Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T22:45:43.514Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cross-cultural support for a link between analytic thinking and disbelief in God: Evidence from India and the United Kingdom

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2023

Michael N. Stagnaro
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Yale University
Robert M. Ross
Affiliation:
Department of Philosophy, Macquarie University, and ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders and the Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London.
Gordon Pennycook
Affiliation:
Hill/Levene Schools of Business, University of Regina.
David G. Rand
Affiliation:
Management Science, Brain and Cognitive Science, MIT.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

A substantial body of evidence suggests that favoring reason over intuition (employing an analytic cognitive style) is associated with reduced belief in God. In the current work, we address outstanding issues in this literature with two studies examining the relationship between analytic cognitive style (as measured by performance on the Cognitive Reflection Test) and belief in God. First, prior research focused on Judeo-Christian cultures, and it is uncertain whether the results generalize to other religious systems or beliefs. Study 1 helps to address this question by documenting a negative correlation between CRT performance and belief in God, r = −.18, in a sample of 513 participants from India, a majority Hindu country. Second, among 150 participants from the United Kingdom, Gervais et al. (2018) reported the first and (to date) only evidence for a positive relationship between CRT and belief in God. In Study 2, we assess the robustness of this result by recruiting 547 participants from the United Kingdom. Unlike Gervais et al., using the same items, we find a negative correlation between CRT and belief in God (r = −.19). Our results add further support to the argument that analytic thinking undermines belief in God.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
The authors license this article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License.
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors [2019] This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

1 Introduction

Religious identification is associated with a number of positive outcomes, from improved mental and physical health (Reference Carrico, Ironson, Antoni, Lechner, Durán, Kumar and SchneidermanCarrico et al., 2006; Reference Ironson, Stuetzle and FletcherIronson et al., 2006), to greater reported happiness (Reference FerrissFerriss, 2002), increased prosocial behavior (Reference Stagnaro, Arechar and RandStagnaro et al., 2018) and even a longer life (Reference McCullough, Hoyt, Larson, Koenig and ThoresenMcCullough et al., 2000).Footnote 1 Yet, despite these potential benefits, religious belief has been steadily declining in recent years (Reference BurkimsherBurkimsher, 2014). Scholars have increasingly directed their attention to why some people believe in God while others do not (Reference Norenzayan and GervaisNorenzayan & Gervais, 2013). Although several potential pathways to religious disbelief have been identified (Reference Mercier, Kramer and ShariffMercier et al., 2018), one pathway in particular has received much attention in the cognitive science of religion – that of analytic cognitive style (Reference Gervais and NorenzayanGervais & Norenzayan, 2012; Reference Pennycook, Cheyne, Seli, Koehler and FugelsangPennycook et al., 2012; Reference Pennycook, Ross, Koehler and FugelsangPennycook, Ross, et al., 2016; Reference Shenhav, Rand and GreeneShenhav et al., 2012).

This line of work is rooted in dual-process theory, in which human cognition is characterized by a distinction between processes that are autonomous and relatively fast (“Type 1” processes), and relatively slower processing that is deliberative and dependent on working memory (“Type 2” processes) (Reference De NeysDe Neys, 2012; Reference Evans and StanovichEvans & Stanovich, 2013; Reference KahnemanKahneman, 2011; Reference Pennycook, Fugelsang and KoehlerPennycook et al., 2015). According to dual-process theory, individuals vary in the extent to which they utilize Type 1 versus Type 2 processing (Reference Stanovich and WestStanovich & West, 2000, Reference Stanovich, Evans and FrankishStanovich, 2009) – that is, some people are more disposed to engaging deliberative Type 2 processing. One name for this individual difference is cognitive style; with those who engage in more Type 2 processing referred to as having a more analytic cognitive style (Reference Pennycook, Cheyne, Seli, Koehler and FugelsangPennycook et al., 2012).

Of the different measures of cognitive style, one of the most widely used is the Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT; Reference FrederickFrederick, 2005). This measure involves a series of “trick questions” with intuitively compelling but incorrect answers. With some reflection, these incorrect answers can be identified and overridden by participants who engage in more analytic thinking. Consider the following item:

A bat and ball cost $1.10 in total. The bat costs $1.00 more than the ball. How much does the ball cost?

