Schäfer, Schellong and Köhler2022). Miragoli and Jennings’s works have indicated that individuals with PTSD use negative and neutral emotional words more frequently than those without PTSD (Jennings et al., Reference Jennings, Soder, Wardle, Schmitz and Vujanovic2021; Miragoli et al., Reference Miragoli, Procaccia and Di Blasio2014). Additionally, some individuals may try to avoid emotional outbursts when confronted with traumatic memories, leading to emotional numbness or the use of substances to dodge unwanted emotions (Brem et al., Reference Brem, Shorey, Anderson and Stuart2018). Thus, understanding the synergistic role of explicit narrative and implicit emotional memory in the processing of traumatic events is crucial for revealing the mechanisms underlying PTSD formation.

Patients with PTSD exhibit certain characteristics in their descriptions of traumatic events, including slower, more monotonous speech with less variation in tone and activation (Marmar et al., Reference Marmar, Brown, Qian, Laska, Siegel, Li and Vergyri2019). These features reflect the difficulties they face in processing emotional information. Specifically, this language pattern may be influenced by emotional states, agitation, and anxiety levels (Marmar et al., Reference Marmar, Brown, Qian, Laska, Siegel, Li and Vergyri2019), illustrating the impact of implicit emotional memory on explicit narrative. Greater use of bodily sensations, perceptual details, and negative emotional words (Ng et al., Reference Ng, Ahishakiye, Miller and Meyerowitz2015), as well as frequent use of singular pronouns (D’Andrea et al., Reference D’Andrea, Chiu, Casas and Deldin2012) and death-related terms in trauma narratives (Papini et al., Reference Papini, Yoon, Rubin, Lopez-Castro and Hien2015), reflect individuals’ emotional responses during trauma such as fear, helplessness, horror, guilt, and shame (Kleim et al., Reference Kleim, Horn, Kraehenmann, Mehl and Ehlers2018; Ozer et al., Reference Ozer, Best, Lipsey and Weiss2003). These features represent not just surface-level descriptions of traumatic events but are outcomes of deep emotional processing. Therefore, it can be posited that explicit narratives are the result of processing traumatic events, influenced significantly by implicit emotional memories, and these two aspects interact synergistically.

At the neural level, studies have explored PTSD’s neural correlates by activating traumatic memories through script-driven imagery. Exposure to traumatic scripts results in decreased functional connectivity between the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and the amygdala (Shin et al., Reference Shin, Orr, Carson, Rauch, Macklin, Lasko and Pitman2004). The anterior cingulate cortex, frontal lobes, and thalamus are also implicated in PTSD neural circuits (Lanius et al., Reference Lanius, Williamson, Densmore, Boksman, Gupta, Neufeld and Menon2001). Additionally, PTSD patients may exhibit functional dysregulation in the mPFC, hippocampus, and visual association cortex (Bremner et al., Reference Bremner, Narayan, Staib, Southwick, McGlashan and Charney1999; Rauch, Reference Rauch1996), as well as reduced functional connectivity strength in the prefrontal-limbic-thalamic-cerebellar circuitry (He et al., Reference He, Fan, Liu, Wang, Zhang, Zhang and Xie2022). These findings suggest abnormal top-down emotional regulation processes in PTSD patients when facing traumatic events.

Notably, studies in healthy populations have shown that contextual processing, semantic tasks, and memory retrieval activate the anterior mPFC (amPFC), as well as the hippocampus and parahippocampal gyrus (Foudil & Macaluso, Reference Foudil and Macaluso2024; Lee & McCarthy, Reference Lee and McCarthy2015). Activation in these regions is closely associated with episodic memory and emotional processing (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Davies, Gerlach, Cooper, Stevens and Craske2018), indicating the amPFC’s significant role in processing traumatic events. It is speculated that PTSD patients may exhibit functional abnormalities in these areas, affecting emotional and memory processing.

According to implicit emotional learning theory, top-down regulation involves two pathways: the low pathway offers a fast but imprecise emotional response route, while the high pathway entails complex perceptual and cognitive processing, leading to more refined and adaptive emotional responses (Gazzaniga & Mangun, Reference Gazzaniga and Mangun2014). Typically, trauma-related fear memories are first encoded in the amygdala, then integrated and consolidated in the hippocampus (Albo & Gräff, Reference Albo and Gräff2018; Dudai, Reference Dudai2004; Frankland & Bontempi, Reference Frankland and Bontempi2005; Goto et al., Reference Goto, Bota, Miya, Wang, Tsukamoto, Jiang and Hayashi2021; Kitamura et al., Reference Kitamura, Ogawa, Roy, Okuyama, Morrissey, Smith and Tonegawa2017), and ultimately achieve long-term storage in the prefrontal cortex (Goto et al., Reference Goto, Bota, Miya, Wang, Tsukamoto, Jiang and Hayashi2021; Sun et al., Reference Sun, Bernstein, Meng, Rao, Sørensen, Yao and Lin2020). The mPFC plays a crucial role in the maintenance, expression, and extinction of fear memory by coordinating emotional responses through the regulation of network connections with the amygdala (Karalis et al., Reference Karalis, Dejean, Chaudun, Khoder, Rozeske, Wurtz and Herry2016). However, PTSD patients may experience dysregulation in this process (Barkay et al., Reference Barkay, Freedman, Lester, Louzoun, Sapoznikov, Luckenbaugh and Bonne2012), resulting in decreased cognitive control over negative emotions and an intensified experience of negative emotions (He et al., Reference He, Fan, Liu, Wang, Zhang, Zhang and Xie2022).