This problem elicits a fast, intuitive response (10 cents) that on reflection can be identified as incorrect (if the ball costs 10 cents, the bat would cost $1.10 and thus total $1.20). Despite this, the incorrect intuitive response is typically the modal response (e.g., 65% in a Canadian undergraduate population; Reference Pennycook, Cheyne, Koehler and FugelsangPennycook, Cheyne, et al., 2016), indicating a common failure to engage in reflective reasoning (Reference Pennycook, Ross, Koehler and FugelsangPennycook & Ross, 2016).

Evidence suggests that analytic cognitive style (as indexed by scores on the CRT) increases the probability of critically evaluating evidence that conflicts with previously held information (Reference Pennycook, Fugelsang and KoehlerPennycook et al., 2015). Some theories (Reference Shenhav, Rand and GreeneShenhav et al., 2012) have used this reasoning to argue that religious beliefs are particularly intuitive (Reference BarrettBarrett, 2000; Reference KelemenKelemen, 2004) and, as such, would be accepted by most individuals save those who are particularly prone to detect conflicting signals and thus override initial beliefs. A different, but related theory (Reference Pennycook, Cheyne, Seli, Koehler and FugelsangPennycook, et al., 2012) posits that there is likely nothing special about religious beliefs per say – there is simply an asymmetry in belief and non-belief such that when an individual is exposed to and attempts to comprehend some claim, they must initially internalize it as true (Reference Gilbert, Tafarodi and MaloneGilbert et al., 1993). Thus, individual who tend to be more “intuitive” in their reasoning are more inclined to simply internalize the initial belief as true (especially if the source is of high status, and or centrally connected in one’s community; Reference Henrich and Gil-WhiteHenrich & Gil-White, 2001). Those who are more inclined to detect inconsistencies and, over time, reject, or reduce confidence, in such claims should show a decrease in such belief.

Critically for this work, evidence supports the hypothesis that people with a propensity to think analytically typically show lower levels of religious belief (Reference Bahçekapili and YilmazBahçekapili & Yilmaz, 2017; Reference Gervais and NorenzayanGervais & Norenzayan, 2012; Reference Pennycook, Cheyne, Seli, Koehler and FugelsangPennycook et al., 2012; Reference Pennycook, Ross, Koehler and FugelsangPennycook, Ross, et al., 2016; Reference Saribay and YilmazSaribay & Yilmaz, 2017; Reference Shenhav, Rand and GreeneShenhav et al., 2012). A recent meta-analysis of 31 studies (N = 15,078) found that a propensity to think analytically (measured the number of correct responses on the CRT) is consistently and negatively associated with religious belief (Reference Pennycook, Ross, Koehler and FugelsangPennycook, Ross, et al., 2016). Furthermore, experimental work has provided mixed evidence that priming analytic thinking can decrease belief in God (Reference Gervais and NorenzayanGervais & Norenzayan, 2012; Reference Shenhav, Rand and GreeneShenhav et al., 2012; Reference Yilmaz, Karadöller and SofuogluYilmaz et al., 2016), although some of the results from Reference Gervais and NorenzayanGervais and Norenzayan (2012) have not replicated (Camerer et al., 2018; Reference Sanchez, Sundermeier, Gray and Calin-JagemanSanchez et al., 2017).

Apart from the uncertainty of the experimental results, a clear shortcoming of research on the relationship between belief in God and cognitive style is that it has relied almost entirely on North American participants, a common problem in psychological research (Reference Henrich, Heine and NorenzayanHenrich et al., 2010). Although the negative relationship between CRT and belief in God has been replicated in a majority Muslim sample in Turkey (Reference Bahçekapili and YilmazBahçekapili & Yilmaz, 2017), the dearth of cross-cultural evidence, especially from non-Abrahamic/monotheistic cultures, has been a notable concern for claims that analytic thinking plays a role in the absence of belief.Footnote 2

However, a recent cross-cultural study (N = 3,461) conducted across 13 religiously and culturally diverse societies has provided some evidence that the relationship between CRT and belief in God is in fact a culturally broad phenomenon (Reference Gervais, van Elk, Xygalatas, McKay, Aveyard, Buchtel and RiekkiGervais et al., 2018). The authors argued, as we do above, that the work supporting the claim for what they term “analytic atheism” (Reference Norenzayan and GervaisNorenzayan & Gervais, 2013) has overwhelmingly come from WEIRD populations (Reference Henrich, Heine and NorenzayanHenrich et al., 2010). Although Gervais, et al. did find a significant negative overall relationship between belief in God and CRT, this relationship interestingly appeared to hold for mostly non-WEIRD samples,Footnote 3 the magnitude (and confidence) of the effect varied across countries. In particular, the negative correlation appeared to be larger in countries that had strong religious traditions than in countries that did not. Based on the cross-cultural variability in magnitude, the authors argued that the negative relationship between CRT and belief in God previously observed among Americans (a WEIRD sample) may be cross-culturally “weak and fickle” (p. 5). Particularly troubling for the negative relationship between CRT and belief in God were Gervais et al.’s United Kingdom data (N = 150). This was the only country in which a significant positive correlation between CRT score and belief in God was found.

In the present work, we present two large sample studies which help address two key questions: First, does the previously observed negative relationship between performance on the CRT and belief in God maintain in non-monotheistic, non-Abrahamic majority culture? Study 1 used participants from India, a majority Hindu country.Footnote 4 Second, in contexts where belief is not the dominant cultural norm, is there the potential for a positive relationship between CRT and belief in God? Study 2 use participants from the United Kingdom, where Reference Gervais, van Elk, Xygalatas, McKay, Aveyard, Buchtel and RiekkiGervais et al. (2018) observed a positive relationship.

2 Study 1 — India

2.1 Methods

To assess the relationship between CRT and belief in God in a predominantly non-monotheistic, non-Western population, we pooled data from two studies that we had previously run on Amazon Mechanical Turk (AMT) that used Indian participants, collected in 2012, when it was still easy for Indian residents to open worker accounts on AMT, and thus a sizable number of AMT workers were Indian. IP addresses were used to confirm their location to be in India. Though not representative of the average Indian citizen (working on AMT implies a level of English proficiency and technical access which may not be broadly available), this convenience sample nonetheless allows us to test the question of interest. We identified N = 513 Indian individuals (185 female; mean age 28) for whom we had data on both belief in God and the original 3-item CRT. Both studies were under 15 min long, and all participants were compensated for their time in US dollars.Footnote 5

Performance on the CRT was comparable to previous samples, M = 1.36, SD = 1.06, Median = 2, Cronbach’s α = .634. Further, there was reasonable variation in performance, with over 15% answering all items correctly and less than 30% missing all three. Of those that gave no correct answers, 44% gave all three of the intuitively incorrect answers and over 97% giving at least one intuitive answer. The correlation between correct and intuitive answers was r = −.815. Thus, there is good evidence these participants understood and engaged with this measure as intended.

Belief in God was measured with the question: “To what extent do you believe in the existence of God (or gods)?”, responding on a scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 10 (very much). Belief was high, M = 7.86, SD = 2.55, Median = 9, with over 40% using the maximum value on the scale and less than 5% using the minimum.

2.2 Results and Discussion

Using linear regression, with correct CRT scores as the independent variable predicting belief in God as the dependent variable, we found a significant, negative relationship (β = −.176, F(1, 511) = 16.30, p < .001, 95% CI [−.63, −.22]Footnote 6; all coefficients represent standardized betas unless otherwise noted). See Figure 1. This relationship became slightly stronger when including a dummy code for study (β = −.185, F(2, 510) = 9.33, p < .001, 95% CI [−.65, −.24]). Further, this relationship was robust to the inclusion of “upstream” demographic variables (those unaffected by belief in God: gender and age; β = −.171, F(4, 507) = 11.33, p < .001, 95% CI [−.62, −.21]), and potentially “downstream” demographic variables (those potentially affected by belief in God: education, income, and self-reported trust in others; β = −.167, F(11, 500) = 5.10, p < .001, 95% CI [−.61, −.20]).Footnote 7 In addition, using the number of intuitive responses, rather than correct responses, as the independent variable yielded very similar results (see the supplement). Analyses are summarized in Table 1.

Figure 1: Violin plots (with jittered individual data points) showing belief in God (represented by POMP scores) as a function of correct responses on the 3-item CRT. Width of each plot shows density. Horizontal lines show median (heavy line) and 25th and 75th percentile.

Table 1: Multiple regression analyses (standardized coefficients) predicting belief in God using the 3-item CRT and controlling for study, age, gender (Male = 1, Female = 2), income, trust, and education (dummy code). Standard errors are in parentheses

p<0.1

* p<0.05

** p<0.01

*** p<0.001.

Thus, in a sample that is substantially larger than the Indian sample included in Reference Gervais, van Elk, Xygalatas, McKay, Aveyard, Buchtel and RiekkiGervais et al. (2018) (which had 224 Indian workers from AMT), we replicate the previously observed negative relationship between CRT and belief in God.