To further investigate the regulatory mechanisms of implicit emotional memory during explicit narrative processes in PTSD patients, this study focuses on the amPFC as the region of interest. We employ functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to measure real-time brain activation levels during self-recalled trauma narratives. Additionally, we analyze narrative content characteristics to explore the relationship between explicit narratives and implicit emotional memory.

fNIRS offers high temporal resolution and tolerance to motion artifacts, making it suitable for immediate brain activity detection during language tasks (Curtin et al., Reference Curtin, Tong, Sun, Wang, Onaral and Ayaz2019; Mazziotti et al., Reference Mazziotti, Scaffei, Conti, Marchi, Rizzi, Cioni and Baroncelli2022). Moreover, fNIRS operates silently, avoiding environmental noise interference with speech tasks and recordings (Hagenaar et al., Reference Hagenaar, Bindels-de Heus, Van Gils, Van Den Berg, Ten Hoopen, Affourtit and Mous2024), aligning well with the design requirements of this study.

Based on this background, the following hypotheses are proposed: (1) during trauma recall, PTSD patients rely more on explicit narratives of negative emotional memories and experience greater emotional distress during expression compared with the control group. (2) There is a time-series correlation between content features of explicit narratives (such as the use of negative emotional memory words) and activation of the amPFC. (3) PTSD patients exhibit sustained high activation of the amPFC during trauma recall narratives, particularly when expressing negative emotions, with higher activation levels than those in the control group.

Methods

Participants

An a priori power analysis was performed using G*Power 3.1 to determine the required sample size for the study (Faul et al., Reference Faul, Erdfelder, Lang and Buchner2007). Anticipating an effect size of Cohen’s d = 0.7—based on previous studies involving neuroimaging and language measures between patient and control groups—and setting a significance level (α) of 0.05 and power (1 − β) of 0.80 for a two-tailed test, the analysis indicated that at least 34 participants were needed per group (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Davies, Gerlach, Cooper, Stevens and Craske2018; De Boer et al., Reference De Boer, Van Hoogdalem, Mandl, Brummelman, Voppel, Begemann and Sommer2020). Accordingly, we recruited 35 patients with PTSD and 37 trauma-exposed controls (TEC).

All participants had experienced traumatic events conforming to the criteria of the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for DSM-5 (CAPS-5) (Weathers et al., Reference Weathers, Blake, Schnurr, Kaloupek, Marx and Keane2015). The PTSD group consisted of 35 unmedicated patients diagnosed by professional physicians according to DSM-5 standards, with CAPS-5 scores equal to or exceeding 45 points. The TEC group included 37 individuals who were trauma-exposed according to DSM-5 PTSD criteria but did not meet any Axis I DSM-5 diagnoses. All participants underwent the same experimental procedures.

Eligibility criteria for participation included: (1) age 18–60; (2) right-handed; (3) native Mandarin speakers; (4) normal hearing and vision, or corrected to normal; (5) no history of brain trauma, epilepsy, or other organic brain disorders. Participants were excluded if they met any of the following criteria: (1) history of head trauma; the presence of organic brain diseases, neurological disorders, or severe endocrine or metabolic diseases; (2) diagnosis of other mental disorders according to DSM-5 criteria, including schizophrenia spectrum and other psychotic disorders, bipolar and related disorders, depressive disorders, anxiety disorders, obsessive–compulsive and related disorders, somatic symptom and related disorders, among others; (3) any severe neurological diseases or impairments, including but not limited to conditions associated with increased intracranial pressure, space-occupying brain lesions, history of epileptic seizures, cerebral aneurysm, Parkinson’s disease, Huntington’s disease, multiple sclerosis, or severe head trauma accompanied by loss of consciousness; (4) alcohol or substance dependence within the past 6 months; (5) received modified electroconvulsive therapy within the past 6 months; (6) pregnancy.

This study was approved by the Affiliated Mental Health Center & Hangzhou Seventh People’s Hospital, Zhejiang University School of Medicine Ethics Committee (Approval numbers: [2022] Ethical Review No. [041] and No. [064]), adhering to the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants signed an informed consent form after fully understanding the study details. The recruitment period was from August 2022 to January 2024.

Data acquisition

Data were acquired using a 48-channel near-infrared optical imaging system (NirScan, Danyang Huichuang Medical Equipment Co., Ltd., China), covering the bilateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) and temporal lobes. The sampling rate was set at 11 Hz, with wavelengths of 730, 808, and 850 nm (730 and 850 nm being the primary measurement wavelengths, 808 nm for isotope correction). Based on the international 10–20 electroencephalogram system, 15 sources and 16 detectors were configured. To ensure adequate coverage of the target brain regions, the detector patches were placed vertically 3 cm above the eyebrows and adjusted using elastic bands on the head to ensure the middle detector was right above the FPz channel.

The experimental environment was in a soundproof room where participants sat comfortably facing a computer screen at a distance of 75 cm. A team member ensured accurate placement of the fNIRS headband on the participant’s head and adjusted it for comfort and good contact between the detectors and the scalp. All channels were checked for signal quality before the experiment began to ensure they met experimental requirements. Participants were instructed to remain still during the experiment.

The experimental content was displayed on a screen with a refresh rate of 60 Hz and a resolution of 2560 × 1440 pixels. Instructions were shown on the screen outlining the experimental process and the requirements participants were expected to follow. Initially, participants sat quietly while their brain activity was measured using fNIRS. During this resting-state measurement, participants were instructed to remain still and avoid any cognitive activity. Throughout data acquisition, participants maintained a comfortable and stable sitting posture, with hands placed naturally on their knees or the armrests of the chair. They were asked to fixate on a ‘+’ at the center of the screen, avoid looking around or engaging in conversation, and minimize body and head movements—including but not limited to raising eyebrows, frowning, moving ears, or scratching the head. After 5 minutes, the screen prompted participants to repeatedly recite the number sequence ‘1, 2, 3, 4, 5’ for 30 s. Subsequently, the screen instructed participants to begin a three-minute impromptu narration of their traumatic event experience, focusing on the worst event as defined by the CAPS-5 (Weathers et al., Reference Weathers, Blake, Schnurr, Kaloupek, Marx and Keane2015). Finally, the screen again prompted participants to recite the number sequence ‘1, 2, 3, 4, 5’ for another 30 s. The narration of the traumatic event was synchronized with audio recording using a wired microphone connected to the computer, positioned 10–20 cm from the source of sound.

Sociodemographic and clinical data processing

Sociodemographic and clinical variables were compared between PTSD and TEC groups. Data normality was assessed using the Shapiro–Wilk test (Shapiro & Wilk, Reference Shapiro and Wilk1965). Statistical analyses were performed using Pathon’s SciPy and pandas libraries. For continuous variables, statistical tests were selected based on data normality: independent samples t-tests were conducted for normally distributed data, and Mann–Whitney U tests were used for non-normally distributed data. Categorical variables were analyzed using Pearson’s Chi-square (χ 2) test.

Language data processing

Audio recordings were processed and analyzed using Python’s pydub library and librosa for acoustic analysis (Table 2). Google Cloud Speech-to-Text API was used to convert recordings into text accurately and generate markers for each word. The Chinese transcription text was processed using the jieba for segmentation. SpaCy was used for part-of-speech tagging and syntactic analysis to compute related linguistic indices (Table 2).

The pre-training phase for emotion analysis used a corpus containing 750MB of text from several major Chinese mental health forums (Li et al., Reference Li, Cao, Fu, Wei, Wang, Yuan and Deng2024). This corpus was used to train word vectors through the large window WordVec algorithm, initializing the model’s word embedding layer. The model featured a hierarchical bi-directional recurrent neural network (biRNN) structure. The embedding vectors of each word are processed through the biRNN, where the forward RNN ( $ \overrightarrow{\mathrm{RNN}} $ ) and backward RNN ( $ \overleftarrow{\mathrm{RNN}} $ ) capture semantic dependencies from left to right and right to left, respectively. The forward and backward hidden states ( $ \overrightarrow{h_i} $ and $ \overleftarrow{h_i} $ ) of each word are calculated and concatenated into $ {h}_i $ . For long texts, sentence-level segmentation is conducted, with each sentence $ {s}_j $ represented by its sequence of word embedding vectors processed through the same biRNN to produce a sentence representation $ {z}_j $ . The sequence of output vectors $ Z=\left\{{z}_1,\right.\left.{z}_2,\dots, {z}_m\right\} $ is then fed into an upper-level RNN ( $ {\mathrm{RNN}}_{\mathrm{top}} $ ), which generates an overall representation of the text. The final output layer of the model has been redesigned to include two parallel softmax layers. These layers predict the presence of emotional and factual vocabulary within the text while retaining marker information associated with each unit. The formula is expressed as follows:

Regarding the input sequence $ X=\left\{{x}_1,\right.\left.{x}_2,\dots, {x}_n\right\} $ , each word $ {x}_i $ is transformed into a vector $ {v}_{\mathrm{i}} $ using the word embedding matrix $ \mathrm{E} $ , and the entire sequence’s hidden states are then computed through the biRNN.

(1) $$ H={\displaystyle \begin{array}{l}\Big[{\left[\overrightarrow{\mathrm{RNN}}\left({v}_1,\dots, {v}_n\right);\overleftarrow{\mathrm{RNN}}\left({v}_n,\dots, {v}_1\right)\right]}_1,\dots, \\ {}{\left[\overrightarrow{\mathrm{RNN}}\left({v}_1,\dots, {v}_n\right);\overleftarrow{\mathrm{RNN}}\left({v}_n,\dots, {v}_1\right)\right]}_n\Big]\end{array}} $$

H is the output sequence processed by the biRNN.

(2) $$ {Y}_{\mathrm{e}\mathrm{motion}}=\mathrm{softmax}\left({W}_{\mathrm{e}}\cdotp {\mathrm{RNN}}_{\mathrm{top}}\left(\mathrm{Pool}\left({H}_{s_1}\right),\dots, \mathrm{Pool}\left({H}_{s_m}\right)\right)+{b}_{\mathrm{e}}\right) $$
(3) $$ {Y}_{\mathrm{f}\mathrm{act}}=\mathrm{softmax}\left({W}_{\mathrm{f}}\cdotp {\mathrm{RNN}}_{\mathrm{top}}\left(\mathrm{Pool}\left({H}_{s_1}\right),\dots, \mathrm{Pool}\left({H}_{s_m}\right)\right)+{b}_{\mathrm{f}}\right) $$

Y represents the final classification outputs, distinguishing between emotional and factual vocabulary. Further, this study utilizes the Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC) to categorize types of emotions, feeding Y emotion into LIWC2015 (McCarthy & Boonthum-Denecke, Reference McCarthy and Boonthum-Denecke2012) to ascertain the proportion of positive and negative emotional vocabulary within the text.

Data processing was performed using NumPy and pandas, and normality tests were conducted using SciPy. Group differences in linguistic features and vocabulary usage were analyzed using appropriate statistical tests based on data normality, as described above. Correlations between the frequency of emotional vocabulary and CAPS-5 symptoms were assessed using Pearson correlation coefficients (stats.pearsonr) for normally distributed data and Spearman’s rank correlation coefficients (stats.spearmanr) for non-normally distributed data.

fNIRS data processing

fNIRS data were preprocessed using NirSpark software. Steps included motion artifact correction, removal of environmental and physiological noise with a 0.01–0.20 Hz bandpass filter, and conversion of optical density values using the modified Beer–Lambert law to compute changes in concentrations of oxyhemoglobin (Oxy-Hb) and deoxyhemoglobin (Deoxy-Hb).

Resting-state data were used to calculate the mean and standard deviation for each channel, and Z-score corrections were applied to data from the standard digit repetition and traumatic event narrative tasks. Markers from the onset of each negative emotional text unit were used to compute mean Oxy-Hb concentration changes within 10 s after the onset, subtracted by the average activation level during digit repetition tasks to determine changes relative to control conditions. Shapiro–Wilk tests assessed data normality. Differences in activation levels between PTSD and TEC groups during traumatic recall were analyzed using Mann–Whitney U tests, with p-values adjusted for false discovery rate (FDR).