3 Study 2 — United Kingdom

We next turn to the surprising result reported by Reference Gervais, van Elk, Xygalatas, McKay, Aveyard, Buchtel and RiekkiGervais et al. (2018): of the 13 countries they studied, only the United Kingdom showed moderate evidence for a positive relationship between belief in God and CRT score. Here we collected a much larger sample from the United Kingdom to see if this result would replicate. The preregistered analysis protocol, survey items, and data can be found at https://osf.io/jb2mr/.

3.1 Methods

We recruited participants using Prolific (https://prolific.ac/), an online marketplace where people can sign up to participate in paid academic studies (Reference Irvine, Hoffman and Wilkinson-RyanIrvine et al., 2018). Only participants with a UK-based IP address were eligible to respond to the study advertisement. Sessions lasted approximately 8 minutes and participants were compensated in pounds for their time. We preregistered an intention to request 550 participants from Prolific and to remove participants who a) reported their age as less than 18, or b) had a repeat IP address, suggesting that they may have previously completed the study. 548 participants completed the study and collected compensation. One participant was removed for reporting their age as 17, meaning that 547 participants (348 female; mean age 36) were retained for analysis.

Analytic cognitive style was measured using the original 3-item CRT (Reference FrederickFrederick, 2005) and a more recent 4-item CRT which was designed to rely less on numeracy (Reference Thomson and OppenheimerThomson & Oppenheimer, 2016).Footnote 8 These were combined into a single, 7-item CRT. Scores on the 3-item CRT were comparable to earlier studies, with 36.2% receiving a score of zero, 21.9% receiving a score of three, and a mean of 1.28 (SD = 1.17). The 7-item scale had a more normal distribution, with only 8% scoring zero, 10.24% scoring a full seven, a mean of 3.66 (SD = 2.13, Cronbach’s α = .772). Belief in God (or gods) was measured used a single item measure anchored from 0 to 100.Footnote 9 Given the potential problem of relying on a single item measure, we also included a broader index of religious belief: Namely, the 6-item Supernatural Belief Scale (Reference Jong, Bluemke and HalberstadtJong et al., 2013; Reference Jong and HalberstadtJong & Halberstadt, 2016) which asks participants about their beliefs in religious supernatural concepts and has good psychometric properties.Footnote 10 We found that this scale had high reliability (Cronbach’s α = .956). We also collected demographic variables: age, gender, education, ethnicity, income, political ideology (social and fiscal) and a measure of attention (an instructional manipulation check; Reference Oppenheimer, Meyvis and DavidenkoOppenheimer et al., 2009). Finally, we collected some additional variables for exploratory analysis that are not reported here.

3.2 Results and Discussion

To test our primary hypothesis, we used linear regression to examine whether performance on the 7-item CRT predicts supernatural belief using the 6-item Supernatural Belief Scale. Secondarily, we used linear regression to examine whether performance on the 3-item CRT predicts belief in God using the single-item belief in God measure. This secondary analysis was performed to directly replicate Reference Gervais, van Elk, Xygalatas, McKay, Aveyard, Buchtel and RiekkiGervais et al.’s (2018) analysis.

As shown in Figure 2, we found a significant negative relationship between the number of correct responses on the 7-item CRT and Supernatural Belief Scale score (β = −.291, F(1, 545) = 50.43, p < .001, 95% CI [−2.67, −1.52]).Footnote 11 We also found a significant negative relationship between the number of correct responses on the 3-item CRT and the single-item belief in God measure, β = −.189, F(1, 545) = 20.15, p < .001, 95% CI [−8.38, −3.28], Figure 3.Footnote 12

Figure 2: Violin plots (with jittered individual data points) showing supernatural belief (represented by POMP scores) as a function of correct responses on the 7-item CRT.

Figure 3: Violin plots (with jittered individual data points) showing belief in God (represented by POMP scores) as a function of number of correct responses on the 3-item CRT.

To test the robustness of our analyses, we also conducted a number of post hoc analyses. Again, we found a significant negative relationship when controlling for demographics “upstream” of belief (age, gender and ethnicity dummies), using both the 7-item CRT and Supernatural Belief Scale score (β = −.231, F(13, 533) = 10.15, p < .001, 95% CIs [−2.23, −1.09]) and the 3-item CRT and single-item belief in God measure (β = −.166, F(13, 533) = 7.28, p < .001, 95% CI [−7.63, −2.60]). Further, this relationship maintains when we added demographics that are potentially “downstream” of belief (education dummies, income, political ideology [social and fiscal], and failed comprehension), for both the 7-item CRT and Supernatural Belief Scale score (β = −.197, F(22, 524) = 7.40, p < .001, 95% CI [−2.01, −.83]), and the 3-item CRT and the single-item belief in God measure (β = −.152, F(22, 524) = 5.64, p < .001, 95% CI [−7.28, −2.12]). Analyses are summarized in Table 2.