Following the between-group comparisons, we extracted channel 27 (CH27), which showed the most significant intergroup difference, as region-of-interest feature variable for in-depth analysis. Precise cortical mapping was performed using the MPFC-ROIsFootnote 1 tool, which identified the corresponding cortical region as the left amPFC (L-amPFC) (Lieberman et al., Reference Lieberman, Straccia, Meyer, Du and Tan2019). To investigate the temporal relationship between CH27 activation and negative emotional processing, we employed Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient in a time-series analysis.

Results

Demographic characteristics

As shown in Table 1, the study included 73 subjects: 35 diagnosed with PTSD according to DSM-5 criteria and 37 TEC. There were no significant differences between groups in terms of age, gender, or educational level. The average duration of illness among PTSD patients was 7.59 (SD = 5.97) years. Clinical evaluations using the CAPS-5 scale showed that the mean score for PTSD patients was 50.89 (SD = 7.57).

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the study sample

N sample size SD standard deviation PTSD post-traumatic stress disorder TEC trauma-exposed controls CAPS-5 Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for DSM-5 ** Indicates significance at the level of α = 0.01.

a Variables not following a normal distribution were analyzed using Mann–Whitney U tests; effect sizes are reported as the absolute value of Cliff’s delta.

b Categorical variables were analyzed using chi-squared tests; effect sizes are reported as the absolute value of Cohen’s h.

Table 2. Description of speech and language variables

* indicated the computation of the feature has been slightly adjusted to accommodate the differences in grammar structures between Chinese and English.

Acoustic and transcription text features

Acoustic analysis of recordings from both the PTSD and control groups (Table 3) revealed significant differences in semitone value (P = 0.001), average pitch (P = 0.033), and average number of pauses (P = 0.040). Specifically, Both groups exhibited negative semitone values, indicating a downward trend in pitch, with a more pronounced decrease in the PTSD group (M = −15.11, SD = 6.53) than the controls (M = −17.10, SD = 6.59). PTSD patients had a higher average pitch (M = 201.21 Hz, SD = 32.84) compared with controls (M = 180.47 Hz, SD = 43.91). PTSD patients (M = 385.09, SD = 118.34) also had slightly more pauses in their narratives compared with controls (M = 362.60, SD = 316.22).

Table 3. Speech and language characteristics between groups

N sample size SD standards deviation * Indicates significance at the level of α = 0.05. ** Indicates significance at the level of α = 0.01.

a Variables following a normal distribution were analyzed using t-tests; effect sizes are reported as the absolute value of Cohen’s d.

b Not included in the group differences test, only displayed for referential purposes.

c Variables not following a normal distribution were analyzed using Mann–Whitney U tests; effect sizes are reported as the absolute value of Cliff’s delta.

Analysis of transcriptions (Table 3) showed a significant difference in Honoré’s R, which reflects lexical richness. The PTSD group had a lower richness in vocabulary usage (M = 30.23, SD = 8.73) compared with controls (M = 34.59, SD = 8.33; P = 0.032). There were no significant differences between the groups in other linguistic forms and structural features.

Language emotional variables

Comparative analysis of language-emotional variables between PTSD patients and controls (Table 3) revealed no significant differences in the use of factual vocabulary. However, analysis of emotional vocabulary indicated that PTSD patients (M = 5.89, SD = 3.00) used negative emotional words significantly more frequently compared with the control group (M = 4.21, SD = 2.83; P = 0.021). There was no significant difference in the use of positive emotional words, though the frequency was slightly higher in the control group (P = 0.372).

Additionally, statistical analysis of the correlation between the frequency of negative emotional words and CAPS-5 scores in PTSD patients and controls was conducted. There was a significant correlation between the total scores of cognitive and mood symptoms under criterion D of the CAPS-5 and the frequency of negative emotional words (r = 0.349, P = 0.037).

Near-infrared brain activation patterns

Oxy-Hb values following the expression of negative emotional words in the PTSD and TEC groups showed significant differences within the 10 s window. Results from Figures 1 and 2 indicated that, at CH27, located in the L-amPFC (Lieberman et al., Reference Lieberman, Straccia, Meyer, Du and Tan2019), PTSD patients exhibited higher levels of Oxy-Hb activation compared with controls, with statistically significant differences after FDR correction (p < 0.001). Specifically, the activation level in CH27 for the PTSD group remained elevated (y max = 0.153, y difference = 0.084) with no apparent latency period. Although there was a slight fluctuation in activation levels during this period, there was no significant correlation with time (Spearman’s r = 0.026, p = 0.785) (Figure 3).

Figure 1. Activation Map of Negative Emotional Memory Words in 2D. The chart indicates the location of leads on the fNIRS cap, with MNI coordinates for CH27: x = -15.37, y = 72.29, z = 11.12. (a) Average activation map in a 10-second window following the extraction of negative emotional memory words during the traumatic event narration task in the PTSD group. (b) Average activation map in a 10-second window following the extraction of negative emotional memory words during the traumatic event narration task in the TEC (trauma-exposed controls) group.

Figure 2. 3D Activation Map of Negative Emotional Memory Words. Within a 10-second window following the appearance of negative emotional words, the PTSD patients showed higher activation levels (mean Oxy-Hb activation = 0.295) in the CH27 region of the left anterior medial prefrontal cortex compared to the control group (mean Oxy-Hb activation = 0.086). This difference was statistically significant after FDR correction (p < 0.001).

Figure 3. Correlation of CH27 Activation Levels with Negative Affective Memory Vocabulary. (a) Time-Series Analysis of CH27 Oxy-Hb Activation Levels in TEC and PTSD Groups. Within the 10s window after negative emotional vocabulary appearance, the TEC group showed a distinct upward and then downward trend starting at 3.82s, while the PTSD group remained at a higher level of activation. (b) Distribution of CH27 Oxy-Hb Activation Levels in TEC and PTSD Groups.

In contrast, the TEC group showed a statistically significant positive correlation with time under the same conditions (Spearman’s r = 0.727, p < 0.01). At 3.82 s after using negative affective vocabulary, the CH27 region Oxy-Hb saturation in the TEC group began to rise significantly, reaching a peak 3.73 s later (y max = 0.184, y difference = 0.273). The rate of change in activation level was approximately 0.0067, followed by a downward trend. There were significant fluctuations in CH27 activation levels in the TEC group, reflecting notable changes in activation levels within the time window following the expression of negative affective vocabulary.

Discussion

This study aimed to investigate the relationships among linguistic features, traumatic fear memory, and activation of the amPFC in PTSD patients compared with a control group. Our findings reveal three major insights: (1) PTSD patients relied more on explicit narratives of negative emotional memories during traumatic recall and experienced greater emotional distress; (2) both healthy controls and PTSD patients exhibited activation of the L-amPFC following the expression of negative emotions; however, activation in healthy controls showed significant temporal sequence correlations. In contrast, (3) PTSD patients showed sustained high activation levels in the L-amPFC, particularly during the expression of negative emotional words, contrasting sharply with the control group’s rapid and effective emotional regulation capabilities, suggesting an overactivity of the low pathway (rapid emotional responses) and insufficient regulation of the high pathway (detailed cognitive processing).

The results indicate that PTSD patients depend more on explicit narratives of negative emotional memories and experience heightened emotional distress during their expression. Acoustic analysis showed that PTSD patients had a significantly higher pitch, potentially linked to anxiety or tension while recalling traumatic events. Furthermore, the analysis of semitone values suggested that PTSD patients exhibited less pitch decline, reflecting a more monotonic and less varied tone during their traumatic narrations, possibly associated with numbing or subdued effect. This is consistent with previous findings indicating a more monotonous voice in PTSD patients (Marmar et al., Reference Marmar, Brown, Qian, Laska, Siegel, Li and Vergyri2019). Additionally, PTSD patients exhibited more frequent pauses during their narratives, likely due to greater emotional distress or difficulty in organizing language while recalling and narrating traumatic events, also suggesting that their recollections are not merely rote descriptions.

Analysis of transcriptions revealed a lower Honoré’s R index in PTSD patients, indicating reduced complexity and diversity in their vocabulary during traumatic narrations. However, no significant differences were found in other linguistic structural features between the PTSD and control groups, affirming that PTSD primarily manifests as changes in emotional, cognitive, and physiological responses. While these may affect the emotional tone and content of language expression, they do not significantly alter its structural characteristics. This contrasts with other mental disorders such as schizophrenia, which can lead to significant changes in language structure, such as formal thought disorder (De Boer et al., Reference De Boer, Van Hoogdalem, Mandl, Brummelman, Voppel, Begemann and Sommer2020; Li et al., Reference Li, Cao, Fu, Wei, Wang, Yuan and Deng2024). The linguistic characteristics of PTSD patients tend to manifest more in terms of affect and content rather than form and structure.

Furthermore, the analysis of the use of emotional and factual vocabulary revealed that PTSD patients described factual and logical information similarly to the control group. However, upon further subdivision of emotional vocabulary, this study found that PTSD patients more frequently used negative emotional words in their language, which may suggest that they experience stronger negative emotions when recounting traumatic experiences. Given that this study did not directly measure negative emotions, this interpretation has certain limitations and should be considered speculative. Nevertheless, this finding aligns with one of the core symptoms of PTSD – the negative alterations in cognition and mood (Krüger-Gottschalk et al., Reference Krüger-Gottschalk, Ehring, Knaevelsrud, Dyer, Schäfer, Schellong and Köhler2022).

In learning and memory, there is a process where short-term memories stored primarily in the hippocampus and amygdala are transformed into long-term memories stored in the prefrontal cortex (Frankland & Bontempi, Reference Frankland and Bontempi2005; Kitamura et al., Reference Kitamura, Ogawa, Roy, Okuyama, Morrissey, Smith and Tonegawa2017). Memory can be disrupted and destroyed through reconsolidation processes (Lacagnina et al., Reference Lacagnina, Brockway, Crovetti, Shue, McCarty, Sattler and Drew2019; Ressler et al., Reference Ressler, Goode, Kim, Ramanathan and Maren2021). Research by Frankland and Bontempi has shown that the mPFC plays a crucial role in maintaining long-term fear memories (Frankland & Bontempi, Reference Frankland and Bontempi2005). The transition from acquiring short-term memory to storing it as long-term memory involves a complex and slow process of enhancing and establishing cortical connections, and the mechanisms for storing and maintaining long-term memory remain unclear.

Our study, by monitoring brain activity during descriptions of negative emotional states, found significant differences in brain activity between PTSD patients and the control group. Notably, within a 10 s window after the expression of negative emotional words, PTSD patients maintained higher activation levels in the L-amPFC, suggesting potential regulatory issues in suppressing or diminishing fear memories. In contrast, the control group’s activation levels showed a significant positive correlation with time, indicating their more effective handling and mitigation of negative memory impacts.

These findings are consistent with prior studies on the role of the prefrontal-amygdala network in the consolidation of fear memories. During the arousal of fear memories, significant neuronal firing activity in the rat infralimbic (IL) region was negatively correlated with the intensity of fear responses. Additionally, the process of fear memory extinction further enhanced activity levels in the IL region. When a fear stimulus was presented, increased activity in the IL region played an inhibitory role, thereby reducing the expression of fear responses (Ashokan et al., Reference Ashokan, Lim, Hang and Mitra2018; Awad et al., Reference Awad, Ferreira and Maroun2015; Bloodgood et al., Reference Bloodgood, Sugam, Holmes and Kash2018). Research by Neumeister et al. (Reference Neumeister, Feldker, Heitmann, Helmich, Gathmann, Becker and Straube2017) found significant increases in anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)/mPFC activation in PTSD patients when exposed to traumatic cues, with hyperconnectivity between the BLA and ACC/mPFC, playing a crucial role in maintaining fear memories, particularly pathological memories. This hyperconnectivity might prevent normal suppression or delay in the diminishment of fear memories in PTSD patients, explaining the sustained high activation levels in the L-amPFC observed in our study, in contrast to the linear decrease observed in the control group after reaching a peak.