Table 2: Multiple regression analyses (standardized) predicting a) belief in God using the 3-item CRT and b) supernatural belief using the 7-item CRT while controlling for age, gender (Male = 1, Female = 2), income, social conservatism, fiscal conservatism, performance in an attention check, ethnicity (dummy coded), and education (dummy coded). Standard errors are in parentheses

* p<0.05

** p<0.01

*** p<0.001

4 General Discussion

In the present paper, we examined the relationship between analytic cognitive style and belief in God (or gods) in India and the United Kingdom. First, consistent with Reference Gervais, van Elk, Xygalatas, McKay, Aveyard, Buchtel and RiekkiGervais et al. (2018), we found a negative relationship between CRT and religious belief in our Indian sample. This further strengthens the evidence that this relationship between belief and analytic thinking does not occur only in WEIRD populations, but can also occur in non-Western cultures in which the majority follow a non-monotheist religion.Footnote 13

Second, in contrast to the results reported by Reference Gervais, van Elk, Xygalatas, McKay, Aveyard, Buchtel and RiekkiGervais et al. (2018), we found a negative relationship between CRT and belief in God in our UK sample. In fact, the magnitude of the negative association we observed was very similar to that found in a recent meta-analysis of 31 studies, r = −.183 (Reference Pennycook, Ross, Koehler and FugelsangPennycook, Ross, et al., 2016). It is not clear why our results differ markedly from Reference Gervais, van Elk, Xygalatas, McKay, Aveyard, Buchtel and RiekkiGervais et al. (2018). However, our sample was recruited online, while Gervais et al. employed a college sample. It is possible that students at British universities show the opposite association to that of a more demographically diverse sample of the British population. This possibility would have to apply to British universities in particular, since studies using North American college samples have found a negative relationship (Reference Gervais and NorenzayanGervais & Norenzayan, 2012; Reference Pennycook, Ross, Koehler and FugelsangPennycook, Ross, et al., 2016; Reference Shenhav, Rand and GreeneShenhav et al., 2012). Another possibility is sample size; our sample was substantially larger (N = 548 vs N = 150). Thus, it could be the case that Gervais et al.’s result was spurious (i.e., a type 1 error).

Reference Gervais, van Elk, Xygalatas, McKay, Aveyard, Buchtel and RiekkiGervais et al. (2018) observe that in their study the countries where the negative relationship between CRT scores and belief was weakest were also the countries where religious belief is less prominent. They suggest that cognitive reflection may be involved in the challenging of cultural norms – irrespective of their theological frame. Thus, if belief was the minority view, utilizing reflection could make individuals more likely to challenge this view and adopt or experiment with religious belief. The British data from the present study, however, are not consistent with this

interpretation. Britain has particularly low levels of religious belief (59% in this sample which is representative of current trends; Reference Evans and BaronavskiEvans & Baronavski, 2018), yet we found a negative association between CRT and religious belief. Still, the level of religious belief in British universities might be particularly low.

A more general explanation for this pattern could be that, in populations of low belief, there is simply less of a signal being sent between reflection and non-belief. Since many individuals relying on intuition would normally adopt a religious world view as a sort of default belief in religious societies, here, it is not a common or easily accessible worldview and thus we should not expect individuals to be defaulting to belief. Thus this lower overall level of belief makes detecting relationships much harder (due to the constrained variance).

Our results from the British sample also support a (perhaps obvious) speculation about methodology made by Reference Pennycook, Ross, Koehler and FugelsangPennycook, Ross, et al. (2016): The effect size of the correlation between religious disbelief and analytic thinking depends on the measures that are used. Reference Gervais, van Elk, Xygalatas, McKay, Aveyard, Buchtel and RiekkiGervais et al. (2018) referred to the association as “weak”, but they used a 3-item measure of analytic thinking and a single-item measure of belief in God. Using these same measures, we found a correlation r = −.19 in our UK sample. However, using a 7-item CRT measure and a 6-item religious belief measure, this correlation increased to r = −.29. Reference Gignac and SzodoraiGignac and Szodorai (2016) found that correlations of r = .11, .19, and .29 represent the 25th, 50th, and 75th percentiles (respectively) for effect sizes in individual differences research in social psychology. Thus, the effect size for the correlation between analytic thinking and religious beliefs appears to be medium using extremely short measures and relatively large when using longer (but nonetheless fairly short) measures (see also Reference Pennycook, Ross, Koehler and FugelsangPennycook, Ross, et al., 2016).