During the process of fear memory extinction, as cues associated with fear in the environment change, the expression of conditioned fear memories is suppressed, forming new extinction memories (Bouton et al., Reference Bouton, Maren and McNally2021; Herry et al., Reference Herry, Ferraguti, Singewald, Letzkus, Ehrlich and Lüthi2010). These extinction memories involve the same brain regions as fear memory acquisition, storage, and retrieval but engage different neuronal circuits. The brainstem and BLA play a primary role in the early formation of extinction memories, with the consolidation of these memories requiring involvement from the IL region and a reduction in activity in the PrL region (Bouton et al., Reference Bouton, Westbrook, Corcoran and Maren2006; Burgos-Robles et al., Reference Burgos-Robles, Vidal-Gonzalez and Quirk2009; Klavir et al., Reference Klavir, Prigge, Sarel, Paz and Yizhar2017; Quirk & Mueller, Reference Quirk and Mueller2008), while the hippocampal–cortical circuit mediates the rekindling of fear memories (Bernier et al., Reference Bernier, Lacagnina, Ayoub, Shue, Zemelman, Krasne and Drew2017; Campese & Delamater, Reference Campese and Delamater2014; Maren et al., Reference Maren, Phan and Liberzon2013). Our study found that after describing traumatic experiences using negative emotional words, the L-amPFC region’s oxygen saturation in the TEC group began to significantly rise and then showed a downward trend, possibly reflecting a reduction in fear information encoding and subsequent fear memory extinction. This aligns with previous research on extinction memory mechanisms and further supports the dynamic interplay between extinction and fear memories.

The time-series results from our study support the dual-pathway model of emotional responses to fear-inducing stimuli, which posits the existence and functionality of both low and high pathways. Our findings reveal that individuals in the control group displayed rapid and effective emotional regulation when confronted with negative emotional words, characterized by a quick rise and fall in L-amPFC activity. This phenomenon aligns with the role of the high pathway in emotional regulation, which involves more detailed emotional analysis and regulation to maintain emotional stability. Conversely, PTSD patients exhibited a sustained high activation state under the same conditions, likely indicating an overactivity of the low pathway. Given the rapid response and lower precision of the low pathway, it may lead to an exaggerated response to negative stimuli in PTSD patients, where the regulatory functions of the high pathway fail to intervene effectively, resulting in emotional regulation difficulties. These findings suggest that emotional regulation issues in PTSD may stem from a functional imbalance between the two pathways, rather than issues within a single pathway.

Consequently, our results provide new insights for future therapeutic strategies. In treating PTSD, there should be a particular focus on enhancing the functionality of the high pathway to improve patients’ emotional regulation capabilities. This may include techniques such as cognitive-behavioral therapy to strengthen cognitive processing capabilities toward negative emotional stimuli, reducing reliance on the low pathway and thus improving the adaptability and stability of emotional responses.

Funding statement

This work was supported by the STI2030-Major Projects (grant number: 2021ZD0200600) and the Special R&D Project for Scientific and Technological support of Biomedicine and Health Industry in Hangzhou (grant number: 2022WJC062).

Competing interest

None.

References

Albo, Z., & Gräff, J. (2018). The mysteries of remote memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1742), 20170029. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0029CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashokan, A., Lim, J. W. H., Hang, N., & Mitra, R. (2018). Complex housing causes a robust increase in dendritic complexity and spine density of medial prefrontal cortical neurons. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 7308. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-25399-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Awad, W., Ferreira, G., & Maroun, M. (2015). Dissociation of the Role of Infralimbic Cortex in Learning and Consolidation of Extinction of Recent and Remote Aversion Memory. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40(11), 25662575. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2015.103CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barkay, G., Freedman, N., Lester, H., Louzoun, Y., Sapoznikov, D., Luckenbaugh, D., … Bonne, O. (2012). Brain activation and heart rate during script-driven traumatic imagery in PTSD: Preliminary findings. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 204(2–3), 155160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.08.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernier, B. E., Lacagnina, A. F., Ayoub, A., Shue, F., Zemelman, B. V., Krasne, F. B., & Drew, M. R. (2017). Dentate Gyrus Contributes to Retrieval as well as Encoding: Evidence from Context Fear Conditioning, Recall, and Extinction. The Journal of Neuroscience, 37(26), 63596371. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3029-16.2017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloodgood, D. W., Sugam, J. A., Holmes, A., & Kash, T. L. (2018). Fear extinction requires infralimbic cortex projections to the basolateral amygdala. Translational Psychiatry, 8(1), 60. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-018-0106-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouton, M. E., Maren, S., & McNally, G. P. (2021). Behavioral and neurobiological mechanisms of pavlovian and instrumental extinction learning. Physiological Reviews, 101(2), 611681. https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00016.2020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouton, M. E., Westbrook, R. F., Corcoran, K. A., & Maren, S. (2006). Contextual and Temporal Modulation of Extinction: Behavioral and Biological Mechanisms. Biological Psychiatry, 60(4), 352360. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.12.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brem, M. J., Shorey, R. C., Anderson, S., & Stuart, G. L. (2018). Does experiential avoidance explain the relationships between shame, PTSD symptoms, and compulsive sexual behaviour among women in substance use treatment? Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 25(5), 692700. https://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.2300CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bremner, J. D., Narayan, M., Staib, L. H., Southwick, S. M., McGlashan, T., & Charney, D. S. (1999). Neural correlates of memories of childhood sexual abuse in women with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 156(11), 17871795. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.156.11.1787CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, L. A., Davies, C. D., Gerlach, A., Cooper, R., Stevens, S., & Craske, M. G. (2018). Linguistic processing and Script-Driven Imagery for trauma exposure: A proof of concept pilot trial. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 57, 1623. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2018.05.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burgos-Robles, A., Vidal-Gonzalez, I., & Quirk, G. J. (2009). Sustained Conditioned Responses in Prelimbic Prefrontal Neurons Are Correlated with Fear Expression and Extinction Failure. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(26), 84748482. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0378-09.2009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campese, V. D., & Delamater, A. R. (2014). Dorsal hippocampus inactivation impairs spontaneous recovery of Pavlovian magazine approach responding in rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 269, 3743. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2014.04.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curtin, A., Tong, S., Sun, J., Wang, J., Onaral, B., & Ayaz, H. (2019). A Systematic Review of Integrated Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (fNIRS) and Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) Studies. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 13, 84. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2019.00084CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D’Andrea, W., Chiu, P. H., Casas, B. R., & Deldin, P. (2012). Linguistic Predictors of Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder Symptoms Following 11 September 2001. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26(2), 316323. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1830CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Boer, J. N., Van Hoogdalem, M., Mandl, R. C. W., Brummelman, J., Voppel, A. E., Begemann, M. J. H., … Sommer, I. E. C. (2020). Language in schizophrenia: Relation with diagnosis, symptomatology and white matter tracts. Npj Schizophrenia, 6(1), 10. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41537-020-0099-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dudai, Y. (2004). The Neurobiology of Consolidations, Or, How Stable is the engram? Annual Review of Psychology, 55(1), 5186. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142050CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foudil, S.-A., & Macaluso, E. (2024). The influence of the precuneus on the medial temporal cortex determines the subjective quality of memory during the retrieval of naturalistic episodes. Scientific Reports, 14(1), 7943. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-58298-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frankland, P. W., & Bontempi, B. (2005). The organization of recent and remote memories. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(2), 119130. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1607CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gazzaniga, M. S. & Mangun, G. R. (Eds.). (2014). The cognitive neurosciences (5th ed.). The MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9504.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goto, A., Bota, A., Miya, K., Wang, J., Tsukamoto, S., Jiang, X., … Hayashi, Y. (2021). Stepwise synaptic plasticity events drive the early phase of memory consolidation. Science, 374(6569), 857863. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.abj9195CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hagenaar, D. A., Bindels-de Heus, K. G. C. B., Van Gils, M. M., Van Den Berg, L., Ten Hoopen, L. W., Affourtit, P., … Mous, S. E. (2024). Outcome measures in Angelman syndrome. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 16(1), 6. https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-024-09516-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrison, P. J., Cowen, P., Burns, T., & Fazel, M. (2018). Shorter Oxford textbook of psychiatry (7th edition). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
He, C., Fan, D., Liu, X., Wang, Q., Zhang, H., Zhang, H., … Xie, C. (2022). Insula network connectivity mediates the association between childhood maltreatment and depressive symptoms in major depressive disorder patients. Translational Psychiatry, 12(1), 89. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-022-01829-wCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herry, C., Ferraguti, F., Singewald, N., Letzkus, J. J., Ehrlich, I., & Lüthi, A. (2010). Neuronal circuits of fear extinction. European Journal of Neuroscience, 31(4), 599612. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07101.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jennings, A. N., Soder, H. E., Wardle, M. C., Schmitz, J. M., & Vujanovic, A. A. (2021). Objective analysis of language use in cognitive-behavioral therapy: Associations with symptom change in adults with co-occurring substance use disorders and posttraumatic stress. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 50(2), 89103. https://doi.org/10.1080/16506073.2020.1819865CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karalis, N., Dejean, C., Chaudun, F., Khoder, S., Rozeske, R. R., Wurtz, H., … Herry, C. (2016). 4-Hz oscillations synchronize prefrontal–amygdala circuits during fear behavior. Nature Neuroscience, 19(4), 605612. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4251CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kitamura, T., Ogawa, S. K., Roy, D. S., Okuyama, T., Morrissey, M. D., Smith, L. M., … Tonegawa, S. (2017). Engrams and circuits crucial for systems consolidation of a memory. Science, 356(6333), 7378. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aam6808CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klavir, O., Prigge, M., Sarel, A., Paz, R., & Yizhar, O. (2017). Manipulating fear associations via optogenetic modulation of amygdala inputs to prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 20(6), 836844. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4523CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleim, B., Horn, A. B., Kraehenmann, R., Mehl, M. R., & Ehlers, A. (2018). Early linguistic markers of trauma-specific processing predict post-trauma adjustment. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9, 645. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00645CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krüger-Gottschalk, A., Ehring, T., Knaevelsrud, C., Dyer, A., Schäfer, I., Schellong, J., … Köhler, K. (2022). Confirmatory factor analysis of the clinician-administered PTSD Scale (CAPS-5) based on DSM-5 vs. ICD-11 criteria. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 13(1), 2010995. https://doi.org/10.1080/20008198.2021.2010995CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lacagnina, A. F., Brockway, E. T., Crovetti, C. R., Shue, F., McCarty, M. J., Sattler, K. P., … Drew, M. R. (2019). Distinct hippocampal engrams control extinction and relapse of fear memory. Nature Neuroscience, 22(5), 753761. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0361-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lanius, R. A., Williamson, P. C., Densmore, M., Boksman, K., Gupta, M. A., Neufeld, R. W., … Menon, R. S. (2001). Neural correlates of traumatic memories in posttraumatic stress disorder: A functional MRI investigation. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(11), 19201922. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1920CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, S. M. & McCarthy, G. (2015). Neural regions discriminating contextual information as conveyed through the learned preferences of others. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9(492), 19. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00492CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, R., Cao, M., Fu, D., Wei, W., Wang, D., Yuan, Z., … Deng, W. (2024). Deciphering language disturbances in schizophrenia: A study using fine-tuned language models. Schizophrenia Research, 271, 120128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2024.07.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, M. D., Straccia, M. A., Meyer, M. L., Du, M., & Tan, K. M. (2019). Social, self, (situational), and affective processes in medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC): Causal, multivariate, and reverse inference evidence. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 99, 311328. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.12.021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maren, S., Phan, K. L., & Liberzon, I. (2013). The contextual brain: Implications for fear conditioning, extinction and psychopathology. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(6), 417428. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3492CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marmar, C. R., Brown, A. D., Qian, M., Laska, E., Siegel, C., Li, M., … Vergyri, D. (2019). Speech-based markers for posttraumatic stress disorder in US veterans. Depression and Anxiety, 36(7), 607616. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22890CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazziotti, R., Scaffei, E., Conti, E., Marchi, V., Rizzi, R., Cioni, G., … Baroncelli, L. (2022). The amplitude of fNIRS hemodynamic response in the visual cortex unmasks autistic traits in typically developing children. Translational Psychiatry, 12(1), 53. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-022-01820-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCarthy, P. M. & Boonthum-Denecke, C. (Eds.). (2012). Applied natural language processing: identification, investigation and resolution. IGI Global. https://doi.org/10.4018/978-1-60960-741-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miragoli, S., Procaccia, R., & Di Blasio, P. (2014). Language Use and PTSD Symptoms: Content Analyses of Allegations of Child Sexual Abuse. Journal of Forensic Psychology Practice, 14(5), 355382. https://doi.org/10.1080/15228932.2014.970423CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neumeister, P., Feldker, K., Heitmann, C. Y., Helmich, R., Gathmann, B., Becker, M. P. I., & Straube, T. (2017). Interpersonal violence in posttraumatic women: Brain networks triggered by trauma-related pictures. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(4), 555568. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw165CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ng, L. C., Ahishakiye, N., Miller, D. E., & Meyerowitz, B. E. (2015). Narrative characteristics of genocide testimonies predict posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms years later. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 7(3), 303311. https://doi.org/10.1037/tra0000024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ozer, E. J., Best, S. R., Lipsey, T. L., & Weiss, D. S. (2003). Predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder and symptoms in adults: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 129(1), 5273. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.52CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Papini, S., Yoon, P., Rubin, M., Lopez-Castro, T., & Hien, D. A. (2015). Linguistic characteristics in a non-trauma-related narrative task are associated with PTSD diagnosis and symptom severity. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 7(3), 295302. https://doi.org/10.1037/tra0000019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quirk, G. J., & Mueller, D. (2008). Neural Mechanisms of Extinction Learning and Retrieval. Neuropsychopharmacology, 33(1), 5672. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1301555CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rauch, S. L. (1996). A symptom provocation study of posttraumatic stress disorder using positron emission tomography and script-driven imagery. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53(5), 380. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830050014003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ressler, R. L., Goode, T. D., Kim, S., Ramanathan, K. R., & Maren, S. (2021). Covert capture and attenuation of a hippocampus-dependent fear memory. Nature Neuroscience, 24(5), 677684. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-021-00825-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shapiro, S. S., & Wilk, M. B. (1965). An analysis of variance test for normality (complete samples). Biometrika, 52(3–4), 591611. https://doi.org/10.1093/biomet/52.3-4.591CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shin, L. M., Orr, S. P., Carson, M. A., Rauch, S. L., Macklin, M. L., Lasko, N. B., … Pitman, R. K. (2004). Regional cerebral blood flow in the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex during traumatic imagery in male and female vietnam veterans With PTSD. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61(2), 168. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.61.2.168CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sun, X., Bernstein, M. J., Meng, M., Rao, S., Sørensen, A. T., Yao, L., … Lin, Y. (2020). Functionally distinct neuronal ensembles within the memory Engram. Cell, 181(2), 410423.e17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2020.02.055CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weathers, F. W., Blake, D. D., Schnurr, P. P., Kaloupek, D. G., Marx, B. P., & Keane, T. M. (2015). The Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for DSM-5 (CAPS-5) – Past Month [Measurement instrument]. https://www.ptsd.va.gov/Google Scholar
Wisco, B. E., Marx, B. P., Miller, M. W., Wolf, E. J., Mota, N. P., Krystal, J. H., … Pietrzak, R. H. (2016). Probable posttraumatic stress disorder in the US veteran population according to DSM-5: Results from the National Health and Resilience in Veterans Study. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 77(11), 15031510. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.15m10188CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wisco, B. E., Marx, B. P., Wolf, E. J., Miller, M. W., Southwick, S. M., & Pietrzak, R. H. (2014). Posttraumatic stress disorder in the US veteran population: Results from the National Health and Resilience in Veterans Study. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 75(12), 13381346. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.14m09328CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the study sample