In conclusion, recent work has raised questions about the robustness and function of analytic processing in the context of belief in God (Camerer et al., 2018; Reference Gervais, van Elk, Xygalatas, McKay, Aveyard, Buchtel and RiekkiGervais et al., 2018; Reference Sanchez, Sundermeier, Gray and Calin-JagemanSanchez et al., 2017). Here, we report results that further reinforce the evidence for a negative relationship between religious belief and analytic thinking in a non-Abrahamic, non-monotheistic, and non-Western population — in this case, India. Furthermore, we have also shown that the negative relationship maintains even in a population where religious belief is much weaker — in this case, the United Kingdom. This suggests that analytic thinking is not motivating people to merely challenge whatever the default norm happens to be (be it belief or non-belief), but is rather motivating people to specifically question religious belief. Although there appears to be cross-cultural variability in the association between analytic thinking and religious disbelief (Reference Gervais, van Elk, Xygalatas, McKay, Aveyard, Buchtel and RiekkiGervais et al., 2018), the causes of this variability are still unknown.

Footnotes

The first two authors contributed equally to this work.

We gratefully acknowledge funding from the Ethics and Governance of Artificial Intelligence Initiative of the Miami Foundation, the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada, the Australian Research Council (DP180102384), and the Templeton Foundation (grant no. 6062.4). The opinions expressed are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of these funders.

1 However, some have challenged the overall health benefits attributed to religious belief (Reference SloanSloan, 2006).

2 There could be a concern that, due to historical divides between Enlightenment Reason and Abrahamic monotheistic faiths, there may be a more pronounced and adversarial relationship between reason and faith for the Abrahamic traditions (Reference DraperDraper, 1875). It also could be that there is some effect of flexibility and diversity differentially experienced in polytheistic communities that may afford those higher on reason to be comfortable with holding some features of a faith-based system and not others. Whatever the case, the strong version of our argument would not predict some faiths sharing a relationship with reflection but not others; thus cross-cultural stability is necessary.

3 Note that Reference Gervais, van Elk, Xygalatas, McKay, Aveyard, Buchtel and RiekkiGervais et al. (2018) also had a similar, albeit somewhat smaller, sample of Indian data (N = 224). This provides a good comparison with our sample in Study 1.

4 Though this dataset does not indicate participant’s religious identity, previous work has shown that the Indian population on AMT is predominately (~70%) Hindu (Reference Groth, Dozier, Demment, Li, Fernandez, Chang and DyeGroth, et al., 2016).

5 Both studies primarily investigated cooperative behavior, and did not contain any mention of religious belief except for the question about belief in God that is our dependent variable (and was embedded in the demographics portion of the surveys). Further, the experimental conditions used within these cooperation studies: (i) were not intended to have an effect on either CRT scores nor belief scores, (ii) in fact did not have a significant effect on belief (p = .458) nor CRT (p = .090), and (iii) do not produce a significant affect when interacted with CRT when predicting belief (interactions term: p = .142).

6 This effect maintains when using an ordinal logistic model (treating belief as an ordinal variable, opposed to continuous), b = −.34, p < .001.

7 The upstream and downstream models referenced here include study controls; however, results are qualitatively identical when removing study from these models.

8 3-item CRT: Cronbach’s α = .690; 4-item CRT: Cronbach’s α = .647

9 The single item was taken from Reference Gervais, van Elk, Xygalatas, McKay, Aveyard, Buchtel and RiekkiGervais et al. (2018): “How strongly do you believe in God or gods (from 0–100)? To clarify, if you are certain that God (or gods) does not exist, please put ‘0’ and if you are certain that God (or gods) does exist, then put ‘100’.”

10 Items include: There is some kind of life after death. There is a spiritual realm besides the physical one. There exists an all-powerful and all-knowing spiritual being, whom we might call God.

11 This effect remins when using an ordinal logistic model (treating religious belief scores as an ordinal variable; b = −.246, p < .001).

12 As above, this effect remains when using an ordinal logistic model and belief in God as the dependent variable (b = −.278, p < .001).

13 Though Islam is the second largest religious tradition in India, the population is still overwhelmingly (80.5%) Hindu polytheistic (Indian Census Commissioner, 2001).