Figure 1

Table 2. Description of speech and language variables

Figure 2

Table 3. Speech and language characteristics between groups

Figure 3

Figure 1. Activation Map of Negative Emotional Memory Words in 2D. The chart indicates the location of leads on the fNIRS cap, with MNI coordinates for CH27: x = -15.37, y = 72.29, z = 11.12. (a) Average activation map in a 10-second window following the extraction of negative emotional memory words during the traumatic event narration task in the PTSD group. (b) Average activation map in a 10-second window following the extraction of negative emotional memory words during the traumatic event narration task in the TEC (trauma-exposed controls) group.

Figure 4

Figure 2. 3D Activation Map of Negative Emotional Memory Words. Within a 10-second window following the appearance of negative emotional words, the PTSD patients showed higher activation levels (mean Oxy-Hb activation = 0.295) in the CH27 region of the left anterior medial prefrontal cortex compared to the control group (mean Oxy-Hb activation = 0.086). This difference was statistically significant after FDR correction (p < 0.001).

Figure 5

Figure 3. Correlation of CH27 Activation Levels with Negative Affective Memory Vocabulary. (a) Time-Series Analysis of CH27 Oxy-Hb Activation Levels in TEC and PTSD Groups. Within the 10s window after negative emotional vocabulary appearance, the TEC group showed a distinct upward and then downward trend starting at 3.82s, while the PTSD group remained at a higher level of activation. (b) Distribution of CH27 Oxy-Hb Activation Levels in TEC and PTSD Groups.