References

Bahçekapili, H. G., & Yilmaz, O. (2017). The relation between different types of religiosity and analytic cognitive style. Personality and Individual Differences, 117, 267272. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.06.013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, J. L. (2000). Exploring the natural foundations of religion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(1), 2934. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01419-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burkimsher, M. (2014). Is religious attendance bottoming out? An examination of current trends across Europe. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 53(2), 432445. https://doi.org/10.1111/jssr.12111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camerer, C. F., Dreber, A., Holzmeister, F., Ho, T.-H., Huber, J., Johannesson, M., … Pfeiffer, T. (2018). Evaluating the replicability of social science experiments in Nature and Science between 2010 and 2015. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(9), 637644. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0399-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carrico, A. W., Ironson, G., Antoni, M. H., Lechner, S. C., Durán, R. E., Kumar, M., & Schneiderman, N. (2006). A path model of the effects of spirituality on depressive symptoms and 24-hurinary-free cortisol in HIV-positive persons. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 61(1), 51-58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2006.04.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Neys, W. (2012). Bias and conflict: A case for logical intuitions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(1), 2838. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691611429354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Draper, J. W. (1875). History of the Conflict between Religion and Science (Vol. 13). New York, NY: D. Appleton & Company.Google Scholar
Evans, J. & Baronavski, C. (2018). How do European countries differ in religious commitment? Use our interactive map to find out. Retrieved from http://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2018/12/05/how-do-european-countries-differ-in-religious-commitment/.Google Scholar
Evans, J. S. B., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 223241. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612460685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferriss, A. L. (2002). Religion and the quality of life. Journal of Happiness Studies, 3(3), 199215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19(4), 2542. https://doi.org/10.1257/089533005775196732.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gervais, W. M., & Norenzayan, A. (2012). Analytic thinking promotes religious disbelief. Science, 336(6080), 493496. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1215647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gervais, W. M., van Elk, M., Xygalatas, D., McKay, R. T., Aveyard, M., Buchtel, E. E., … Riekki, T. (2018). Analytic atheism: A cross-culturally weak and fickle phenomenon? Judgment and Decision Making, 13(3), 268274. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/92r8x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., Tafarodi, R. W., & Malone, P. S. (1993). You can’t not believe everything you read. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(2), 221233. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.2.221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gignac, G. E., & Szodorai, E. T. (2016). Effect size guidelines for individual differences researchers. Personality and Individual Differences, 102, 7478. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.06.069.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Groth, S. W., Dozier, A., Demment, M., Li, D., Fernandez, I. D., Chang, J., & Dye, T. (2016). Participation in genetic research: Amazon’s Mechanical Turk workforce in the United States and India. Public Health Genomics, 19(6), 325335. https://doi.org/10.1159/000452094.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22(3), 165196. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1090-5138(00)00071-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2-3), 6183. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.1601785.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Indian Census Commissioner. (2001). Religion. Retrieved from http://censusindia.gov.in/Census\_And\_You/religion.aspx.Google Scholar
Ironson, G., Stuetzle, R., & Fletcher, M. A. (2006). An increase in religiousness/spirituality occurs after HIV diagnosis and predicts slower disease progression over 4 years in people with HIV. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 21(5), S62-S68. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1525-1497.2006.00648.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irvine, K., Hoffman, D. A., & Wilkinson-Ryan, T. (2018). Law and psychology grows up, goes online, and replicates. Journal of Empirical Legal Studies, 15(2), 320355. https://doi.org/10.1111/jels.12180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jong, J., Bluemke, M., & Halberstadt, J. (2013). Fear of death and supernatural beliefs: Developing a new supernatural belief scale to test the relationship. European Journal of Personality, 27, 495506. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jong, J., & Halberstadt, J. (2016). Death anxiety and religious belief: An existential psychology of religion. London, UK: Bloomsbury Publishing.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York, NY: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Kelemen, D. (2004). Are children “intuitive theists”? Reasoning about purpose and design in nature. Psychological Science, 15(5), 295-301. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00672.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCullough, M. E., Hoyt, W. T., Larson, D. B., Koenig, H. G., & Thoresen, C. (2000). Religious involvement and mortality: A meta-analytic review. Health Psychology, 19(3), 211222. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.19.3.211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mercier, B., Kramer, S. R., & Shariff, A. F. (2018). Belief in god: Why people believe, and why they don’t. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(4), 263268. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721418754491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norenzayan, A., & Gervais, W. M. (2013). The origins of religious disbelief. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(1), 2025. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.11.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oppenheimer, D. M., Meyvis, T. & Davidenko, N. (2009). Instructional manipulation checks: Detecting satisficing to increase statistical power. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(4), 867872. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.03.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Koehler, D. J., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2016). Is the cognitive reflection test a measure of both reflection and intuition? Behavior Research Methods, 48(1), 341348. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0576-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Seli, P., Koehler, D. J., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2012). Analytic cognitive style predicts religious and paranormal belief. Cognition, 123(3), 335346. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.03.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pennycook, G., Fugelsang, J. A., & Koehler, D. J. (2015). Everyday consequences of analytic thinking. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(6), 425432. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2644392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pennycook, G., & Ross, R. M. (2016). Commentary: Cognitive reflection vs. calculation in decision making. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pennycook, G., Ross, R. M., Koehler, D. J., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2016). Atheists and agnostics are more reflective than religious believers: Four empirical studies and a meta-analysis. PLoS ONE, 11(4), e0153039. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0153039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanchez, C., Sundermeier, B., Gray, K., & Calin-Jageman, R. J. (2017). Direct replication of Gervais & Norenzayan (2012): No evidence that analytic thinking decreases religious belief. PLoS ONE, 12(2), e0172636. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saribay, S. A., & Yilmaz, O. (2017). Analytic cognitive style and cognitive ability differentially predict religiosity and social conservatism. Personality and Individual Differences, 114, 2429. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.03.056.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shenhav, A., Rand, D. G., & Greene, J. D. (2012). Divine intuition: Cognitive style influences belief in God. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(3), 423428. https://doi.org/10.1037/e519702015-023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sloan, R. P. (2006). Blind faith: The unholy alliance of religion and medicine. New York, NY: St. Martin’s Press.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate?. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(5), 645665. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00003435.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanovich, K. E. (2009). Distinguishing the reflective, algorithmic, and autonomous minds: Is it time for a tri-process theory? In Evans, E. & Frankish, K. (Eds.), In two minds: Dual processes and beyond (pp. 55-88) Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199230167.003.0003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stagnaro, M., Arechar, A., & Rand, D. G. Are those who believe in God really more prosocial? (April 15 , 2018). Available at SSRN: https://ssrn.com/abstract=3160453 or http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3160453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomson, K. S., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2016). Investigating an alternative form of the Cognitive Reflection Test. Judgment and Decision Making, 11(1), 99113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yilmaz, O., Karadöller, D. Z., & Sofuoglu, G. (2016). Analytic thinking, religion, and prejudice: An experimental test of the dual-process model of mind. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 26(4), 360369. https://doi.org/10.1080/10508619.2016.1151117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1: Violin plots (with jittered individual data points) showing belief in God (represented by POMP scores) as a function of correct responses on the 3-item CRT. Width of each plot shows density. Horizontal lines show median (heavy line) and 25th and 75th percentile.

Figure 1

Table 1: Multiple regression analyses (standardized coefficients) predicting belief in God using the 3-item CRT and controlling for study, age, gender (Male = 1, Female = 2), income, trust, and education (dummy code). Standard errors are in parentheses

Figure 2

Figure 2: Violin plots (with jittered individual data points) showing supernatural belief (represented by POMP scores) as a function of correct responses on the 7-item CRT.

Figure 3

Figure 3: Violin plots (with jittered individual data points) showing belief in God (represented by POMP scores) as a function of number of correct responses on the 3-item CRT.

Figure 4

Table 2: Multiple regression analyses (standardized) predicting a) belief in God using the 3-item CRT and b) supernatural belief using the 7-item CRT while controlling for age, gender (Male = 1, Female = 2), income, social conservatism, fiscal conservatism, performance in an attention check, ethnicity (dummy coded), and education (dummy coded). Standard errors are in parentheses

Supplementary material: File

Stagnaro et al. supplementary material

Stagnaro et al. supplementary material 1
Download Stagnaro et al. supplementary material(File)
File 14.5 KB
Supplementary material: File

Stagnaro et al. supplementary material

Stagnaro et al. supplementary material 2
Download Stagnaro et al. supplementary material(File)
File 28.7 KB
Supplementary material: File

Stagnaro et al. supplementary material

Stagnaro et al. supplementary material 3
Download Stagnaro et al. supplementary material(File)
File 4.5 KB
Supplementary material: File

Stagnaro et al. supplementary material

Supplemental information
Download Stagnaro et al. supplementary material(File)
File 518.8 KB